Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 50
Filtrar
Mais filtros

Base de dados
Tipo de documento
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Psychol Med ; : 1-7, 2024 Feb 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38314511

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: With efforts increasing worldwide to understand and treat paranoia, there is a pressing need for cross-culturally valid assessments of paranoid beliefs. The recently developed Revised Green et al., Paranoid Thoughts Scale (R-GPTS) constitutes an easy to administer self-report assessment of mild ideas of reference and more severe persecutory thoughts. Moreover, it comes with clinical cut-offs for increased usability in research and clinical practice. With multiple translations of the R-GPTS already available and in use, a formal test of its measurement invariance is now needed. METHODS: Using data from a multinational cross-sectional online survey in the UK, USA, Australia, Germany, and Hong Kong (N = 2510), we performed confirmatory factory analyses on the R-GPTS and tested for measurement invariance across sites. RESULTS: We found sufficient fit for the two-factor structure (ideas of reference, persecutory thoughts) of the R-GPTS across cultures. Measurement invariance was found for the persecutory thoughts subscale, indicating that it does measure the same construct across the tested samples in the same way. For ideas of reference, we found no scalar invariance, which was traced back to (mostly higher) item intercepts in the Hong Kong sample. CONCLUSION: We found sufficient invariance for the persecutory thoughts scale, which is of substantial practical importance, as it is used for the screening of clinical paranoia. A direct comparison of the ideas of reference sum-scores between cultures, however, may lead to an over-estimation of these milder forms of paranoia in some (non-western) cultures.

2.
Psychol Med ; 53(13): 5933-5944, 2023 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37427557

RESUMO

Paranoia is common in clinical and nonclinical populations, consistent with continuum models of psychosis. A number of experimental studies have been conducted that attempt to induce, manipulate or measure paranoid thinking in both clinical and nonclinical populations, which is important to understand causal mechanisms and advance psychological interventions. Our aim was to conduct a systematic review and meta-analysis of experimental studies (non-sleep, non-drug paradigms) on psychometrically assessed paranoia in clinical and nonclinical populations. The review was conducted using PRISMA guidelines. Six databases (PsycINFO, PubMed, EMBASE, Web of Science, Medline and AMED) were searched for peer-reviewed experimental studies using within and between-subject designs to investigate paranoia in clinical and nonclinical populations. Effect sizes for each study were calculated using Hedge's g and were integrated using a random effect meta-analysis model. Thirty studies were included in the review (total n = 3898), which used 13 experimental paradigms to induce paranoia; 10 studies set out to explicitly induce paranoia, and 20 studies induced a range of other states. Effect sizes for individual studies ranged from 0.03 to 1.55. Meta-analysis found a significant summary effect of 0.51 [95% confidence interval 0.37-0.66, p < 0.001], indicating a medium effect of experimental paradigms on paranoia. Paranoia can be induced and investigated using a wide range of experimental paradigms, which can inform decision-making about which paradigms to use in future studies, and is consistent with cognitive, continuum and evolutionary models of paranoia.


Assuntos
Transtornos Psicóticos , Humanos , Transtornos Psicóticos/psicologia , Transtornos Paranoides/psicologia , Sono
3.
Psychol Med ; 53(12): 5748-5755, 2023 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36065655

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: The term 'pandemic paranoia' has been coined to refer to heightened levels of mistrust and suspicion towards other people specifically due to the COVID-19 pandemic. In this study, we examine the international prevalence of pandemic paranoia in the general population and its associated sociodemographic profile. METHODS: A representative international sample of general population adults (N = 2510) from five sites (USA N = 535, Germany N = 516, UK N = 512, Australia N = 502 and Hong Kong N = 445) were recruited using stratified quota sampling (for age, sex, educational attainment) and completed the Pandemic Paranoia Scale (PPS). RESULTS: The overall prevalence rate of pandemic paranoia was 19%, and was highest in Australia and lowest in Germany. On the subscales of the PPS, prevalence was 11% for persecutory threat, 29% for paranoid conspiracy and 37% for interpersonal mistrust. Site and general paranoia significantly predicted pandemic paranoia. Sociodemographic variables (lower age, higher population size and income, being male, employed and no migrant status) explained additional variance and significantly improved prediction of pandemic paranoia. CONCLUSIONS: Pandemic paranoia was relatively common in a representative sample of the general population across five international sites. Sociodemographic variables explained a small but significant amount of the variance in pandemic paranoia.


Assuntos
COVID-19 , Transtornos Paranoides , Adulto , Humanos , Masculino , Feminino , Transtornos Paranoides/epidemiologia , Pandemias , Prevalência , COVID-19/epidemiologia , Relações Interpessoais
4.
J Pers ; 91(3): 556-565, 2023 06.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35837856

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: To test a novel proposition that dispositional forgiveness has the unrecognized benefit of buffering feelings of paranoia following negative interpersonal experiences and interpersonal transgressions. METHODS: In Study 1 (N = 128), we used an experimental paradigm, the Prisoner's Dilemma Game (PDG), to test the premise that an interpersonal transgression increases state paranoia. Study 2 (N = 180) used a longitudinal design to test the central proposition that dispositional forgiveness buffers state paranoia following naturally occurring difficult (vs pleasant) interpersonal events. Study 3 (N = 102) used a novel experimental paradigm to determine the causal effect of manipulating forgiveness on paranoia. RESULTS: In Study 1, interpersonal transgressions in the PDG increased paranoia. In Study 2, paranoia was higher following difficult (rather than pleasant) events, and higher levels of dispositional forgiveness moderated the negative effect of difficult events on paranoia. In Study 3, there was a causal effect of forgiveness on (reduced) paranoia. CONCLUSIONS: This is the first evidence that (1) interpersonal transgressions increase paranoia, (2) high dispositional forgiveness moderates the deleterious effect of interpersonal transgression on paranoia, and (3) dispositional forgiveness is causally related to less paranoia.


Assuntos
Perdão , Humanos , Relações Interpessoais , Transtornos Paranoides , Emoções , Personalidade
5.
J Pers ; 90(5): 727-747, 2022 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34888884

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: Three studies tested a novel model of the narcissism-paranoia link, whereby narcissism (primarily its socially maladaptive facets) is associated with paranoia via over-use of defensive self-protection and/or under-use of self-affirmation. METHODS: In Study 1, 245 online volunteers (87% female; MAGE  = 20.92; 44% White-British) completed trait measures of narcissism, self-enhancement/protection strategies and paranoia. In Study 2, 116 students (82% female; MAGE  = 20.23; 70% White-British) completed baseline measures, then reported state reactions and paranoia following two difficult and two pleasant interpersonal events after 3-10 days. In Study 3, 517 online volunteers (64% female; MAGE  = 22.76; 77% White/Caucasian) completed baseline measures, experienced a standardized social exclusion (vs. neutral) manipulation (Cyberball), then reported state reactions and paranoia. RESULTS: In Study 1, narcissism was associated with higher paranoia via defensiveness. In Study 2, this was replicated in difficult but not pleasant events, and was driven by the Entitlement/Exploitativeness facet of narcissism. In Study 3, narcissistic rivalry and vulnerable narcissism, but not admiration, were associated with Cyberball-related paranoia via general defensiveness and denigration of others. CONCLUSIONS: Individuals high in narcissism-especially its socially maladaptive facets-who over-rely on defensive self-protection strategies in response to threat, are particularly vulnerable to paranoia. Findings help to understand individual differences in paranoia.


Assuntos
Narcisismo , Transtornos Paranoides , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Individualidade , Masculino , Autoimagem , Estudantes , Adulto Jovem
6.
Br J Psychiatry ; 219(6): 629-631, 2021 12.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35048869

RESUMO

There is increasing interest in potential harmful effects of mindfulness-based interventions. In relation to psychosis, inconsistency and shortcomings in how harm is monitored and reported are holding back our understanding. We offer eight recommendations to help build a firmer evidence base on potential harm in mindfulness for psychosis.


Assuntos
Atenção Plena , Transtornos Psicóticos , Humanos , Transtornos Psicóticos/terapia
7.
Learn Behav ; 48(1): 66-83, 2020 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32170595

RESUMO

Polymorphous concepts are hard to learn, and this is perhaps surprising because they, like many natural concepts, have an overall similarity structure. However, the dimensional summation hypothesis (Milton and Wills Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 30, 407-415 2004) predicts this difficulty. It also makes a number of other predictions about polymorphous concept formation, which are tested here. In Experiment 4, we confirm the theory's prediction that polymorphous concept formation should be facilitated by deterministic pretraining on the constituent features of the stimulus. This facilitation is relative to an equivalent amount of training on the polymorphous concept itself. In further experiments, we compare the predictions of the dimensional summation hypothesis with a more general strategic account (Experiment 2), a seriality of training account (Experiment 3), a stimulus decomposition account (also Experiment 3), and an error-based account (Experiment 4). The dimensional summation hypothesis provides the best account of these data. In Experiment 5, a further prediction is confirmed-the single feature pretraining effect is eliminated by a concurrent counting task. The current experiments suggest the hypothesis that natural concepts might be acquired by the deliberate serial summation of evidence. This idea has testable implications for classroom learning.


Assuntos
Formação de Conceito , Aprendizagem , Animais , Cognição , Memória , Tempo de Reação
8.
BMC Psychiatry ; 17(1): 54, 2017 02 06.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28166760

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: The outcome of first episode psychosis (FEP) is highly variable and difficult to predict. Cognitive insight measured at illness onset has previously been found to predict psychopathology 12-months later. The aims of this study were to examine whether the prospective relationship between cognitive insight and symptom severity is evident at four-years following FEP and to examine some psychological correlates of cognitive insight. METHODS: FEP participants (n = 90) completed the Beck Cognitive Insight Scale (BCIS) at illness onset, and associations between BCIS scores with symptom severity outcomes (4-years after FEP) were assessed. The BCIS scales (self-reflectiveness and self-certainty) were examined as a composite score, and individually compared to other cognitive measures (IQ and jumping to conclusions (JTC) bias). RESULTS: Regression analyses revealed that the cognitive insight composite did not predict 4-year symptom remission in this study while the self-reflection subscale of the BCIS predicted severity of symptoms at 4-years. Self-certainty items of the BCIS were not associated with symptom severity. Significant correlations between the JTC bias, self-certainty and IQ were found, but self-reflection did not correlate with these other cognitive measures. CONCLUSIONS: Self-reflective capacity is a more relevant and independent cognitive construct than self-certainty for predicting prospective symptom severity in psychosis. Improving self-reflection may be a useful target for early intervention research.


Assuntos
Conscientização , Cognição , Transtornos Psicóticos/psicologia , Autoimagem , Adulto , Estudos de Coortes , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Valor Preditivo dos Testes , Escalas de Graduação Psiquiátrica , Psicopatologia , Análise de Regressão
9.
Br J Clin Psychol ; 56(3): 303-309, 2017 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28493561

RESUMO

OBJECTIVES: Command hallucinations are considered to be one of the most distressing and disturbing symptoms of schizophrenia. Building on earlier studies, we compare key attributes in the symptomatic, affective, and cognitive profiles of people diagnosed with schizophrenia and hearing voices that do (n = 77) or do not (n = 74) give commands. METHODS: The study employed a cross-sectional design, in which we assessed voice severity, distress and control (PSYRATs), anxiety and depression (HADS), beliefs about voices (BAVQ-R), and responsibility beliefs (RIQ). Clinical and demographic variables were also collected. RESULTS: Command hallucinations were found to be more distressing and controlling, perceived as more omnipotent and malevolent, linked to higher anxiety and depression, and resisted more than hallucinations without commands. Commanding voices were also associated with higher conviction ratings for being personally responsible for preventing harm. CONCLUSIONS: The findings suggest key differences in the affective and cognitive profiles of people who hear commanding voices, which have important implications for theory and psychological interventions. PRACTITIONER POINTS: Command hallucinations are associated with higher distress, malevolence, and omnipotence. Command hallucinations are associated with higher responsibility beliefs for preventing harm. Responsibility beliefs are associated with voice-related distress. Future psychological interventions for command hallucinations might benefit from focussing not only on omnipotence, but also on responsibility beliefs, as is done in psychological therapies for obsessive compulsive disorder. Limitations The cross-sectional design does not assess issues of causality. We did not measure the presence or severity of delusions.


Assuntos
Alucinações/psicologia , Esquizofrenia/diagnóstico , Adolescente , Adulto , Estudos Transversais , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Inquéritos e Questionários , Adulto Jovem
10.
J Nerv Ment Dis ; 203(11): 878-82, 2015 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26513511

RESUMO

Previous studies have identified a positive relationship between aggression and paranoia, yet the relationship between the emotion of anger and paranoia in forensic populations has not been examined. Possible confounding variables, such as social desirability and mood, should also be considered. Sixty-six participants who had a violent conviction and mental disorder completed self-report questionnaires that measured anger, paranoid ideation, socially desirable responding, anxiety, and depression. The findings indicated that increased anger was associated with increased paranoia. Partial correlations showed that anger remained significantly associated with paranoia after socially desirable responding, anxiety, depression, gender, and violence history were controlled, suggesting anger and paranoia were not associated due to indirect relationships with these constructs. This could suggest that integrative psychological interventions that consider experiences of both anger and paranoia may be beneficial with forensic populations.


Assuntos
Ira , Criminosos/psicologia , Transtornos Paranoides/diagnóstico , Transtornos Paranoides/psicologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Autorrelato , Adulto Jovem
11.
Behav Cogn Psychother ; 43(4): 490-501, 2015 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24618456

RESUMO

BACKGROUND AND AIMS: This study explored therapists' and clients' experiences of paranoia about the therapist in cognitive behaviour therapy. METHOD: Ten therapists and eight clients engaged in cognitive behaviour therapy for psychosis were interviewed using a semi-structured interview. Data were analyzed using thematic analysis. RESULTS: Clients reported experiencing paranoia about their therapist, both within and between therapy sessions. Therapists' accounts highlighted a number of dilemmas that can arise in responding to clients' paranoia about them. CONCLUSIONS: The findings highlight helpful ways of working with clients when they become paranoid about their therapist, and emphasize the importance of developing a therapeutic relationship that is radically collaborative, supporting a person-based approach to distressing psychotic experience.


Assuntos
Terapia Cognitivo-Comportamental/métodos , Transtornos Paranoides/etiologia , Relações Profissional-Paciente , Transtornos Psicóticos/psicologia , Transtornos Psicóticos/terapia , Adulto , Atitude do Pessoal de Saúde , Feminino , Humanos , Entrevista Psicológica , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Transtornos Paranoides/psicologia , Transtornos Paranoides/terapia
12.
Behav Cogn Psychother ; 42(5): 634-9, 2014 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24460954

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Numerous studies have shown that paranoia is common in the nonclinical population; however, little research has examined whether nonclinical paranoid beliefs change over time, or considered potential reasons for change. AIMS: The aim of the present study was therefore to examine naturalistic change in nonclinical paranoid experiences. METHOD: 60 participants described an idiosyncratic experience of paranoia, including when it occurred, and rated their experience along four key belief dimensions: preoccupation, impact, distress and conviction. Participants provided two ratings for each dimension, retrospective recall at the time of the occurrence of the paranoid event, and again at the time of the interview. Participants were also asked to provide qualitative descriptions of reasons for change in belief dimensions. RESULTS: Participants described paranoid experiences that had occurred over a large timeframe (1 day-25 years). Reductions across all four belief dimensions were found, and seven key themes emerged following qualitative analysis of the participants' reason for change in response to the paranoid event. CONCLUSIONS: The findings highlight a number of factors associated with reported naturalistic changes in belief dimensions of conviction, distress, preoccupation and impact, which might be useful in enhancing interventions for clinical and nonclinical paranoia, and in helping to build models to account for why people showing clear paranoid ideation do, or do not, go on to develop clinical paranoia.


Assuntos
Adaptação Psicológica , Cultura , Transtornos Paranoides/psicologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Seguimentos , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Transtornos Paranoides/diagnóstico , Estudantes/psicologia , Adulto Jovem
13.
Psychol Psychother ; 97(1): 34-40, 2024 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37387330

RESUMO

PURPOSE & METHOD: Mindfulness for psychosis research has grown exponentially over the last 15 years. In this paper, a brief overview of mindfulness for psychosis is provided followed by a summary of the findings from a systematic search of meta-analyses dated up to February 2023. Current issues in the field are discussed and a future research agenda is presented. RESULTS: Ten meta-analyses published between 2013 and 2023 were identified. Reported effect sizes on reductions in psychotic symptoms ranged from small-large across reviews. Four key issues in the field are identified and discussed - (1) is mindfulness for psychosis safe? (2) is home practice essential and related to clinical outcomes? (3) what is the impact of mindfulness practice versus metacognitive insights derived from practice, on clinical outcomes? (4) do the benefits translate into routine clinical practice? CONCLUSIONS: Mindfulness is a promising intervention that is emerging as being both safe and effective for people with psychosis. Future research focused on evaluating mechanisms of change and implementation in routine clinical practice should be prioritised.


Assuntos
Metacognição , Atenção Plena , Transtornos Psicóticos , Humanos , Transtornos Psicóticos/terapia , Transtornos Psicóticos/psicologia
14.
Clin Psychol Rev ; 112: 102445, 2024 May 18.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38851179

RESUMO

Harmful outcomes of psychological interventions are under-researched, including in mindfulness-based interventions (MBI) for psychosis. This systematic review summarizes reporting and prevalence of 8 harm indices (death, adverse events, hospitalisation, study drop out, noncompletion of therapy, side effects of therapy, symptom deterioration and crisis service use) in Randomised Controlled Trials (RCTs) of MBIs for psychosis. Meta-analyses of risk differences were also calculated for each harm index. The review included 39 studies, with a total n of 2684 participants across studies. The percentage of studies reporting on each index of harm, and the prevalence of harm, varied greatly across each index. 0% of studies reported on side effects of interventions compared to 92% of studies reporting on study dropout. Meta-analyses of risk differences (RD) found a higher risk of hospitalisation (RD (95% CI) = -0.136 (-0.23 to -0.05), p = 0.003) and crisis service use (RD (95% CI) = -0.160 (-0.299, -0.024), p = 0.02) in control arms compared to intervention arms, and no significant difference in adverse events, death, symptom deterioration, noncompletion of therapy, drop out and side effects of therapy. Overall, reporting of harm was inconsistent across studies and the quality of data collection and reporting varied. MBIs for psychosis appear to be safe and may reduce the risk of hospitalisation and use of crisis services. However, the absence of thorough reporting on harm precludes a balanced analysis of benefits versus harms. Future research into the effectiveness of MBIs should consistently operationalise, monitor and report data on harm.

15.
Res Child Adolesc Psychopathol ; 52(2): 267-275, 2024 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37740777

RESUMO

Paranoia is a common experience in adolescence that may entail the use of safety behaviours (e.g. avoidance), which are assumed to maintain paranoia in the long run. As the development of paranoia and related safety behaviours in youth may be influenced by their caregivers, we aimed to investigate the associations of paranoia and safety behaviours in adolescents and their parents. Adolescents from the general population aged 14-17 and one of their parents (N = 142 dyads) were recruited via Qualtrics to complete online surveys including measures of paranoia, safety behaviour use, anxiety, and demographics. We fitted an Actor-Partner-Interdependence Model (APIM) for testing dyadic parent-child interaction by using structural equation modelling and controlled for adolescents' and parents' anxiety. Results indicated that paranoia positively predicted safety behaviour use in adolescents and in parents. There were significant positive intra-dyad (i.e. parent-adolescent) correlations for both paranoia and safety behaviour use. One partner effect was significant: parental paranoia positively predicted the safety behaviour use of their adolescent child. Conversely, adolescents' paranoia did not predict their parents' safety behaviour use. Our findings corroborate prior research demonstrating an association between paranoia and safety behaviours among adults, and extend this association to adolescents. Children of parents experiencing paranoia are at increased risk of developing paranoia and safety behaviours, which indicates the need for interventions that target paranoia and safety behaviours in family systems.


Assuntos
Comportamento do Adolescente , Transtornos Paranoides , Adulto , Humanos , Adolescente , Transtornos Paranoides/epidemiologia , Pais , Comportamentos Relacionados com a Saúde , Inquéritos e Questionários
16.
Br J Clin Psychol ; 52(1): 1-11, 2013 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23398108

RESUMO

OBJECTIVES: Social rank theory has been applied to psychosis, in particular the relationship between an individual and their voices. However, perceived peer group rank has not been empirically tested in an early psychosis group. The purpose of the study was to test the prediction that individuals with early psychosis will have lower perceived social status, engage in submissive behaviours more frequently, and will feel more entrapped by external events compared to a healthy control group. DESIGN: The study employed a cross-sectional design, comparing individuals with early psychosis and healthy controls. METHODS: A total of 24 participants with early psychosis and 24 matched controls completed self-report measures of social rank, including social comparison, submissive behaviours and entrapment, measures of depression, anxiety and psychotic symptoms, and measures of peer network size and peer relationship quality. RESULTS: Individuals with early psychosis viewed themselves as being of lower social rank and inferior in relation to matched controls, and also reported engaging in submissive behaviours more frequently and felt more entrapped by external events. CONCLUSIONS: Perception of lower social rank and inferiority amongst individuals with early psychosis may impact on engagement in peer relationships and impact on the social decline in early psychosis, which could have significant implications for interventions and recovery.


Assuntos
Grupo Associado , Transtornos Psicóticos/psicologia , Autoimagem , Percepção Social , Adulto , Estudos Transversais , Feminino , Amigos/psicologia , Humanos , Relações Interpessoais , Londres , Masculino , Satisfação Pessoal , Escalas de Graduação Psiquiátrica , Psicologia do Esquizofrênico , Predomínio Social , Isolamento Social/psicologia , Inquéritos e Questionários , Adulto Jovem
17.
Cogn Emot ; 27(1): 53-62, 2013.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22731988

RESUMO

A growing body of research shows that paranoia is common in the general population. We report three studies that examined the Prisoner's Dilemma Game (PDG) as a paradigm for evaluation of non-clinical paranoia. The PDG captures three key qualities that are at the heart of paranoia--it is interpersonal, it concerns threat, and it concerns the perception of others' intentions towards the self. Study 1 (n=175) found that state paranoia was positively associated with selection of the competitive PDG choice. Study 2 (n=111) found that this association was significant only when participants believed they were playing the PDG against another person, and not when playing against a computer. This finding underscores the interpersonal nature of paranoia and the concomitant necessity of studying paranoia in interpersonal context. In Study 3 (n=152), we assessed both trait and state paranoia, and differentiated between distrust- and greed-based competition. Both trait and state paranoia were positively associated with distrust-based competition (but not with greed-based competition). Crucially, we found that the association between trait paranoia and distrust-based competition was fully mediated by state paranoia. The PDG is a promising paradigm for the study of non-clinical paranoia.


Assuntos
Comportamento Competitivo , Teoria dos Jogos , Relações Interpessoais , Transtornos Paranoides/psicologia , Confiança , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Análise de Regressão , Reino Unido
18.
Behav Cogn Psychother ; 41(2): 238-42, 2013 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22974494

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Emerging evidence suggests that mindfulness can be beneficial for people with distressing psychosis. This study examined the hypothesis that for people with persecutory delusions in the absence of voices, mindfulness training would lead to reductions in conviction, distress, preoccupation and impact of paranoid beliefs, as well as anxiety and depression. METHOD: Two case studies are presented. Participants completed measures of mindfulness, anxiety and depression at baseline, end of therapy and 1 month follow-up, and bi-weekly ratings of their paranoid belief on the dimensions of conviction, preoccupation, distress and impact. RESULTS: Ratings of conviction, distress, impact and preoccupation, and measures of anxiety and depression, reduced for both participants from baseline to end of intervention. Improvements in mindfulness of distressing thoughts and images occurred for both participants. These gains were maintained at 1 month follow-up. CONCLUSIONS: Findings suggest that mindfulness training can impact on cognition and affect specifically associated with paranoid beliefs, and is potentially relevant to both Poor Me and Bad Me paranoia.


Assuntos
Conscientização , Terapia Cognitivo-Comportamental/métodos , Cultura , Meditação/psicologia , Transtornos Paranoides/terapia , Adulto , Transtornos de Ansiedade/diagnóstico , Transtornos de Ansiedade/psicologia , Transtornos de Ansiedade/terapia , Delusões/diagnóstico , Delusões/psicologia , Delusões/terapia , Transtorno Depressivo/diagnóstico , Transtorno Depressivo/psicologia , Transtorno Depressivo/terapia , Seguimentos , Humanos , Masculino , Meditação/métodos , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Transtornos Paranoides/diagnóstico , Transtornos Paranoides/psicologia , Inquéritos e Questionários
19.
Schizophr Res ; 260: 99-112, 2023 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37634388

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: As we face the largest refugee crisis since World War Two, research is increasingly examining the impact of forced displacement. The risk of non-affective psychosis in refugees is evidenced to be significantly greater than non-refugees, and the role of pre-, peri- and post-migratory trauma and dissociation is increasingly implicated. AIMS: To determine the prevalence of non-affective psychosis in refugee populations. METHOD: PRISMA guidelines were followed. Three key databases (PubMed, PsychINFO and Web of Science), Google scholar and study references were searched. The full-text of 62 studies were screened and 23 studies were eligible for inclusion. A narrative synthesis was undertaken and the Quality Assessment Tool for Quantitative Studies was used to assess methodological quality. (PROSPERO registration CRD42019152170). RESULTS: The results were widely heterogeneous. The combined weighted average prevalence of non-affective psychosis in refugee populations was 0.9 %. Psychosis prevalence for individual psychotic symptoms was 28.4 %; 0.5 % for schizophrenia; 1.0 % for psychosis; 0.6 % for mixed psychotic disorders and 2.9 % for psychotic episodes. CONCLUSIONS: Variations in examined populations, diagnostic and prevalence classifications, and study designs and methodologies likely contributed to heterogeneity across the data. The findings highlight a greater need to provide more specialist mental health services and trauma-focused interventions, as well as transculturally sensitive assessment and treatment to address refugee vulnerability to psychosis. Future research should examine psychosis prevalence longitudinally and in refugees-only, address methodological bias and further examine the role of trauma and dissociation in refugee psychosis prevalence.


Assuntos
Transtornos Psicóticos , Refugiados , Esquizofrenia , Humanos , Refugiados/psicologia , Prevalência , Transtornos Psicóticos/epidemiologia
20.
Schizophr Bull ; 49(4): 1088-1094, 2023 07 04.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36912015

RESUMO

BACKGROUND AND HYPOTHESIS: Game theory paradigms, such as the Prisoner's Dilemma Game (PDG), have been used to study nonclinical paranoia, though research using clinical populations has been scarce. We test our novel theoretical model that schizophrenia leads to competitiveness in interpersonal interactions, and that this link is serially mediated by trait paranoia, state paranoia, and distrust. STUDY DESIGN: In this quasi-experimental study, individuals with schizophrenia spectrum diagnoses with current persecutory delusions (n = 46) and a nonclinical control group (n = 43) played the PDG, and completed measures of trait paranoia, state paranoia, and distrust. STUDY RESULTS: Individuals with schizophrenia competed more in the PDG than the control group. Supporting our theoretical model, all direct effects were significant: schizophrenia was associated with higher trait paranoia (H1); trait paranoia predicted state paranoia in the PDG (H2); state paranoia in the PDG predicted distrust of the opponent in the PDG (H3); and distrust predicted competition in the PDG (H4). The hypothesized indirect effect of schizophrenia on competition in the PDG via trait paranoia, state paranoia, and distrust was supported in a serial mediation model (H5). CONCLUSIONS: The findings make clear theoretical and methodological contributions. We provide the first evidence for a theoretical process model by which schizophrenia leads to competitiveness in interpersonal interactions via trait paranoia, state paranoia, and distrust. Game theory paradigms, and the PDG in particular, are important for advancing theory and research on paranoia as it occurs in both clinical and nonclinical populations.


Assuntos
Esquizofrenia , Humanos , Esquizofrenia/diagnóstico , Transtornos Paranoides/diagnóstico , Delusões/diagnóstico , Relações Interpessoais
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA