Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 86
Filtrar
Mais filtros

Base de dados
País/Região como assunto
Tipo de documento
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Science ; 185(4148): 374-6, 1974 Jul 26.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-4857788

RESUMO

Copulating male rats were permitted a maximum of seven intromissions in which to ejaculate. This experimental constraint of the male rat's sexual behavior produced an increase in the number of sessions in which the male ejaculated before the seventh intromrission. This species-specific behavior pattern is therefore susceptible to environmental conditioning.


Assuntos
Condicionamento Psicológico , Ratos/fisiologia , Esquema de Reforço , Comportamento Sexual Animal , Animais , Ejaculação , Meio Ambiente , Humanos , Masculino , Fatores Sexuais , Especificidade da Espécie , Comportamento Estereotipado
2.
Mech Ageing Dev ; 5(6): 437-42, 1976.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-994562

RESUMO

The concentration of hyaluronic acid which is the fixed loose connective tissue component determining water and other potentials in the interstitium remains constant with age. Since the collagen fiber network in the native tissue state is unstressed, changes in collagen fiber morphology and in collagen content with age have no influence.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento , Tecido Conjuntivo/análise , Animais , Bovinos , Sulfatos de Condroitina/análise , Colágeno/análise , Dermatan Sulfato/análise , Valvas Cardíacas/análise , Ácido Hialurônico/análise , Hialuronoglucosaminidase/análise , Tripsina/análise
3.
Ann N Y Acad Sci ; 275: 2-9, 1976.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1070271

RESUMO

It is shown that the presence of highly deformable surface layers of low elastic modulus produce effects in flow that tend to reduce throughout. This phenomenon seems to be due to the generation of surface waves and a corresponding fluid zone into which these waves are also partially propagated. Experiments are summarized that are consistent with this point of view. The implications for arterial and other blood flow are discussed.


Assuntos
Artérias/fisiopatologia , Arteriosclerose/fisiopatologia , Reologia , Fenômenos Biomecânicos , Substâncias Macromoleculares , Matemática , Modelos Teóricos
4.
J Appl Physiol (1985) ; 71(3): 1025-31, 1991 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1757297

RESUMO

Cardiac-related deflections in thoracic electrical impedance have been thought to correlate sufficiently well with cardiac stroke volume to be used as the basis for a noninvasive estimation of cardiac output. To determine more precisely the physiological origin of the impedance deflection (DZ), we regarded right ventricular stroke volume (SVa) as the sum of two components: 1) that part of SVa responsible for the transient increment in pulmonary blood volume within a cardiac cycle, SVa-v and 2) the remaining part of SVa, (SVa-SVa-v). SVa-v was measured in lambs by integration of the difference between pulmonary arterial and pulmonary venous flow. SVa and its components were varied experimentally by opening and closing an aorticocaval shunt or by inflating and deflating a cuff implanted around the pulmonary artery. DZ was measured using a tetrapolar disk electrode system. Multivariate linear regression analysis revealed that SVa-v had a significant positive effect on DZ, and, at the same time, (SVa-SVa-v) had a significant negative effect on DZ. In the pulmonary artery occluder model, the positive effect of SVa-v dominated the opposing negative effect of (SVa - SVa-v) so that the net effect of SVa on DZ was positive and significant. In the aorticocaval shunt model, these effects opposed each other to the extent that there was no significant correlation between SVa and DZ. These results shed new light on the physiological origin of DZ. They also demonstrate that use of DZ to measure acute changes in cardiac output may yield misleading results. Changes or the lack of changes in thoracic electrical impedance do not necessarily reflect cardiac output status.


Assuntos
Débito Cardíaco/fisiologia , Cardiografia de Impedância , Volume Sistólico/fisiologia , Animais , Volume Sanguíneo/fisiologia , Modelos Biológicos , Artéria Pulmonar/fisiologia , Circulação Pulmonar/fisiologia , Veias Pulmonares/fisiologia , Ovinos
5.
J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process ; 24(2): 215-28, 1998 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9556910

RESUMO

In 5 experiments, 4 monkeys and 1 ape chose between 2 food sources, each held in 1 of the experimenter's hands while he stood in front of a cage. When choosing between 2 sources of the same food that differed in amount, preference for the larger amount decreased as the size of each good proportionately increased. A second finding was that subjects were indifferent between a 2-food mixture and a single food that was part of the mixture if the single food was the preferred food of the mixture, a result suggesting the less preferred food had no value. Experiment 6 replicated these effects in 4 additional monkeys. These effects may be incompatible with previous theorizing about animal choice and may reflect a cognitive difference between nonhuman primates and humans.


Assuntos
Comportamento de Escolha , Condicionamento Operante , Preferências Alimentares/psicologia , Macaca fascicularis/psicologia , Macaca/psicologia , Motivação , Pan troglodytes/psicologia , Animais , Comportamento Apetitivo , Feminino , Masculino , Especificidade da Espécie
6.
Biophys Chem ; 41(1): 9-13, 1991 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1751773

RESUMO

It is recalled that the tension in a stretched polyelectrolyte chain mechanically compensates both the coulomb interaction and the hydrostatic pressure increase around the chain in a compromise which minimises the free energy and keeps water chemical potential constant throughout. Stretching strongly favors parallel cylinder nematic order in polyelectrolyte brushes on a surface or in the slit between two surfaces when the polyelectrolyte chains function as bridges. Strong, stiffly stretched chains result when the molarity of the fixed charge distribution is larger than the molarity of the neutral salt solution with which the brushes are in equilibrium. The relevance of these two systems to the endothelial cells which cover the walls of blood vessels is discussed.


Assuntos
Eletrólitos/metabolismo , Endotélio Vascular/fisiologia , Membrana Celular/metabolismo , Membrana Celular/fisiologia , Endotélio Vascular/metabolismo , Endotélio Vascular/ultraestrutura , Glicoproteínas de Membrana/metabolismo , Glicoproteínas de Membrana/fisiologia
7.
Pediatr Pulmonol ; 12(3): 146-52, 1992 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1641270

RESUMO

We examined the effects of within-breath changes in compliance (C) upon the accuracy of measurements of compliance and resistance (R) by linear regression analysis and by Mead and Wittenberger's method. These effects were illustrated by a computer model and by lung models with linear and nonlinear pressure/volume relationships, and were also studied in 14 normal spontaneously breathing premature infants (mean +/- SD, BW 1,290 +/- 200 g, GA 29.9 +/- 2.7 weeks, age 7.4 +/- 2.1 days). Flow was measured by pneumotachography and tidal volume was derived as digitally integrated flow, and transpulmonary pressure as airway minus esophageal pressure. We found that C and R calculated from the equation of motion is accurate only if C and R remain constant throughout the respiratory cycle. Calculated compliance depends more on C at the end than at the beginning of inspiration. A decreasing C leads to underestimation or R, while an increasing C leads to an overestimation of inspiratory R. Calculated total R may be accurate, but with low r values for measurement points. Mead and Wittenberger's method and the regression method are similarly affected by changing C; however, since the regression method is based on many more measurement points and therefore allows the detection and analysis of within-breath changes of C and R, it is less prone to erroneous results secondary to signal artifacts than Mead and Wittenberger's method.


Assuntos
Resistência das Vias Respiratórias/fisiologia , Simulação por Computador , Recém-Nascido Prematuro/fisiologia , Modelos Lineares , Complacência Pulmonar/fisiologia , Pulmão/fisiologia , Modelos Biológicos , Volume de Ventilação Pulmonar , Humanos , Lactente , Recém-Nascido , Pressão , Ventilação Pulmonar
8.
Physiol Behav ; 50(3): 555-61, 1991 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1801009

RESUMO

Rats were trained on a series of 9 tasks, each of which required discrimination among 8 different and unique odors. Discrimination accuracy improved across successive problems and, by the end of training, most rats made few errors after their initial exposure to each new odor. Despite the number of stimuli to be discriminated, this acquisition of a learning set was not appreciably different from that demonstrated in an earlier study that used only 2 odors per task. In subsequent retention tests, most rats also showed excellent memory for odors used in prior problems.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Memória/fisiologia , Odorantes , Olfato/fisiologia , Animais , Aprendizagem por Discriminação/fisiologia , Masculino , Ratos
9.
Physiol Behav ; 53(4): 795-804, 1993 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8511188

RESUMO

To assess olfactory matching-to-sample learning, rats were exposed to two odors separated by a 1-s presentation of clean air. If, and only if, the odors were identical, a response produced a water reinforcer. High levels of performance were maintained over a series of 10 novel three-odor matching-to-sample problems on this conditional go/no-go discrimination procedure. In general, performance accuracy improved over problems and errorless or near errorless performance on many stimulus combinations, particularly near the end of training, indicated acquisition of a learning set. There was little decrement in performance when the interstimulus interval was increased gradually from 1 to 10 s and matching-to-sample was not disrupted when a novel odor was presented during the interstimulus interval. These results demonstrate that rats readily learn an olfactory matching-to-sample task, maintain high levels of performance even with delays of 10-s between stimuli, and can acquire a matching-to-sample learning set. The outcomes are in agreement with prior studies demonstrating exceptional learning of instrumental tasks by rats when they are provided with odor cues.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem por Discriminação , Rememoração Mental , Olfato , Animais , Atenção , Masculino , Odorantes , Resolução de Problemas , Ratos , Retenção Psicológica
10.
Laryngoscope ; 101(3): 297-304, 1991 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2000019

RESUMO

The gas contents of the physiological middle ear periodically cycle through two phases in steady state. During phase I, the eustachian tube is shut and the middle ear gas space is effectively closed. Gas is absorbed or produced at the mucosal surface, and the total pressure changes correspondingly. During phase II, which is of very short duration, the eustachian tube opens, a bolus of gas passes between the middle ear and the nasopharynx, and the total pressure in the middle ear rapidly adjusts to that in the nasopharynx. Since nasopharyngeal pressure fluctuates in time, so does the pressure in the middle ear. The effect of these pressure changes is to produce a level of ventilation in the middle ear, which depends on a combination of three parameters: the volume of the middle ear, multiplied by the mean amplitude of pressure variations in the nasopharynx, divided by the mean elapsed time between successive eustachian tube openings. Assuming steady-state conditions, the composition of middle ear gas can be computed and is predicted to range from PN2 = 621, PO2 = 46, PCO2 = 46, PH2O = 47 mm Hg in the case when nasopharynx fluctuations are small, to a match with nasopharyngeal gas composition, when the fluctuations are large.


Assuntos
Orelha Média/fisiologia , Gases/metabolismo , Transporte Biológico , Tuba Auditiva/fisiologia , Humanos , Matemática , Modelos Biológicos , Nasofaringe/fisiologia , Pressão
11.
Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg ; 117(12): 1390-4, 1991 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1845267

RESUMO

Transient increases in total pressure in the ear (1) during sleep, after hypoventilating in a supine position with a closed eustachian tube, and (2) after the partial pressures in middle-ear gas are lowered by a total pressure decrease and the eustachian tube is voluntarily maintained closed can be accounted for quantitatively on the basis of the standard mucosal gas exchange model and the following data: (1) partial pressures in tissue: pN2* = 573 mm Hg (7621 decaPascals [daPa]), pO2* = 40 mm Hg (532 daPa), pCO2* = 46 mm Hg (612 daPa), and pH2O* R = 47 mm Hg (625 daPa); (2) partial pressures in the nasopharynx: pN2' = 566 mm Hg (7528 daPa), pO2' = 120 mm Hg (1596 daPa), pCO2' = 27 mm Hg (359 daPa), and pH20' = 47 mm Hg (625 daPa); (3) a middle-ear gas space of 2 x 10(-5) m3; (4) an absorption rate for nitrogen, when the partial pressure difference is 1 atm, of 3 x 10(15) molecules per second; and (5) mucosal absorption rates for oxygen and carbon dioxide 1.8 and 34 times larger, respectively, than for nitrogen.


Assuntos
Orelha Média/fisiologia , Tuba Auditiva/fisiologia , Humanos , Modelos Biológicos , Pressão , Respiração , Decúbito Dorsal
12.
Adv Exp Med Biol ; 144: 53-74, 1982.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7044068

RESUMO

Discussing the available evidence a fairly strong case can be made for the existence of a basic glycoprotein unit, characterized by what may be a common protein backbone (Fig. 1). This is far less likely for the carbohydrate portion. The considerably more variability in the amount and composition of the carbohydrate coat and species and organ differences may arise because of this fact. Very large aggregates are built up from the basic unit using cross-links of disulfide bonds either intermolecularly, i.e. directly, or intramolecularly, i.e. indirectly via a possible lectin-like structure which forms its bond with some of the carbohydrate side chains. Structures of the order of 10-100 million molecular weight are to be expected which, being heavily entangled, give rise to the special rheological character of the mucus. In most instances mucus behaves rheologically like a gel. The concentration of glycoprotein in the mucus may be the most important parameter which determines the special rheological features required in a special functional context. A unified point of view, when discussing mucus structure and function, was taken. On the evidence available, it seems well justified to continue to do so.


Assuntos
Muco/fisiologia , Fenômenos Químicos , Química , Epitélio/fisiologia , Géis , Glicoproteínas/fisiologia , Humanos , Peso Molecular , Peptídeos/análise , Polissacarídeos/análise
13.
Ann Otol Rhinol Laryngol ; 101(5): 445-9, 1992 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1570940

RESUMO

As gas flows in and out of the nasopharynx, the pressure in that region fluctuates. It drops below or rises above atmospheric pressure, which is itself not constant but is subject to changes in altitude and weather. Such pressure changes in the nasopharynx produce a pumping of gas into and out of the middle ear. The net amount of middle ear gas transferred from or to the nasopharynx will, component for component, in steady state exactly equal the amount of middle ear gas transferred to or from the microcirculation by means of diffusional absorption by (or release from) the mucosa. In the case of a permanently patulous eustachian tube, a single parameter, characteristic of the rate of ventilation through the open eustachian tube, is found to determine the gas composition in the middle ear, whereas in the case of a middle ear ventilated by tympanostomy, two rate-of-ventilation parameters, one for gas flow through the ventilation tube and one for flow through a periodically open eustachian tube, determine the steady state gas composition. A high rate of ventilation favors absorption of oxygen and venting of carbon dioxide from the middle ear in both cases.


Assuntos
Dióxido de Carbono/fisiologia , Orelha Média/fisiologia , Modelos Biológicos , Nitrogênio/fisiologia , Oxigênio/fisiologia , Dióxido de Carbono/análise , Otopatias/fisiopatologia , Tuba Auditiva/fisiopatologia , Humanos , Nitrogênio/análise , Oxigênio/análise , Pressão
14.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 57(1): 119-25, 1992 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16812646

RESUMO

Three rats earned their daily food ration by responding during individual trials either on a lever that delivered one food pellet immediately or on a second lever that delivered three pellets after a delay that was continuously adjusted to ensure substantial responding to both alternatives. Choice of the delayed reinforcer increased when the number of trials per session was reduced. This result suggests that models seeking closure on choice effects must include a parameter reflecting how preference changes with sessionwide income. Moreover, models positing that reinforcer probability and immediacy (1/delay) function equivalently in choice are called into question by the finding that probability and immediacy produce opposing effects when income level is changed.

15.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 53(2): 273-84, 1990 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16812610

RESUMO

Three experiments examined the effect of food availability on pigeons' choice behavior under concurrent schedules of reinforcement. In Experiment 1, 3 pigeons earned their daily food ration by choosing, in 30-min sessions, between concurrent variable-ratio 30 variable-interval 40-s schedules. Food presentations during both schedules lasted 2 or 12 s, depending upon the condition. Relative variable-ratio response rate was inversely related to hopper duration. In Experiment 2, 4 pigeons received their daily feeding by responding on the same schedule pair as in Experiment 1 (with 4-s food presentations) in sessions that varied in length from 10 to 30 min, depending on the condition. The length of a vertical slit projected on a response key increased with time so that "passage of time" might be more easily discriminable. As session duration decreased, relative variable-ratio response rate increased. In Experiment 3, 4 pigeons chose between two variable-interval 40-s schedules. One schedule operated without regard to the schedule selected, whereas the other operated only when the subject responded in its presence (dependent). Although these schedules had the same feedback function, preference for the dependent variable interval increased as session duration decreased from 30 to 10 min. The preference changes in these studies reveal the operation of an income-maximizing process in choice.

16.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 22(1): 21-30, 1974 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16811779

RESUMO

Two experimental chambers were electrically connected so that the component selected by a pigeon confronting concurrent variable-interval schedules in one chamber could be successively presented as a multiple schedule to a second pigeon in the other chamber. Component duration was regulated by the use of a changeover delay, the value of which was systematically varied between 0 and 30 sec. It was found that the relative local response rates on the preferred key (absolute response rate to that component divided by the sum of the absolute response rates during both components) tended to increase with increasing component durations for the birds in the concurrent chamber, but decreased for the birds in the multiple chamber. These data support the interpretation that there are fundamental differences in the mode of responding to multiple and concurrent schedules. Based on these findings, it was concluded that previous demonstrations of matching on multiple schedules do not establish that response allocation is controlled by a process equivalent to that found on choice paradigms. It now appears that matching on multiple (but not concurrent) schedules is a consequence of selecting short component durations. The implications of these data for Herrnstein's (1970) and Rachlin's (1973) formulations of the relationship between multiple and concurrent schedules are examined.

17.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 22(3): 497-506, 1974 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16811813

RESUMO

Pairing the presentations of one lever (cueing lever) with food led to the acquisition and persistence of lever contacts by rats. This behavior did not occur with a second lever, which was presented randomly with regard to food delivery. This finding obtained whether food delivery was independent of cueing-lever contacts (positive automaintenance) or dependent on the absence of cueing-lever contacts (negative automaintenance). The general findings were: (1) cueing-lever contacts on the positive automaintenance procedure occurred on a higher proportion of trials and at higher rates when contacts occurred than on the negative automaintenance procedure; (2) instances of the cueing lever's failure to support responding were more frequent on the negative than the positive automaintenance procedure; and (3) the topography and median contact duration of positively automaintained responding differed from negatively automaintained responding. These findings agree substantially with the automaintenance literature on pigeons, suggesting that similar processes may characterize automaintained responding in both pigeons and rats.

18.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 13(2): 187-97, 1970 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16811435

RESUMO

Pigeons distribute their responses on concurrently available variable-interval schedules in the same proportion as reinforcements are distributed on the two schedules only when a changeover delay is used. The present study shows that this equality between proportions of responses and proportions of reinforcements ("matching") is obtained when the value of the changeover delay is varied. When responses are partitioned into the set of rapid response bursts occurring during the delay interval and the set of responses occurring subsequently, the proportion of neither set of responses matches the proportion of reinforcements. Instead, each set deviates from matching but in opposite directions. Matching on the gross level results from the interaction of two patterns evident in the local response rates: (I) the lengthening of the changeover delay response burst is accompanied by a commensurate decrease in the number of changeovers; (2) the changeover delay response burst is longer than the scheduled delay duration. When delay responses are eliminated by introducing a blackout during the delay interval, response matching is eliminated; the pigeon, however, continues to match the proportion of time spent responding on a key to the proportion of reinforcements obtained on that key.

19.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 41(1): 83-100, 1984 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16812360

RESUMO

Herrnstein and Heyman (1979) showed that when pigeons' pecking is reinforced on concurrent variable-interval variable-ratio schedules, (1) their behavior ratios match the ratio of the schedules' reinforcer frequencies, and (2) there is more responding on the variable interval. Since maximizing the reinforcement rate would require responding more on the variable ratio, these results were presented as establishing the primacy of matching over maximizing. In the present report, different ratios of behavior were simulated on a computer to see how they would affect reinforcement rates on these concurrent schedules. Over a wide range of experimenter-specified choice ratios, matching obtained - a result suggesting that changes in choice allocation produced changes in reinforcer frequencies that correspond to the matching outcome. Matching also occurred at arbitrarily selected choice ratios when reinforcement rates were algebraically determined by each schedule's reinforcement-feedback function. Additionally, three birds were exposed to concurrent variable-interval variable-ratio schedules contingent on key pecking in which hopper durations were varied in some conditions to produce experimenter-specified choice ratios. Matching generally obtained between choice ratios and reinforcer-frequency ratios at these different choice ratios. By suggesting that reinforcer frequencies track choice on this procedure, instead of vice versa, this outcome questions whether matching-as-outcome was due to matching-as-process in the Herrnstein and Heyman study.

20.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 30(1): 107-22, 1978 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16812081

RESUMO

Thirteen master pigeons were exposed to multiple schedules in which reinforcement frequency (Experiment I) or duration (Experiment II) was varied. In Phases 1 and 3 of Experiment I, the values of the first and second components' random-interval schedules were 33 and 99 seconds, respectively. In Phase 2, these values were 99 seconds for both components. In Experiment II, a random-interval 33-second schedule was associated with each component. During Phases 1 and 3, the first and second components had hopper durations of 7.5 and 2.5 seconds respectively. During Phase 2, both components' hopper durations were 2.5 seconds. In each experiment, positive contrast obtained for about half the master subjects. The rest showed a rate increase in both components (positive induction). Each master subject's key colors and reinforcers were synchronously presented on a response-independent basis to a yoked control. Richer component key-pecking occurred during each experiment's Phases 1 and 3 among half these subjects. However, none responded during the contrast condition (unchanged component of each experiment's Phase 2). From this it is inferred that autoshaping did not contribute to the contrast and induction findings among master birds. Little evidence of local contrast (highest rate at beginning of richer component) was found in any subject. These data show that (a) contrast can occur independently from autoshaping, (b) contrast assays during equal-valued components may produce induction, (c) local contrast in multiple schedules often does not occur, and (d) differential hopper durations can produce autoshaping and contrast.

SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA