Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 35
Filtrar
Más filtros

Banco de datos
País/Región como asunto
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Exp Brain Res ; 240(7-8): 2039-2050, 2022 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35727366

RESUMEN

Peripersonal space (PPS), the space closely surrounding the body, is typically characterised by enhanced multisensory integration. Neurophysiological and behavioural studies have consistently shown stronger visuo-tactile integration when a visual stimulus is presented close to the tactually stimulate body part in near space (within PPS) than in far space. However, in the majority of these studies, tactile stimuli were delivered to the upper limbs, torso and face. Therefore, it is not known whether the space surrounding the lower limbs is characterised by similar multisensory properties. To address this question, we asked participants to complete two versions of the classic visuo-tactile crossmodal congruency task in which they had to perform speeded elevation judgements of tactile stimuli presented to the dorsum of the hand and foot while a simultaneous visual distractor was presented at spatially congruent or incongruent locations either in near or far space. In line with existing evidence, when the tactile target was presented to the hand, the size of the crossmodal congruency effect (CCE) decreased in far as compared to near space, suggesting stronger visuo-tactile multisensory integration within PPS. In contrast, when the tactile target was presented to the foot, the CCE decreased for visual distractors in near than far space. These findings show systematic differences between the representation of PPS around upper and lower limbs, suggesting that the multisensory properties of the different body part-centred representations of PPS are likely to depend on the potential actions performed by the different body parts.


Asunto(s)
Espacio Personal , Percepción del Tacto , Pie/fisiología , Humanos , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Tacto/fisiología , Percepción del Tacto/fisiología
2.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 30(8): 1098-1107, 2018 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29668393

RESUMEN

Changes in the perceived size of a body part using magnifying lenses influence tactile perception and pain. We investigated whether the visual magnification of one's hand also influences the motor system, as indexed by transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS)-induced motor evoked potentials (MEPs). In Experiment 1, MEPs were measured while participants gazed at their hand with and without magnification of the hand. MEPs were significantly larger when participants gazed at a magnified image of their hand. In Experiment 2, we demonstrated that this effect is specific to the hand that is visually magnified. TMS of the left motor cortex did not induce an increase of MEPs when participants looked at their magnified left hand. Experiment 3 was performed to determine if magnification altered the topography of the cortical representation of the hand. To that end, a 3 × 5 grid centered on the cortical hot spot (cortical location at which a motor threshold is obtained with the lowest level of stimulation) was overlaid on the participant's MRI image, and all 15 sites in the grid were stimulated with and without magnification of the hand. We confirmed the increase in the MEPs at the hot spot with magnification and demonstrated that MEPs significantly increased with magnification at sites up to 16.5 mm from the cortical hot spot. In Experiment 4, we used paired-pulse TMS to measure short-interval intracortical inhibition and intracortical facilitation. Magnification was associated with an increase in short-interval intracortical inhibition. These experiments demonstrate that the visual magnification of one's hand induces changes in motor cortex excitability and generates a rapid remapping of the cortical representation of the hand that may, at least in part, be mediated by changes in short-interval intracortical inhibition.


Asunto(s)
Excitabilidad Cortical , Mano/fisiología , Corteza Motora/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Imagen Corporal , Potenciales Evocados Motores , Femenino , Mano/inervación , Humanos , Masculino , Plasticidad Neuronal , Estimulación Magnética Transcraneal , Adulto Joven
3.
Brain Cogn ; 123: 103-109, 2018 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29550505

RESUMEN

Impulsivity, conceptualized as impulsive personality trait, poor inhibitory control and enhanced reward sensitivity, has been strongly linked to obesity. In particular, a disequilibrium between cognitive control and reward sensitivity has been observed in obese individuals in both behavioural and imaging studies. While this issue has been widely investigated in children and adults, it has received little attention in older adults. Here, obese and non-obese participants aged between 40 and 70 years completed the Barratt Impulsiveness scale (assessing motor, non-planning and attentional impulsiveness), a Go/no-go task with foods and non-foods (assessing inhibitory control) and a reward sensitivity battery with high and low caloric foods (assessing liking, wanting, tastiness and frequency of consumption). We observed that participants with higher BMI reported increased wanting for high calorie foods, but did not show poorer inhibitory control. Interestingly, participants who scored lower on the MMSE reported to consume high calorie more than low calorie foods. Finally, those who presented low scores on non-planning and motor impulsiveness subscales reported higher tastiness ratings for low calorie foods. These results show that increased reward sensitivity but not reduced inhibitory control may characterize higher BMI during aging. Importantly, they also highlight new findings concerning food preferences among older adults.


Asunto(s)
Peso Corporal/fisiología , Conducta Impulsiva/fisiología , Obesidad/psicología , Recompensa , Adulto , Anciano , Atención/fisiología , Femenino , Preferencias Alimentarias/psicología , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas
4.
Brain Cogn ; 110: 74-84, 2016 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26786416

RESUMEN

Food is so central to humans' life that keeping our mind away from it is not an easy task. Because of its strong motivational value, food cues attract our attention. However, often food is truly not relevant to our on-going activities. In the present study we investigated the distracting role that task-irrelevant foods (natural and manufactured) and food-cues play in performing goal-directed reaching movements. We explored whether spatial and temporal parameters of reaching movement were influenced by the presence of task-irrelevant stimuli (i.e., distractor effect), and whether this effect was modulated by participants' implicit and explicit ratings of food items and participants' tendency to restrain their diet. First we found that the movement trajectory veered consistently toward food items and food-related distractors. Second, we found that participants' own evaluation of natural and manufactured food played a differential predicting role of the magnitude of temporal and spatial parameters of the distractor effect induced by these types of food. We conclude that perceptual and attentional systems provide preferential access to stimuli in the environment with high significance for organisms.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Conducta Alimentaria/fisiología , Preferencias Alimentarias/fisiología , Alimentos , Actividad Motora/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
5.
Brain Cogn ; 95: 77-89, 2015 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25725189

RESUMEN

Normally we can perform a variety of goal-directed movements effortlessly. However, damage to the parietal cortex may dramatically reduce this ability, giving rise to optic ataxia and limb apraxia. Patients with optic ataxia show clear misreaches towards targets when presented in the peripheral visual field, whereas limb apraxia refers to the inability to use common tools or to imitate simple gestures. In the present paper we describe the case of a left-brain damaged patient, who presented both symptoms. We systematically investigated both spatial and temporal parameters of his movements, when asked to reach and grasp common objects to move (Experiment 1) or to use them (Experiment 2), presented either in the central or peripheral visual field. Different movement parameters changed in relation to the goal of the task (grasp to move vs. grasp to use), reflecting a normal modulation of the movement to accomplish tasks with different goals. On the other hand, grip aperture appeared to be more affected from both task goal and viewing condition, with a specific decrement observed when CF was asked to use objects presented peripherally. On the contrary, a neat effect of the viewing condition was observed in the spatial distribution of the end-points of the movements, and of the horizontal end point in particular, which were shifted towards the fixation point when reaching towards peripheral targets. We hypothesized that optic ataxia and limb apraxia have a differential effect on the patient's performance. The specific presence of optic ataxia would have an effect on the movement trajectory, but both symptoms might interact and influence the grasping component of the movement. As a 'cognitive side of motor control impairment', the presence of limb apraxia may have increased the task demands in grasping to use the objects thus exacerbating optic ataxia.


Asunto(s)
Apraxias/psicología , Ataxia/psicología , Desempeño Psicomotor , Visión Ocular , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Apraxias/patología , Ataxia/patología , Encéfalo/patología , Objetivos , Fuerza de la Mano , Humanos , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas
6.
Cortex ; 173: 138-149, 2024 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38394974

RESUMEN

Although behavioral evidence has shown that postural changes influence the ability to localize or detect tactile stimuli, little is known regarding the brain areas that modulate these effects. This 7T functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study explores the effects of touch of the hand as a function of hand location (right or left side of the body) and hand configuration (open or closed). We predicted that changes in hand configuration would be represented in contralateral primary somatosensory cortex (S1) and the anterior intraparietal area (aIPS), whereas change in position of the hand would be associated with alterations in activation in the superior parietal lobule. Multivoxel pattern analysis and a region of interest approach partially supported our predictions. Decoding accuracy for hand location was above chance level in superior parietal lobule (SPL) and in the anterior intraparietal (aIPS) area; above chance classification of hand configuration was observed in SPL and S1. This evidence confirmed the role of the parietal cortex in postural effects on touch and the possible role of S1 in coding the body form representation of the hand.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico , Lóbulo Parietal , Humanos , Mapeo Encefálico/métodos , Lóbulo Parietal/diagnóstico por imagen , Lóbulo Parietal/fisiología , Postura , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética/métodos , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Mano , Corteza Somatosensorial/diagnóstico por imagen , Corteza Somatosensorial/fisiología
7.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 49(6): 877-892, 2023 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37276125

RESUMEN

Using a mirror box, the concurrent stroking of the lateral side of the fifth finger behind the mirror along with stroking the empty space next to the mirror-reflected hand's fifth finger results in a strong sense of having a sixth finger-the Anne Boleyn illusion. We used this illusion to understand what constraints illusory embodiment. In Experiment 1, we manipulated the anatomical constraints, posture, and stroking of the sixth finger, along with other variants. Given evidence from other body illusions, we predicted no illusory embodiment in conditions in which the sixth finger was created in a manner incompatible with a typical hand, when the mirror and viewed hands were in different posture, and when stroking differed. Surprisingly, the illusion was persistent in most variants, including those with curved fingers, elongated fingers, and even with mismatches between the posture of the viewed and hidden hand. In Experiment 2, we manipulated the orientation, shape, and length of the illusory sixth finger, presenting more extreme versions of the illusion. The illusion was significantly diminished only when the sixth finger was far from the hand, or in a very implausible posture. This evidence suggests that body representations are extremely flexible and allow for embodiment of empty space in conditions not seen in other body illusions. We suggest that bottom-up information from concurrent visuotactile input, combined with reduced constraints provided by the "blank canvas" of empty space, results in a particularly robust illusion. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Ilusiones , Percepción del Tacto , Humanos , Propiocepción , Postura , Mano , Dedos , Imagen Corporal , Percepción Visual
8.
Sci Rep ; 13(1): 3082, 2023 Feb 22.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36813847

RESUMEN

Magnified vision of one's body part has been shown to improve tactile discrimination. We used an anesthetic cream (AC) to determine if somesthetic stimulation that alters the perception of the size of one's body would also improve two point-discrimination (2PD). In Experiment 1, application of AC caused an increase in perceived lip size and an improvement in a 2PD. As perceived lip size increased, subjects became more accurate in identifying that they had been touched in two locations. Experiment 2 confirmed this effect in a larger sample and introduced a control condition (no AC) that demonstrated that the change in performance was not attributable to practice or familiarity with the task. In Experiment 3, we showed that both AC and moisturizing cream improved subjects' ability to indicate that they had been touched in 2 locations, but the improvement was modulated by perceived lip size only for AC. These results support the idea that changes in the body representation influence 2PD.

9.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc ; 18(2): 269-76, 2012 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22172148

RESUMEN

This study explored Closing-in behavior (CIB), the tendency in figure copying to draw very close to or on top of the model, in mild cognitive impairment (MCI). The files of 154 people diagnosed with MCI were reviewed and CIB was identified in 21% of cases. Two approaches were used to explore CIB. First, we capitalized on the diverse cognitive profiles within MCI, subdividing the overall sample into people with and without memory deficits. The frequency of CIB was significantly higher in multidomain non-amnestic MCI than in multidomain amnestic MCI, suggesting that CIB is not associated with specific memory impairment. Second, we assessed the cognitive correlates of CIB, by selecting patients with MCI who completed a battery of executive, visuo-constructional and memory tasks. Sub-groups of patients with and without CIB showed a similar overall severity of cognitive decline and comparable performance in visuo-constructional and memory tasks, but those with CIB were slightly but significantly more impaired on executive function tasks. The study provides evidence against memory-based accounts of CIB, and supports recent suggestions that executive impairments are the dominant cognitive correlate of this clinical sign.


Asunto(s)
Disfunción Cognitiva/diagnóstico , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Disfunción Cognitiva/complicaciones , Femenino , Humanos , Italia , Masculino , Trastornos de la Memoria/diagnóstico , Trastornos de la Memoria/etiología , Persona de Mediana Edad , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Estimulación Luminosa , Escalas de Valoración Psiquiátrica , Estadísticas no Paramétricas
10.
Neuropsychologia ; 166: 108137, 2022 02 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34953796

RESUMEN

How does the brain localize touch under conditions of uncertainty caused by brain damage? By testing single cases, previous work found mislocalization of touch toward the center of the hand. We investigated whether such central bias changes as a function of uncertainty in somatosensory system. Fifty-one brain-damaged individuals were presented with a tactile detection task to establish their tactile threshold, and a tactile localization task in which they localized suprathreshold stimuli presented at different locations on the hand. We predicted that with increased somatosensory uncertainty, indexed by higher detection thresholds, participants would more likely to localize the stimuli toward the center of the hand. Consistent with this prediction, participants' localization errors were biased towards the center of the hand and, importantly, this bias increased as detection threshold increased. These findings provide evidence that instead of showing random errors, uncertainty leads to systematic localization errors toward the center of the hand or the center of the stimulus distribution, which overlapped in the present study. We discuss these findings under different frameworks as potential mechanisms to explain biases in tactile localization subsequent to brain damage.


Asunto(s)
Percepción del Tacto , Sesgo , Encéfalo , Mano , Humanos , Estimulación Física , Tacto
11.
Front Psychol ; 13: 934573, 2022.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35911043

RESUMEN

Recent evidence has identified the N140cc lateralized component of event-related potentials as a reliable index of the deployment of attention to task-relevant items in touch. However, existing ERP studies have presented the tactile search array to participants' limbs, most often to the hands. Here, we investigated distractor interference effects when the tactile search array was presented to a portion of the body that is less lateralized and peripheral compared to the hands. Participants were asked to localize a tactile target presented among distractors in a circular arrangement to their back. The N140cc was elicited contralateral to the target when the singleton distractor was absent. Its amplitude was reduced when the singleton distractor was present and contralateral to the target, suggesting that attention was directed at least in part to the distractor when the singletons are on opposite sides. However, similar N140cc were observed when the singleton distractor was ipsilateral to the target compared to distractor absent trials. We suggest that when target and singleton distractor are ipsilateral, the exact localization of the target requires the attentional processing of all items on the same side of the array, similar to distractor absent trials. Together, these observations replicate the distractor interference effects previously observed for the hands, suggesting that analogous mechanisms guide attentional selectivity across different body parts.

12.
Biol Psychol ; 162: 108098, 2021 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33901576

RESUMEN

Evidence suggests that the N140cc component of event-related potentials (ERP) observed in tactile search tasks reflects the attentional selection of the target. Here, we investigated whether the target selection processes are affected by the separation between the target and an ipsilateral singleton distractor (singletons delivered to contiguous or non-contiguous fingers of the same hand). In addition, the external distance between search items was varied through posture (splayed or touching fingers). Accuracy improved when target and distractor were delivered to contiguous fingers that were also touching. Regardless of target-distractor separation, the N140cc was larger when the external distance between search-array stimuli decreased (touching fingers). Importantly, a smaller N140cc was observed at reduced target-distractor separations, suggesting a narrower attentional focus for contiguous singletons. These findings reveal that the mechanisms responsible for tactile target selection in the presence of an ipsilateral singleton distractor are fundamentally different from those emerged in vision.


Asunto(s)
Electroencefalografía , Tacto , Atención , Potenciales Evocados , Mano , Humanos , Tiempo de Reacción
13.
Neurorehabil Neural Repair ; 35(12): 1100-1111, 2021 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34704486

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Phantom limb pain (PLP) is a common and in some cases debilitating consequence of upper- or lower-limb amputation for which current treatments are inadequate. OBJECTIVE: This small clinical trial tested whether game-like interactions with immersive VR activities can reduce PLP in subjects with transtibial lower-limb amputation. METHODS: Seven participants attended 5-7 sessions in which they engaged in a visually immersive virtual reality experience that did not require leg movements (Cool! TM), followed by 10-12 sessions of targeted lower-limb VR treatment consisting of custom games requiring leg movement. In the latter condition, they controlled an avatar with 2 intact legs viewed in a head-mounted display (HTC Vive TM). A motion-tracking system mounted on the intact and residual limbs controlled the movements of both virtual extremities independently. RESULTS: All participants except one experienced a reduction of pain immediately after VR sessions, and their pre session pain levels also decreased over the course of the study. At a group level, PLP decreased by 28% after the treatment that did not include leg movements and 39.6% after the games requiring leg motions. Both treatments were successful in reducing PLP. CONCLUSIONS: This VR intervention appears to be an efficacious treatment for PLP in subjects with lower-limb amputation.


Asunto(s)
Videojuego de Ejercicio , Extremidad Inferior/fisiopatología , Rehabilitación Neurológica , Miembro Fantasma/rehabilitación , Terapia Asistida por Computador , Realidad Virtual , Adulto , Anciano , Amputación Quirúrgica , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Evaluación de Resultado en la Atención de Salud
14.
Cogn Process ; 11(3): 207-11, 2010 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19526260

RESUMEN

Many pre-school children show closing-in behaviour (CIB) in graphic copying tasks: a tendency to place their copy abnormally close to or even on top of the model. Similar phenomena have been studied in patients with dementia, though it is unclear whether the superficial similarities between CIB in development and dementia reflect common underlying cognitive mechanisms. The aim of the present study was to investigate the cognitive functions involved in CIB in pre-school children. Forty-one children (3-5 years) were assessed for CIB, and completed a neuropsychological battery targeting visuospatial abilities, short term memory (verbal and spatial) and attention (sustained attention, selective attention and attention switching). Binary logistic regression found that performance on the attention subtests was the best unique predictor of CIB. A second analysis, in which the three attention subtests were entered as separate predictors, suggested that attention switching ability was most strongly related to CIB. These results support the view that CIB in children reflects inadequate attentional control. The convergence of these results with similar observations in patients with dementia further suggests that similar cognitive factors underlie CIB in these two populations.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Conducta Infantil/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Modelos Logísticos , Masculino , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Estimulación Luminosa
15.
Sci Rep ; 10(1): 9515, 2020 06 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32528087

RESUMEN

The rubber hand illusion (RHI) demonstrates that under some circumstances a fake hand can be regarded as part of one's body; the RHI and related phenomena have been used to explore the flexibility of the body schema. Recent work has shown that a sense of embodiment may be generated by virtual reality (VR). In a series of experiments, we used VR to assess the effects of the displacement of the virtual image of subjects' hands on action. Specifically, we tested whether spatial and temporal parameters of action change when participants perform a reaching movement towards the location of their virtual hand, the position of which was distorted on some trials. In different experiments, participants were sometimes provided with incorrect visual feedback regarding the position of the to-be-touched hand (Experiment 1), were deprived of visual feedback regarding the position of the reaching hand when acting (Experiment 2) or reached with the hand, the apparent position of which had been manipulated (Experiment 3). The effect was greatest when participants reached towards (Experiment 1) or with (Experiment 3) the displaced hand when the hand was visible during the reaching, but not when the vision of the hand was removed during the action (Experiment 2). Taken together, these data suggest that visual images of one's hand presented in VR influence the body schema and action performance.


Asunto(s)
Mano , Ilusiones/psicología , Realidad Virtual , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
16.
Cortex ; 45(3): 278-84, 2009 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18760777

RESUMEN

Many pre-school children show 'Closing-in behaviour' (CIB) in graphic copying, placing their copy excessively close to, or even on top of the original. This behaviour can also be observed in patients with dementia, though it is unclear whether the superficial similarities between CIB in development and dementia reflect common underlying mechanisms. Two main hypotheses have been proposed to account for CIB: the compensation hypothesis considers CIB as a strategic adaptation to underlying deficits in visuospatial and/or memory functions; the attraction hypothesis proposes that CIB is a primitive default behaviour in which the acting hand is drawn towards the focus of visual attention. The present study tested between these hypotheses in a group of 15 pre-school children. The children performed a simple straight-line drawing task whilst naming line drawings of animals printed at the top or bottom of the sheet. The drawn lines veered reliably towards the named animals, mimicking CIB in copying tasks. This pattern is not predicted by the compensation hypothesis, but is consistent with the attraction account. We suggest that this default attraction may emerge in children with insufficiently developed attentional and/or executive control.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Imitativa/fisiología , Orientación/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Niño , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas
17.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc ; 15(5): 787-92, 2009 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19570308

RESUMEN

The frequency and characteristics of closing-in behavior (CIB) were examined in 797 patients with Alzheimer's disease (AD), 132 of whom were followed up longitudinally. The frequency of CIB increased with the complexity of the graphic copying task and with the severity of Alzheimer's disease. Regression analyses suggested that attentional deficits are critical factors for the appearance of CIB, but that visuospatial impairments also play an important role in the emergence of severe forms of CIB.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedad de Alzheimer/complicaciones , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/psicología , Conducta Imitativa/fisiología , Trastornos de la Percepción/diagnóstico , Trastornos de la Percepción/etiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Estudios de Cohortes , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Escalas de Valoración Psiquiátrica , Estudios Retrospectivos
18.
PeerJ ; 7: e7114, 2019.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31392085

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Increasing perceived hand size with magnifying lenses improves tactile discrimination and induces changes in action performance. We previously demonstrated that motor skills (tested with grip force, finger tapping, and a reach to grasp tasks) improved when actions were performed with magnified compared to normal vision; twenty-eight percent of 25 participants with stroke exhibited significant improvement on a composite measure of motor performance with magnification as compared to a session without magnification. METHODS: To investigate the potential implications of magnification of vision for motor rehabilitation, we recruited individuals with stroke from the original cohort who exhibited an improvement of at least 10% in grip force and/or finger tapping for a home training protocol. Six individuals with stroke completed a two-week home-based training program in which they performed a range of activities while looking at their hand magnified. Motor skills were measured before, immediately after, and two weeks after the training. RESULTS: Five of the six participants showed an improvement on motor tasks when tested after the training. In two participants the improvement was evident immediately after the training and persisted in time, while it occurred at two-weeks post-training in the other participants. These results suggest that the magnification of vision is a potential tool for the rehabilitation of post-stroke motor deficits.

19.
Arch Clin Neuropsychol ; 34(2): 214-221, 2019 Mar 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29688299

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: This study investigates closing-in behavior (CIB), a phenomenon observed in graphic copying tasks when responses encroach upon or overlap the model. The behavior is most common amongst individuals with dementia and amongst pre-school children. We explored the relationship between CIB and the 'distractor effect' in reaching, whereby salient visual stimuli can influence the spatial trajectory of the reach. METHOD: A group of individuals with overlap-CIB (n = 9), without CIB (n = 9) and healthy controls (HC; n = 6) underwent a task-irrelevant and a task-relevant distractors and the deviation of the movement trajectory towards the distractor location was measured in both tasks. RESULTS: Individuals with graphic CIB showed more distractor-directed veering during reaching than did individuals without CIB or HC, provided that the distractor was relevant for the reaching task. CONCLUSIONS: These results strengthen the relationship between CIB and the distractor effect and reinforce the hypothesis that CIB represents a disinhibited tendency to act towards the focus of attention.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Lesiones Encefálicas/psicología , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Adulto , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Estimulación Luminosa , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología
20.
PLoS One ; 14(6): e0206270, 2019.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31185013

RESUMEN

Bodily states are fundamental to emotions' emergence and are believed to constitute the first step in the chain of events that culminate in emotional awareness. Recent works have shown that distinct topographical maps can be derived to describe how basic and more complex emotions are represented in the body. However, it is still unclear whether these bodily maps can also extend to emotions experienced specifically within social interactions and how these representations relate to basic emotions. To address this issue, we used the emBODY tool to obtain high-resolution bodily maps that describe the body activation and deactivation experienced by healthy participants when presented with social scenarios depicting establishment or loss of social bonds. We observed patterns of activation/deactivation for each single social scenario depending on the valence, but also a common activation of head, chest and deactivation of limbs for positive and negative social scenarios, respectively. Furthermore, we show that these maps are comparable to those obtained when taking the perspective of a third person, suggesting the existence of common body representation of social emotions for the self and other person evaluation. Finally, we showed that maps related to complex social scenarios are strongly correlated with bodily states experienced in basic emotions, suggesting that the patterns of body activation/deactivation observed for social scenarios might arise from a complex interaction of the basic emotions that these experiences elicit.


Asunto(s)
Imagen Corporal/psicología , Emociones/fisiología , Relaciones Interpersonales , Sensación/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Voluntarios Sanos , Humanos , Masculino , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Adulto Joven
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA