Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 17 de 17
Filtrar
1.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev ; 5(3): 307-14, 1981.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7029359

RESUMO

This paper provides an introduction to the field of behavioral pharmacology. A brief description of the main characteristics of behavior analysis is followed by a review of the contributions of experimental pharmacology to behavior analysis and vice versa. Finally, a section on new procedures in behavioral pharmacology outlines experimental situations in which complex kinds of behavior can to studied. These include: (a) temporal discrimination; (b) preference to be free and informed; (c) commitment, choice and self-control; and (d) high-order concept formation.


Assuntos
Comportamento Animal/efeitos dos fármacos , Psicofarmacologia , Animais , Comportamento de Escolha/efeitos dos fármacos , Discriminação Psicológica/efeitos dos fármacos , Generalização do Estímulo/efeitos dos fármacos , Esquema de Reforço , Fatores de Tempo
2.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 17(1): 45-9, 1972 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16811566

RESUMO

Pigeons were trained on a multiple variable-interval 30-sec, variable-interval 90-sec schedule with each component presented alternately for an equal (on the average) duration. This average duration of exposure to each component was varied from 5 to 300 sec. The main concern was with rate of response in the variable-interval 30-sec component relative to rate of response in the variable-interval 90-sec component. In all cases, rate of response was higher in the variable-interval 30 sec component, but the discrepancy in the rate produced by the two schedules tended to be greatest when the duration of component presentation was brief. The mean proportion of responses emitted during the variable-interval 30-sec component (responses in variable-interval 30-sec component divided by total responses) varied from about 0.60 to 0.71, where 0.75 would be expected on the basis of a matching rule, and 0.59 was that obtained by Lander and Irwin (1968). These results are in agreement with data reported by Shimp and Wheatley (1971) from a similar experiment.

3.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 19(3): 451-8, 1973 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16811675

RESUMO

Frequency and magnitude of reinforcement were varied in concurrent variable-interval variable-interval schedules of reinforcement. The relative response rate to the two stimuli did not support the notion that choice approximately matches relative total access to food (the product of frequency and magnitude of reinforcement in one schedule divided by the sum of products of frequency and magnitude in both schedules). Relative response rates matched relative reinforcement value when that measure was adjusted to give more emphasis to reinforcement frequency than to reinforcement duration.

4.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 16(1): 51-62, 1971 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16811534

RESUMO

Pigeons' key-pecking responses were maintained under concurrently available variable-interval schedules of reinforcement. Responses in the presence of two different key-colors were reinforced on two independent and concurrent variable-interval schedules of food reinforcement, each associated with one of the key colors (red or green). Pecks at a second key (changeover key), always white, would alternate the colors on the main key. In Exp. 1 and 2, electric shock of 50 msec duration followed immediately after changeovers. The proportion of responses in the presence of the color associated with the higher frequency of reinforcements per hour was a direct function of shock intensity contingent on changeovers. When both schedules provided equal number of reinforcements per hour, there was no systematic effect of shock intensity on response distribution. In Exp. 3, a timeout period was contingent on changeovers, and response distribution was a function of timeout length.

5.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 34(3): 329-33, 1980 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16812193

RESUMO

Three pigeons were studied on concurrent, unsignaled, avoidance schedules in a two-key procedure. Shock-shock intervals were two seconds in both schedules. The response-shock interval on one key was always 22 seconds, while the response-shock interval associated with the other key was varied from 7 to 52 seconds in different experimental conditions. Response rates on the key associated with the varied schedule tended to decrease when the response-shock interval length was increased. Responding on the key associated with the constant schedule was not systematically affected.

6.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 27(2): 281-91, 1977 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16811990

RESUMO

Two pigeons were trained to peck a key under a free-operant avoidance schedule. Then, changes in key color signalled the beginning (safe period) and the end (warning period) of the response-shock interval, with a response required to change the key color. Finally, a change in key color signalled the warning period and either a response or a shock reinstated the safe stimulus. During signalled avoidance, response rate was higher during the warning stimulus than during the safe stimulus. More responding tended to occur in the warning stimulus when it was terminated by either a response or a shock than by only a response. In either procedure, response latency during the warning stimulus was a function of the duration of the warning stimulus. In general, response and shock rate were higher during unsignalled than during signalled avoidance. When the warning stimulus was brief, the results were similar to those of unsignalled avoidance. These results confirm previous findings with pigeons, are in general agreement with data provided by other species in studies of signalled avoidance, and thereby indicate the transituationality of the key-pecking operant.

7.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 60(2): 415-35, 1993 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16812704

RESUMO

Eight pigeons were trained on concurrent variable-interval variable-interval schedules with a minimum interchangeover time programmed as a consequence of changeovers. In Experiment 1 the reinforcement schedules remained constant while the minimum interchangeover time varied from 0 to 200 s. Relative response rates and relative time deviated from relative reinforcement rates toward indifference with long minimum interchangeover times. In Experiment 2 different reinforcement ratios were scheduled in successive experimental conditions with the minimum interchangeover time constant at 0, 2, 10, or 120 s. The exponent of the generalized matching equation was close to 1.0 when the minimum interchangeover time was 0 s (the typical procedure for concurrent schedules without a changeover delay) and decreased as that duration was increased. The data support the momentary maximizing theory and contradict molar maximizing theories and the melioration theory.

8.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 22(1): 215-8, 1974 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16811780

RESUMO

Five pigeons were exposed to an unsignalled avoidance procedure where key pecks were maintained through shock postponement. Functions obtained showed an inverse relationship between rate of responding and length of the response-shock interval, while changes in the shock-shock interval had no systematic effect on response rates. The rate of shocks delivered generally decreased with increases in length of both response-shock and shock-shock intervals. Results show that key pecking in pigeons, maintained through an unsignalled avoidance procedure, was affected by changes in response-shock and shock-shock intervals in the same manner as other responses in pigeons and in rats.

9.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 19(2): 211-8, 1973 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16811659

RESUMO

Four pigeons were trained to avoid shock by pecking a key on a free-operant avoidance schedule in which no exteroceptive stimulus signalled impending shock. Response rate was an inverse function of response-shock interval when shock-shock interval was held constant at 2 sec and response-shock intervals varied from 5 to 40 sec. Amphetamine increased response rates in two subjects and reserpine markedly reduced responding in one.

10.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 58(2): 335-47, 1992 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16812669

RESUMO

Six experimentally naive pigeons were exposed to concurrent variable-interval variable-interval schedules in a three-key procedure in which food reinforcement followed pecks on the side keys and pecks on the center key served as changeover responses. In Phase 1, 3 birds were exposed to 20 combinations of five variable-interval values, with each variable-interval value consistently associated with a different color on the side keys. Another 3 pigeons were exposed to the same 20 conditions, but with a more standard procedure that used a nondifferential discriminative stimulus on the two side keys throughout all conditions. In Phase 2, the differential and nondifferential stimulus conditions were reversed for each pigeon. Each condition lasted for one 5-hr session and one subsequent 1-hr session. In the last 14 conditions of each phase, the presence of differential discriminative stimuli decreased the time necessary for differential responding to develop and increased the sensitivity of behavior to reinforcement distribution in the 1st hr of training; during the last hours of training in each condition, however, the effects of the differential discriminative stimuli could not be distinguished from the effects of reinforcement distribution per se. These results show the importance of studying transitions in behavior as well as final performance. They may also be relevant to discrepancies in the results of previous experiments that have used nonhuman and human subjects.

11.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 41(2): 157-67, 1984 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16812364

RESUMO

Two pigeons, with previous exposure to concurrent schedules, were submitted to 29 sessions of 8 hours each with concurrent variable-interval variable-interval schedules in which reinforcement parameters changed from session to session. In the first nine sessions reinforcement durations were equal in both schedules while reinforcement frequencies varied; in Sessions 10 through 18, both frequency and duration of reinforcement were varied; in Sessions 19 through 29, only reinforcement duration was varied. Results with this different procedure confirm previous findings that behavior is more sensitive to changes in reinforcement frequency than to reinforcement magnitude.

12.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 42(1): 67-74, 1984 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16812381

RESUMO

Five rats were submitted to a signaled free-operant avoidance contingency. Throughout the experiment, shock intensity was varied from 0.1 to 8.0 mA, with shock duration constant at 200 milleseconds. Results indicate: (a) an all-or-none effect of shock intensity on response and shock rates, on percentage of shocks avoided, and on frequency of occurrence of responding during the preshock stimulus; and (b) no systematic effect of shock intensity on stimulus control, measured either by the percentage of stimulus presentations accompanied by a response or by the percentage of responses that occurred during those preshock stimuli. Such results indicate that for each subject there is a minimum shock intensity necessary to establish and maintain avoidance responding; intensities higher than this minimum value have little or no effect on responding (with an upper limit for those strong intensities with a general disruptive effect on behavior).

13.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 40(2): 99-111, 1983 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-6631314

RESUMO

Five pigeons were exposed to different pairs of concurrent variable-interval, variable-interval schedules on nine experimental conditions of 30 sessions each. For every session, the parameters of the generalized matching equation were computed for the first five, six, seven, eight, and nine experimental conditions. The exponent a, both for response and time distribution, tended to decrease with increases in number of experimental conditions and to increase with number of sessions per condition, but values of k (bias) varied unsystematically. When the subjects were exposed to five new pairs of schedules, with 55 sessions per condition, the findings were confirmed. Data from the literature on the generalized matching law suggest that the variability of exponent values may be explained in part by the use of naive or experienced subjects in different investigations and by the variability in number of experimental conditions and in number of sessions per condition.


Assuntos
Comportamento de Escolha , Percepção de Cores , Aprendizagem por Discriminação , Prática Psicológica , Animais , Columbidae , Masculino , Esquema de Reforço
14.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 54(2): 151-2, 1990 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16812616
15.
Nephron ; 24(2): 78-80, 1979.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-492414

RESUMO

Two groups of rats were submitted to a 2-hour training session under a free operant avoidance of shock contingency 23 weeks after surgery, which provoked a state of chronic renal failure (experimental group) or after a sham operation (control group). Subjects in the control group had superior performances than those from the experimental group when total of shocks received and total numbers of avoidance and escape responses were compared. Results of biochemical blood analyses obtained from samples taken immediately after the avoidance training session indicate that the experimental group showed higher plasma urea concentration, higher levels of creatinine and osmolarity. The data show that the procedure used to provoke chronic renal failure was successful, and results in general confirm and extend findings relative to the effects of acute renal failure on behavior.


Assuntos
Comportamento Animal , Falência Renal Crônica/psicologia , Animais , Aprendizagem da Esquiva , Creatinina/sangue , Reação de Fuga , Falência Renal Crônica/sangue , Masculino , Ratos , Ureia/sangue
16.
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA