Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 27
Filtrar
1.
J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis ; 37(4): A237-A243, 2020 Apr 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32400548

RESUMEN

Color and brightness constancies may not be independent, since increasing a sample saturation should decrease the demand on the color constancy and increase that on the brightness constancy [Color Res. Appl.43, 630 (2018)CREADU0361-231710.1002/col.22227]. We tested this claim using color and brightness constancy data from Foster et al. [Vis. Res.41, 285 (2001)VISRAM0042-698910.1016/S0042-6989(00)00239-X], whose observers made side-by-side and successive comparisons of central patches ("asymmetric matches") presented in pairs of identical Mondrian displays with simulated illuminants of 25000 K and 6700 K daylights. Saturations (CIE "chroma") of the central patches varied from 0.007 to 0.092. For most observers (as in a toy Gaussian model of the surface reflectance spectra), increasing saturation reduced color constancy and increased brightness constancy.

2.
J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis ; 36(4): 606-627, 2019 Apr 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31044981

RESUMEN

This tutorial offers an introduction to terrestrial and close-range hyperspectral imaging and some of its uses in human color vision research. The main types of hyperspectral cameras are described together with procedures for image acquisition, postprocessing, and calibration for either radiance or reflectance data. Image transformations are defined for colorimetric representations, color rendering, and cone receptor and postreceptor coding. Several example applications are also presented. These include calculating the color properties of scenes, such as gamut volume and metamerism, and analyzing the utility of color in observer tasks, such as identifying surfaces under illuminant changes. The effects of noise and uncertainty are considered in both image acquisition and color vision applications.


Asunto(s)
Visión de Colores , Imagen Óptica/métodos , Colorimetría , Humanos , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador , Imagen Óptica/instrumentación
3.
J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis ; 35(4): B324-B333, 2018 Apr 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29603961

RESUMEN

Popular color reproductions of art paintings such as postcards are intended to remind viewers of the original works. It is, however, unclear how well the quality of the reproductions is preserved under various illuminations. Color constancy of the reproductions in relation to colors in the original paintings was estimated computationally with hyperspectral images of 15th-century Flemish paintings, 20th-century modern abstract paintings, and their corresponding postcards with a series of illuminants: the CIE daylight D65 with correlated color temperature (CCT) 6500 K, daylight D40, fluorescent lamps F2 and F11, and a LED lamp designed for museums with CCT approximately 3500-4000 K. Despite large colorimetric differences between the types of art paintings and between the illuminants simulated, local areas showed good color constancy: skin areas in the Flemish paintings ranged from 0.76 to 0.81, whereas nonskin areas ranged from 0.19 to 0.68. This result suggests that viewers may be able to achieve color constancy with the reproduction postcards by disregarding inconsistent colors representations from the original paintings caused by changes in illumination conditions.

4.
J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis ; 31(4): A254-62, 2014 Apr 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24695179

RESUMEN

Where observers concentrate their gaze during visual search depends on several factors. The aim here was to determine how much of the variance in observers' fixations in natural scenes can be explained by local scene color and how that variance is related to viewing bias. Fixation data were taken from an experiment in which observers searched images of 20 natural rural and urban scenes for a small target. The proportion R2 of the variance explained in a regression on local color properties (lightness and the red-green and yellow-blue chromatic components) ranged from 1% to 85%, depending mainly on how well those properties were consistent with observers' viewing bias. When viewing bias was included in the regression, values of R2 increased, ranging from 62% to 96%. By comparison, local lightness and local lightness contrast, edge density, and entropy each explained less variance than local color properties. Local scene color may have a much stronger influence on gaze position than is generally recognized, capturing significant aspects of scene structure on target search behavior.

5.
J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis ; 29(2): A194-9, 2012 Feb 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22330379

RESUMEN

Success in visually searching for a small object or target in a natural scene depends on many factors, including the spatial structure of the scene and the pattern of observers' eye movements. The aim of this study was to determine to what extent local color properties of natural scenes can account for target-detection performance. A computer-controlled high-resolution color monitor was used to present images of natural scenes containing a small, randomly located, shaded gray sphere, which served as the target. Observers' gaze position was simultaneously monitored with an infrared video eye-tracker. About 60% of the adjusted variance in observers' detection performance was accounted for by local color properties, namely, lightness and the red-green and blue-yellow components of chroma. A similar level of variance was accounted for by observers' fixations. These results suggest that local color can be as influential as gaze position in determining observers' search performance in natural scenes.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Color/fisiología , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Adulto , Color , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Análisis de Regresión , Adulto Joven
6.
PLoS One ; 15(12): e0233816, 2020.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33315862

RESUMEN

The main ingredient of sunless tanning products is dihydroxyacetone (DHA). DHA reacts with the protein and amino acid composition in the surface layers of the skin, producing melanoidins, which changes the skin colour, imitating natural skin tan caused by melanin. The purpose of this study was to characterise DHA-induced skin colour changes and to test whether we can predict the outcome of DHA application on skin tone changes. To assess the DHA-induced skin colour shift quantitatively, colorimetric and spectral measurements of the inner forearm were obtained before, four hours and 24 hours after application of a 7.5% concentration DHA gel in the experimental group (n = 100). In a control group (n = 60), the same measurements were obtained on both the inner forearm (infrequently sun-exposed) and the outer forearm (frequently sun-exposed); the difference between these two areas was defined as the naturally occurring tan. Skin colour shifts caused by DHA tanning and by natural tanning were compared in terms of lightness (L*), redness (a*) and yellowness (b*) in the standard CIELAB colour space. Naturalness of the DHA-induced skin tan was evaluated by comparing the trajectory of the chromaticity distribution in (L*, b*) space with that of naturally occurring tan. Twenty-four hours after DHA application, approximately 20% of the skin colour samples became excessively yellow, with chromaticities outside the natural range in (L*, b*) space. A principal component analysis was used to characterise the tanning pathway. Skin colour shifts induced by DHA were predicted by a multiple regression on the chromaticities and the skin properties. The model explained up to 49% of variance in colorimetric components with a median error of less than 2 ΔE. We conclude that the control of both the magnitude and the direction of the colour shift is a critical factor to achieve a natural appearance.


Asunto(s)
Dihidroxiacetona/farmacología , Pigmentación de la Piel/efectos de los fármacos , Piel/efectos de los fármacos , Adulto , Color , Colorimetría/métodos , Dihidroxiacetona/análisis , Dihidroxiacetona/química , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Baño de Sol , Protectores Solares/análisis , Protectores Solares/química
7.
Iperception ; 11(2): 2041669520915734, 2020.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32313615

RESUMEN

We estimated Trump's skin colour from 70 internet images and also from the "twitter tan line" image (February 8, 2020; Twitter). We then compared the estimated skin colours with two existing data sets of skin colours: the range of skin tans that occur naturally in the Caucasian population and the range skin colours brought about by a sunless tan. We find that Trump's skin colour is close to the edge of the natural skin tan gamut and firmly within the gamut of a sunless skin tan. The skin colour above Trump's tan line is outside of the naturally occurring range of skin colours, even outside the skin tan of nonmelanized albinos. The latter finding is consistent with the hypothesis that part of the image may have been digitally distorted.

8.
J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis ; 26(11): B14-24, 2009 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19884913

RESUMEN

Variations in illumination on a scene and trichromatic sampling by the eye limit inferences about scene content. The aim of this work was to elucidate these limits in relation to an ideal observer using color signals alone. Simulations were based on 50 hyperspectral images of natural scenes and daylight illuminants with correlated color temperatures 4000 K, 6500 K, and 25,000 K. Estimates were made of the (Shannon) information available from each scene, the redundancies in receptoral and postreceptoral coding, and the information retrieved by an observer identifying corresponding points across image pairs. For the largest illuminant difference, between 25,000 K and 4000 K, a postreceptoral transformation providing minimum redundancy yielded an efficiency of about 80% in the information retrieved. This increased to about 89% when the transformation was optimized directly for information retrieved, corresponding to an equivalent Gaussian noise amplitude of 3.0% or to a mean of 3.6 x 10(4) distinct identifiable points per scene. Using color signals to retrieve information from natural scenes can approach ideal observer efficiency levels.

9.
Vision Res ; 130: 76-84, 2017 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27913105

RESUMEN

One hypothesis to explain the aesthetics of paintings is that it depends on the extent to which they mimic natural image statistics. In fact, paintings and natural scenes share several statistical image regularities but the colors of paintings seem generally more biased towards red than natural scenes. Is the particular option for colors in each painting, even if less naturalistic, critical for perceived beauty? Here we show that it is. In the experiments, 50 naïve observers, unfamiliar with the 10 paintings tested, could rotate the color gamut of the paintings and select the one producing the best subjective impression. The distributions of angles obtained are described by normal distributions with maxima deviating, on average, only 7 degrees from the original gamut orientation and full width at half maximum just above the threshold to perceive a chromatic change in the paintings. Crucially, for data pooled across observers and abstract paintings the maximum of the distribution was at zero degrees, i.e., the same as the original. This demonstrates that artists know what chromatic compositions match viewers' preferences and that the option for less naturalistic colors does not constrain the aesthetic value of paintings.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Color/fisiología , Color , Estética , Pinturas , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
10.
Vision Res ; 120: 39-44, 2016 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26291072

RESUMEN

In natural complex environments, the elevation of the sun and the presence of occluding objects and mutual reflections cause variations in the spectral composition of the local illumination across time and location. Unlike the changes in time and their consequences for color appearance and constancy, the spatial variations of local illumination color in natural scenes have received relatively little attention. The aim of the present work was to characterize these spatial variations by spectral imaging. Hyperspectral radiance images were obtained from 30 rural and urban scenes in which neutral probe spheres were embedded. The spectra of the local illumination at 17 sample points on each sphere in each scene were extracted and a total of 1904 chromaticity coordinates and correlated color temperatures (CCTs) derived. Maximum differences in chromaticities over spheres and over scenes were similar. When data were pooled over scenes, CCTs ranged from 3000 K to 20,000 K, a variation of the same order of magnitude as that occurring over the day. Any mechanisms that underlie stable surface color perception in natural scenes need to accommodate these large spatial variations in local illumination color.


Asunto(s)
Visión de Colores/fisiología , Luz , Iluminación , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Humanos
11.
Vision Res ; 120: 45-60, 2016 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25847405

RESUMEN

The illumination in natural environments varies through the day. Stable inferences about surface color might be supported by spatial ratios of cone excitations from the reflected light, but their invariance has been quantified only for global changes in illuminant spectrum. The aim here was to test their invariance under natural changes in both illumination spectrum and geometry, especially in the distribution of shadows. Time-lapse hyperspectral radiance images were acquired from five outdoor vegetated and nonvegetated scenes. From each scene, 10,000 pairs of points were sampled randomly and ratios measured across time. Mean relative deviations in ratios were generally large, but when sampling was limited to short distances or moderate time intervals, they fell below the level for detecting violations in ratio invariance. When illumination changes with uneven geometry were excluded, they fell further, to levels obtained with global changes in illuminant spectrum alone. Within sampling constraints, ratios of cone excitations, and also of opponent-color combinations, provide an approximately invariant signal for stable surface-color inferences, despite spectral and geometric variations in scene illumination.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Color/fisiología , Iluminación , Células Fotorreceptoras Retinianas Conos/fisiología , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Humanos , Estimulación Luminosa , Imagen de Lapso de Tiempo
12.
Proc Biol Sci ; 271(1555): 2319-26, 2004 Nov 22.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15556884

RESUMEN

Two kinds of constancy underlie the everyday perception of surface colour: constancy under changes in illuminant and constancy under changes in surface position. Classically, these two constancies seem to place conflicting demands on the visual system: to both take into account the region surrounding a surface and also discount it. It is shown here, however, that the ability of observers to make surface-colour matches across simultaneous changes in test-surface position and illuminant in computer-generated 'Mondrian' patterns is almost as good as across changes in illuminant alone. Performance was no poorer when the surfaces surrounding the test surface were permuted, or when information from a potential comparison surface, the one with the highest luminance, was suppressed. Computer simulations of cone-photoreceptor activity showed that a reliable cue for making surface-colour matches in all experimental conditions was provided by the ratios of cone excitations between the test surfaces and a spatial average over the whole pattern.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Color/fisiología , Color , Iluminación , Adulto , Simulación por Computador , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Células Fotorreceptoras Retinianas Conos/fisiología , Propiedades de Superficie
13.
Vision Res ; 57: 18-25, 2012 Mar 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22227608

RESUMEN

There is no standard method for classifying eye fixations. Thresholds for speed, acceleration, duration, and stability of point of gaze have each been employed to demarcate data, but they have no commonly accepted values. Here, some general distributional properties of eye movements were used to construct a simple method for classifying fixations, without parametric assumptions or expert judgment. The method was primarily speed-based, but the required optimum speed threshold was derived automatically from individual data for each observer and stimulus with the aid of Tibshirani, Walther, and Hastie's 'gap statistic'. An optimum duration threshold, also derived automatically from individual data, was used to eliminate the effects of instrumental noise. The method was tested on data recorded from a video eye-tracker sampling at 250 frames a second while experimental observers viewed static natural scenes in over 30,000 one-second trials. The resulting classifications were compared with those by three independent expert visual classifiers, with 88-94% agreement, and also against two existing parametric methods. Robustness to instrumental noise and sampling rate were verified in separate simulations. The method was applied to the recorded data to illustrate the variation of mean fixation duration and saccade amplitude across observers and scenes.


Asunto(s)
Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Umbral Sensorial/fisiología , Estadísticas no Paramétricas , Factores de Tiempo , Adulto Joven
14.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 51(4): 2286-93, 2010 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19892868

RESUMEN

Purpose. Color-vision deficiency is associated with abnormalities in color matching and color discrimination, but its impact on the ability of people to judge the constancy of surface colors under different lights (color constancy) is less clear. This work had two aims: first, to quantify the degree of color constancy in subjects with congenital red-green color deficiency; second, to test whether the degree of color constancy in anomalous trichromats can be predicted from their Rayleigh anomaloscope matches. Methods. Color constancy of red-green color-deficient subjects was tested in a task requiring the discrimination of illuminant changes from surface-reflectance changes. Mondrian-like colored patterns, generated on the screen of a computer monitor, were used as stimuli to avoid the spatial cues provided by natural objects and scenes. Spectral reflectances were taken from the Munsell Book of Color and from natural scenes. Illuminants were taken from the daylight locus. Results. Protanopes and deuteranopes performed more poorly than normal trichromats with Munsell spectral reflectances but were less impaired with natural spectral reflectances. Protanomalous and deuteranomalous trichromats performed as well as, or almost as well as, normal trichromats, independent of the type of reflectance. Individual differences were not correlated with Rayleigh anomaloscope matches. Conclusions. Despite the evidence of clinical color-vision tests, red-green color-deficient persons are less disadvantaged than might be expected in their judgments of surface colors under different lights.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Color/fisiología , Defectos de la Visión Cromática/fisiopatología , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Adulto , Pruebas de Percepción de Colores , Femenino , Humanos , Luz , Masculino , Células Fotorreceptoras Retinianas Conos/fisiología , Agudeza Visual/fisiología , Adulto Joven
15.
Percept Psychophys ; 70(2): 219-28, 2008 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18372745

RESUMEN

Naive observers viewed a sequence of colored Mondrian patterns, simulated on a color monitor. Each pattern was presented twice in succession, first under one daylight illuminant with a correlated color temperature of either 16,000 or 4000 K and then under the other, to test for color constancy. The observers compared the central square of the pattern across illuminants, either rating it for sameness of material appearance or sameness of hue and saturation or judging an objective property-that is, whether its change of color originated from a change in material or only from a change in illumination. Average color constancy indices were high for material appearance ratings and binary judgments of origin and low for hue-saturation ratings. Individuals' performance varied, but judgments of material and of hue and saturation remained demarcated. Observers seem able to separate phenomenal percepts from their ontological projections of mental appearance onto physical phenomena; thus, even when a chromatic change alters perceived hue and saturation, observers can reliably infer the cause, the constancy of the underlying surface spectral reflectance.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Color , Aprendizaje Discriminativo , Iluminación , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Atención , Humanos , Juicio , Psicofísica
16.
Vis Neurosci ; 23(3-4): 341-9, 2006.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16961965

RESUMEN

To what extent do observers' judgments of surface color with natural scenes depend on global image statistics? To address this question, a psychophysical experiment was performed in which images of natural scenes under two successive daylights were presented on a computer-controlled high-resolution color monitor. Observers reported whether there was a change in reflectance of a test surface in the scene. The scenes were obtained with a hyperspectral imaging system and included variously trees, shrubs, grasses, ferns, flowers, rocks, and buildings. Discrimination performance, quantified on a scale of 0 to 1 with a color-constancy index, varied from 0.69 to 0.97 over 21 scenes and two illuminant changes, from a correlated color temperature of 25,000 K to 6700 K and from 4000 K to 6700 K. The best account of these effects was provided by receptor-based rather than colorimetric properties of the images. Thus, in a linear regression, 43% of the variance in constancy index was explained by the log of the mean relative deviation in spatial cone-excitation ratios evaluated globally across the two images of a scene. A further 20% was explained by including the mean chroma of the first image and its difference from that of the second image and a further 7% by the mean difference in hue. Together, all four global color properties accounted for 70% of the variance and provided a good fit to the effects of scene and of illuminant change on color constancy, and, additionally, of changing test-surface position. By contrast, a spatial-frequency analysis of the images showed that the gradient of the luminance amplitude spectrum accounted for only 5% of the variance.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Color/fisiología , Color , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Naturaleza , Diseño Asistido por Computadora , Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Humanos , Luz , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Psicofísica/métodos , Percepción Espacial/fisiología
17.
Vis Neurosci ; 23(3-4): 351-6, 2006.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16961966

RESUMEN

Observers can generally make reliable judgments of surface color in natural scenes despite changes in an illuminant that is out of view. This ability has sometimes been attributed to observers' estimating the spectral properties of the illuminant in order to compensate for its effects. To test this hypothesis, two surface-color-matching experiments were performed with images of natural scenes obtained from high-resolution hyperspectral images. In the first experiment, the sky illuminating the scene was directly visible to the observer, and its color was manipulated. In the second experiment, a large gray sphere was introduced into the scene so that its illumination by the sun and sky was also directly visible to the observer, and the color of that illumination was manipulated. Although the degree of color constancy varied across this and other variations of the images, there was no reliable effect of illuminant color. Even when the sky was eliminated from view, color constancy did not worsen. Judging surface color in natural scenes seems to be independent of an explicit illuminant cue.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Color/fisiología , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Señales (Psicología) , Iluminación , Naturaleza , Análisis de Varianza , Color , Pruebas de Percepción de Colores/métodos , Humanos , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Psicofísica/métodos , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados
18.
J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis ; 23(10): 2359-72, 2006 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16985522

RESUMEN

Estimates of the frequency of metameric surfaces, which appear the same to the eye under one illuminant but different under another, were obtained from 50 hyperspectral images of natural scenes. The degree of metamerism was specified with respect to a color-difference measure after allowing for full chromatic adaptation. The relative frequency of metameric pairs of surfaces, expressed as a proportion of all pairs of surfaces in a scene, was very low. Depending on the criterion degree of metamerism, it ranged from about 10(-6) to 10(-4) for the largest illuminant change tested, which was from a daylight of correlated color temperature 25,000 K to one of 4000 K. But, given pairs of surfaces that were indistinguishable under one of these illuminants, the conditional relative frequency of metamerism was much higher, from about 10(-2) to 10(-1), sufficiently large to affect visual inferences about material identity.

19.
Vis Neurosci ; 23(3-4): 629-35, 2006.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16962006

RESUMEN

Deuteranomalous trichromacy, which affects medium-wavelength-sensitive cones, is more common than protanomalous trichromacy, which affects long-wavelength-sensitive cones. The aim of the present work was to test the extent to which these two kinds of anomalous trichromacy affect surface-color judgments in the natural world. Simulations of 18 natural scenes under different daylight illuminants were presented on a high-resolution color monitor to 7 deuteranomalous, 7 protanomalous, and 12 normal trichromatic observers, who had to discriminate between reflectance and illuminant changes in the images. Observers' ability to judge surface color was quantified by a standard color-constancy index. Deuteranomalous trichromats performed as well as normal trichromats, but protanomalous trichromats performed more poorly than both. The results are considered in relation to the spectral coverage of cones, rod intrusion, and the characterization of anomalous trichromacy by the Rayleigh match.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Color/fisiología , Defectos de la Visión Cromática/fisiopatología , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Juicio/fisiología , Luz , Naturaleza , Adolescente , Adulto , Color , Pruebas de Percepción de Colores/métodos , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Detección de Señal Psicológica/fisiología
20.
Vis Neurosci ; 23(3-4): 555-9, 2006.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16961995

RESUMEN

Simple color-difference formulae and pictorial images have traditionally been used to estimate the visual impact of color errors introduced by image-reproduction processes. But the limited gamut of RGB cameras constrains such analyses, particularly of natural scenes. The purpose of this work was to estimate visual sensitivity to color errors introduced deliberately into pictures synthesized from hyperspectral images of natural scenes without gamut constraints and to compare discrimination thresholds expressed in CIELAB and S-CIELAB color spaces. From each original image, a set of approximate images with variable color errors were generated and displayed on a calibrated RGB color monitor. The threshold for perceptibility of the errors was determined in a paired-comparison experiment. In agreement with previous studies, it was found that discrimination between original and approximate images needed on average a CIELAB color difference DeltaEab* of about 2.2. Although a large variation of performance across the nine images tested was found when errors were expressed in CIELAB units, little variation was obtained when they were expressed in S-CIELAB units.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Color/fisiología , Color , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Visión Ocular/fisiología , Pruebas de Percepción de Colores , Humanos , Aumento de la Imagen , Luz , Naturaleza , Psicofisiología/métodos , Sensibilidad y Especificidad , Umbral Sensorial/fisiología
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA