Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 24
Filtrar
1.
Mem Cognit ; 46(7): 1058-1075, 2018 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29796864

RESUMEN

Dual-process models of episodic retrieval reveal consistent deficits of controlled recollection in aging and Alzheimer disease (AD). In contrast, automatic familiarity is relatively spared. We extend standard dual-process models by showing the importance of a third capture process. Capture produces a failure to attempt recollection, which might reflect a distinct error from an inability to recollect when attempted (Jacoby et al. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134(2), 131-148, 2005a). We used multinomial process tree (MPT) modeling to estimate controlled recollection and capture processes, as well as automatic retrieval processes, in a large group of middle-aged to older adults who were cognitively normal (N = 519) or diagnosed with the earliest detectable stage of AD (N = 107). Participants incidentally encoded word pairs (e.g., knee bone). At retrieval, participants completed cued word fragments (e.g., knee b_n_) with primes that were congruent (e.g., bone), incongruent (e.g., bend), or neutral (i.e., &&&) to the target (e.g., bone). MPT models estimated retrieval processes both at the group and the individual levels. A capture parameter was necessary to fit MPT models to the observed data, suggesting that dual-process models of this task can be contaminated by a capture process. In both group- and individual-level analyses, aging and very mild AD were associated with increased susceptibility to capture, decreased recollection, and no differences in automatic influences. These results suggest that it is important to consider two distinct modes of attentional control when modeling retrieval processes. Both forms of control (recollection and avoiding capture) are particularly sensitive to cognitive decline in aging and early-stage AD.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/fisiología , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/fisiopatología , Disfunción Cognitiva/fisiopatología , Memoria Episódica , Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Modelos Psicológicos , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Modelos Estadísticos
2.
Memory ; 25(9): 1303-1308, 2017 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28276976

RESUMEN

Studies of childhood memory typically show that our earliest memories come from between three and four years of age. This finding is not universal, however. The age estimate varies across cultures and is affected by social influences. Research from the judgments and decision-making literature suggests that these estimates might also involve a judgment under uncertainty. Therefore, they might be susceptible to less social influences such as heuristics and biases. To investigate this possibility, we conducted two experiments that used anchoring paradigms to influence participants' estimates of their age during early autobiographical memories. In Experiment 1, participants answered either a high-anchor or a low-anchor question, and were warned that the anchor was uninformative; they went on to estimate their age during their earliest autobiographical memory. In Experiment 2, we replicated Experiment 1 and extended the design to examine additional early autobiographical memories. In both experiments, participants in the low-anchor condition gave earlier age estimates than those in the high-anchor condition. These results provide new insights into the methods used to investigate autobiographical memory. Moreover, they show that reports of early autobiographical memories can be influenced by a relatively light touch - a change to a single digit in a single question.


Asunto(s)
Memoria Episódica , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Juicio , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Adulto Joven
3.
Behav Res Methods ; 49(1): 294-309, 2017 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26822671

RESUMEN

With nonnormal data, the typical confidence interval of the correlation (Fisher z') may be inaccurate. The literature has been unclear as to which of several alternative methods should be used instead, and how extreme a violation of normality is needed to justify an alternative. Through Monte Carlo simulation, 11 confidence interval methods were compared, including Fisher z', two Spearman rank-order methods, the Box-Cox transformation, rank-based inverse normal (RIN) transformation, and various bootstrap methods. Nonnormality often distorted the Fisher z' confidence interval-for example, leading to a 95 % confidence interval that had actual coverage as low as 68 %. Increasing the sample size sometimes worsened this problem. Inaccurate Fisher z' intervals could be predicted by a sample kurtosis of at least 2, an absolute sample skewness of at least 1, or significant violations of normality hypothesis tests. Only the Spearman rank-order and RIN transformation methods were universally robust to nonnormality. Among the bootstrap methods, an observed imposed bootstrap came closest to accurate coverage, though it often resulted in an overly long interval. The results suggest that sample nonnormality can justify avoidance of the Fisher z' interval in favor of a more robust alternative. R code for the relevant methods is provided in supplementary materials.


Asunto(s)
Intervalos de Confianza , Método de Montecarlo , Distribución Normal , Tamaño de la Muestra , Estadística como Asunto/métodos
4.
Memory ; 23(7): 1013-28, 2015.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25122187

RESUMEN

Previous research has shown that multiple choice tests often improve memory retention. However, the presence of incorrect lures often attenuates this memory benefit. The current research examined the effects of "all of the above" (AOTA) options. When such options are correct, no incorrect lures are present. In the first three experiments, a correct AOTA option on an initial test led to a larger memory benefit than no test and standard multiple choice test conditions. The benefits of a correct AOTA option occurred even without feedback on the initial test; for both 5-minute and 48-hour retention delays; and for both cued recall and multiple choice final test formats. In the final experiment, an AOTA question led to better memory retention than did a control condition that had identical timing and exposure to response options. However, the benefits relative to this control condition were similar regardless of the type of multiple choice test (AOTA or not). Results suggest that retrieval contributes to multiple choice testing effects. However, the extra testing effect from a correct AOTA option, rather than being due to more retrieval, might be due simply to more exposure to correct information.


Asunto(s)
Conducta de Elección , Evaluación Educacional , Retención en Psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Educacionales , Solución de Problemas , Tiempo de Reacción , Adulto Joven
5.
Mem Cognit ; 41(6): 928-37, 2013 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23508339

RESUMEN

Some learning schedules can foster an illusion of competence, whereby the learner feels that the skill will be retained better than it actually will be. Consider fixed-order practice, in which a person practices a task repeatedly before switching to the next task (e.g., task order A, A, B, B); in contrast, in random-order practice, a person randomly alternates among two or more tasks (e.g., task order C, D, D, C). In the present experiment, participants (n = 25) who had formal training in piano practiced melodies under fixed- or random-order conditions (within-subjects), and then returned for a retention test 2 days later. Initially, the participants performed faster on melodies practiced in a fixed-order. However, on a retention test 2 days later, participants were faster with melodies from the random-order condition. Despite the within-subjects design, which facilitated the comparison of practice conditions, participants' metacognitive judgments indicated an illusion of competence, whereby they erroneously believed that fixed-order practice would result in faster retention performance. Our results suggest that even some trained musicians may use ease of acquisition as a heuristic for predicting future performance.


Asunto(s)
Ilusiones/fisiología , Destreza Motora/fisiología , Música/psicología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Retención en Psicología/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Cognición/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Juicio/fisiología , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Autoimagen , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas , Factores de Tiempo , Adulto Joven
6.
J Affect Disord ; 330: 319-328, 2023 06 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36889442

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: From a behavioural perspective anhedonia is defined as diminished interest in the engagement of pleasurable activities. Despite its presence across a range of psychiatric disorders, the cognitive processes that give rise to anhedonia remain unclear. METHODS: Here we examine whether anhedonia is associated with learning from positive and negative outcomes in patients diagnosed with major depression, schizophrenia and opiate use disorder alongside a healthy control group. Responses in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test - a task associated with healthy prefrontal cortex function - were fitted to the Attentional Learning Model (ALM) which separates learning from positive and negative feedback. RESULTS: Learning from punishment, but not from reward, was negatively associated with anhedonia beyond other socio-demographic, cognitive and clinical variables. This impairment in punishment sensitivity was also associated with faster responses following negative feedback, independently of the degree of surprise. LIMITATIONS: Future studies should test the longitudinal association between punishment sensitivity and anhedonia also in other clinical populations controlling for the effect of specific medications. CONCLUSIONS: Together the results reveal that anhedonic subjects, because of their negative expectations, are less sensitive to negative feedbacks; this might lead them to persist in actions leading to negative outcomes.


Asunto(s)
Alcaloides Opiáceos , Esquizofrenia , Humanos , Castigo/psicología , Anhedonia/fisiología , Depresión , Recompensa
7.
Pers Individ Dif ; 50(4): 492-495, 2011 Apr 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21359157

RESUMEN

The Iowa Gambling Task (IGT) is sensitive to decision making impairments in several clinical groups with frontal impairment. However the complexity of the IGT, particularly in terms of its learning requirements, makes it difficult to know whether disadvantageous (risky) selections in this task reflect deliberate risk taking or a failure to recognise risk. To determine whether propensity for risk taking contributes to IGT performance, we correlated IGT selections with a measure of propensity for risk taking from the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART), taking into account potential moderating effects of IGT learning requirements, and trait impulsivity, which is associated with learning difficulties. We found that IGT and BART performance were related, but only in the later stages of the IGT, and only in participants with low trait impulsivity. This finding suggests that IGT performance may reflect different underlying processes in individuals with low and high trait impulsivity. In individuals with low trait impulsivity, it appears that risky selections in the IGT reflect in part, propensity for risk seeking, but only after the development of explicit knowledge of IGT risks after a period of learning.

8.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 28(4): 1164-1182, 2021 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33660213

RESUMEN

Researchers sometimes use informal judgment for statistical model diagnostics and assumption checking. Informal judgment might seem more desirable than formal judgment because of a paradox: Formal hypothesis tests of assumptions appear to become less useful as sample size increases. We suggest that this paradox can be resolved by evaluating both formal and informal statistical judgment via a simplified signal detection framework. In 4 studies, we used this approach to compare informal judgments of normality diagnostic graphs (histograms, Q-Q plots, and P-P plots) to the performance of several formal tests (Shapiro-Wilk test, Kolmogorov-Smirnov test, etc.). Participants judged whether or not graphs of sample data came from a normal population (Experiments 1-2) or whether or not from a population close enough to normal for a parametric test to be more powerful than a nonparametric one (Experiments 3-4). Across all experiments, participants' informal judgments showed lower discriminability than did formal hypothesis tests. This pattern occurred even after participants were given 400 training trials with feedback, a financial incentive, and ecologically valid distribution shapes. The discriminability advantage of formal normality tests led to slightly more powerful follow-up tests (parametric vs. nonparametric). Overall, the framework used here suggests that formal model diagnostics may be more desirable than informal ones.


Asunto(s)
Juicio , Modelos Estadísticos , Humanos , Distribución Normal , Tamaño de la Muestra
9.
Psychol Sci ; 19(10): 1015-22, 2008 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19000212

RESUMEN

A recent study demonstrated that individuals making experience-based choices underweight small probabilities, in contrast to the overweighting observed in a typical descriptive paradigm. We tested whether trial-by-trial feedback in a repeated descriptive paradigm would engender choices more correspondent with experiential or descriptive paradigms. The results of a repeated gambling task indicated that individuals receiving feedback underweighted small probabilities, relative to their no-feedback counterparts. These results implicate feedback as a critical component during the decision-making process, even in the presence of fully specified descriptive information. A model comparison at the individual-subject level suggested that feedback drove individuals' decision weights toward objective probability weighting.


Asunto(s)
Conducta de Elección , Toma de Decisiones , Retroalimentación Psicológica , Juego de Azar/psicología , Aprendizaje por Probabilidad , Atención , Cultura , Humanos , Motivación , Aprendizaje Basado en Problemas , Asunción de Riesgos
10.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 15(1): 52-7, 2008 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18605479

RESUMEN

Spaced retrieval is a memory-training technique whereby information is tested at progressively longer delays. Two experiments were conducted in order to examine the effects of spaced retrieval on controlled recollection and automatic influences of memory. In Experiment 1, word pairs were read once, three times, or once and retrieved twice by young and older adults. Retrieval practice improved performance on a later test for both age groups. Experiment 2 was arranged so that recollection opposed automatic influences of retrieval practice. Retrieval practice increased intrusions on a later test only for older adults. Results suggest that because of a deficit in recollection, older adults were less able to oppose the automatic influence of spaced retrieval and thus exhibited less flexible memory performance.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/psicología , Atención , Recuerdo Mental , Aprendizaje por Asociación de Pares , Práctica Psicológica , Retención en Psicología , Adolescente , Adulto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Semántica
11.
Br J Math Stat Psychol ; 71(1): 167-185, 2018 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28872186

RESUMEN

When bivariate normality is violated, the default confidence interval of the Pearson correlation can be inaccurate. Two new methods were developed based on the asymptotic sampling distribution of Fisher's z' under the general case where bivariate normality need not be assumed. In Monte Carlo simulations, the most successful of these methods relied on the (Vale & Maurelli, 1983, Psychometrika, 48, 465) family to approximate a distribution via the marginal skewness and kurtosis of the sample data. In Simulation 1, this method provided more accurate confidence intervals of the correlation in non-normal data, at least as compared to no adjustment of the Fisher z' interval, or to adjustment via the sample joint moments. In Simulation 2, this approximate distribution method performed favourably relative to common non-parametric bootstrap methods, but its performance was mixed relative to an observed imposed bootstrap and two other robust methods (PM1 and HC4). No method was completely satisfactory. An advantage of the approximate distribution method, though, is that it can be implemented even without access to raw data if sample skewness and kurtosis are reported, making the method particularly useful for meta-analysis. Supporting information includes R code.


Asunto(s)
Intervalos de Confianza , Interpretación Estadística de Datos , Psicometría/métodos , Algoritmos , Simulación por Computador , Humanos , Método de Montecarlo , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados
12.
J Exp Psychol Gen ; 136(2): 200-16, 2007 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17500646

RESUMEN

Probabilistic retroactive interference (RI) refers to the interfering effects of intermixing presentations of an earlier studied response (A-B) with presentations of a competing response (A-D). As an example, for a 2/3 condition, a cue word was presented with its earlier studied response twice and its competing response once during the interference phase. Performance on direct and indirect tests of memory for earlier studied responses was combined to reveal dissociations between effects on recollection and accessibility bias. Manipulating probabilistic RI influenced accessibility bias but left recollection unchanged. Effects of probabilistic RI were compared with effects of traditional, nonprobabilistic RI. The authors contrast their dual-process model with traditional accounts of RI and discuss the importance of distinguishing between recollection and accessibility bias for understanding interference effects.


Asunto(s)
Recuerdo Mental , Modelos Estadísticos , Psicología/métodos , Psicología/estadística & datos numéricos , Vocabulario , Sesgo , Señales (Psicología) , Humanos , Modelos Psicológicos
13.
Brain ; 129(Pt 1): 128-40, 2006 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16280354

RESUMEN

Exposure to misleading information, presented after a critical episode, can alter or impair memory reports about that episode. Here, we examine vulnerability to misleading information in patients with traumatic brain injury (TBI). The ability to initiate an effective retrieval strategy and inhibit irrelevant or interfering information requires participation from the prefrontal cortices, which are susceptible to damage following brain injury. We report that TBI patients are more prone to interference effects produced by misleading information during a cued-recall task and are more likely to accept this information as the product of 'remembering' compared with healthy controls. The results are consistent with a model proposing that patients are captured by highly accessible responses eliminating their opportunity to engage in recollection. Correlations between the cued-recall interference task and other executive measures helped elucidate the processes underlying 'capture'. In TBI patients, reduced recollection produced by a misleading prime was associated with impaired prospective remembering when engaged in a background task. A common functional deficit that may underlie poor performance on both tasks is the failure to inhibit previously relevant but currently irrelevant information. Subjective reports pertaining to the subject's cued-recall response were indexed by electrodermal activity. In control subjects, larger skin conductance responses (SCRs) were associated with a greater frequency of guess reports, suggesting that SCRs provide a marker for uncertainty regarding the candidacy of a selected response. TBI patients did not show this relationship, suggesting that impairments of post-retrieval evaluation might also underlie greater false acceptance of misinformation. Discussion focuses on the role of the prefrontal cortex and cognitive processes that mediate the selection and evaluation of memories.


Asunto(s)
Lesiones Encefálicas/complicaciones , Señales (Psicología) , Decepción , Trastornos de la Memoria/etiología , Recuerdo Mental , Adulto , Lesiones Encefálicas/fisiopatología , Lesiones Encefálicas/psicología , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Femenino , Dedos , Respuesta Galvánica de la Piel , Humanos , Masculino , Trastornos de la Memoria/fisiopatología , Trastornos de la Memoria/psicología , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Corteza Prefrontal/lesiones
14.
J Exp Psychol Gen ; 134(2): 131-48, 2005 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15869342

RESUMEN

Recent research suggests that older adults are more susceptible to interference effects than are young adults; however, that research has failed to equate differences in original learning. In 4 experiments, the authors show that older adults are more susceptible to interference effects produced by a misleading prime. Even when original learning was equated, older adults were 10 times as likely to falsely remember misleading information and were much less likely to increase their accuracy by opting not to answer under conditions of free responding. The results are well described by a multinomial model that postulates multiple modes of cognitive control. According to that model, older adults are likely to be captured by misleading information, a form of goal neglect or deficit in inhibitory functions.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/fisiología , Cognición , Trastornos de la Memoria/diagnóstico , Adolescente , Adulto , Anciano , Atención , Señales (Psicología) , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Recuerdo Mental , Vocabulario
15.
Educ Psychol Meas ; 75(5): 785-804, 2015 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29795841

RESUMEN

It is more common for educational and psychological data to be nonnormal than to be approximately normal. This tendency may lead to bias and error in point estimates of the Pearson correlation coefficient. In a series of Monte Carlo simulations, the Pearson correlation was examined under conditions of normal and nonnormal data, and it was compared with its major alternatives, including the Spearman rank-order correlation, the bootstrap estimate, the Box-Cox transformation family, and a general normalizing transformation (i.e., rankit), as well as to various bias adjustments. Nonnormality caused the correlation coefficient to be inflated by up to +.14, particularly when the nonnormality involved heavy-tailed distributions. Traditional bias adjustments worsened this problem, further inflating the estimate. The Spearman and rankit correlations eliminated this inflation and provided conservative estimates. Rankit also minimized random error for most sample sizes, except for the smallest samples (n = 10), where bootstrapping was more effective. Overall, results justify the use of carefully chosen alternatives to the Pearson correlation when normality is violated.

16.
Schizophr Bull ; 40(6): 1422-32, 2014 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24214932

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: Converging research suggests that individuals with schizophrenia show a marked impairment in reinforcement learning, particularly in tasks requiring flexibility and adaptation. The problem has been associated with dopamine reward systems. This study explores, for the first time, the characteristics of this impairment and how it is affected by a behavioral intervention-cognitive remediation. METHOD: Using computational modelling, 3 reinforcement learning parameters based on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) trial-by-trial performance were estimated: R (reward sensitivity), P (punishment sensitivity), and D (choice consistency). In Study 1 the parameters were compared between a group of individuals with schizophrenia (n = 100) and a healthy control group (n = 50). In Study 2 the effect of cognitive remediation therapy (CRT) on these parameters was assessed in 2 groups of individuals with schizophrenia, one receiving CRT (n = 37) and the other receiving treatment as usual (TAU, n = 34). RESULTS: In Study 1 individuals with schizophrenia showed impairment in the R and P parameters compared with healthy controls. Study 2 demonstrated that sensitivity to negative feedback (P) and reward (R) improved in the CRT group after therapy compared with the TAU group. R and P parameter change correlated with WCST outputs. Improvements in R and P after CRT were associated with working memory gains and reduction of negative symptoms, respectively. CONCLUSION: Schizophrenia reinforcement learning difficulties negatively influence performance in shift learning tasks. CRT can improve sensitivity to reward and punishment. Identifying parameters that show change may be useful in experimental medicine studies to identify cognitive domains susceptible to improvement.


Asunto(s)
Terapia Cognitivo-Conductual/métodos , Modelos Teóricos , Refuerzo en Psicología , Recompensa , Esquizofrenia/fisiopatología , Adulto , Conducta de Elección/fisiología , Retroalimentación Psicológica , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Castigo/psicología , Esquizofrenia/terapia , Resultado del Tratamiento
17.
Psychol Methods ; 17(3): 399-417, 2012 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22563845

RESUMEN

It is well known that when data are nonnormally distributed, a test of the significance of Pearson's r may inflate Type I error rates and reduce power. Statistics textbooks and the simulation literature provide several alternatives to Pearson's correlation. However, the relative performance of these alternatives has been unclear. Two simulation studies were conducted to compare 12 methods, including Pearson, Spearman's rank-order, transformation, and resampling approaches. With most sample sizes (n ≥ 20), Type I and Type II error rates were minimized by transforming the data to a normal shape prior to assessing the Pearson correlation. Among transformation approaches, a general purpose rank-based inverse normal transformation (i.e., transformation to rankit scores) was most beneficial. However, when samples were both small (n ≤ 10) and extremely nonnormal, the permutation test often outperformed other alternatives, including various bootstrap tests.


Asunto(s)
Modelos Lineales , Dinámicas no Lineales , Estadística como Asunto/métodos , Interpretación Estadística de Datos , Método de Montecarlo , Tamaño de la Muestra , Distribuciones Estadísticas , Estadísticas no Paramétricas
18.
Psychol Aging ; 27(1): 22-32, 2012 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22023508

RESUMEN

Results of three experiments revealed that older, as compared to young, adults are more reliant on context when "seeing" a briefly flashed word that was preceded by a prime. In a congruent condition, the prime was the same word as flashed (e.g., DIRT dirt) whereas in an incongruent condition, the prime differed in a single letter from the word that was flashed (DART dirt). Following their attempt to identify the flashed word, participants were asked to report whether they had "seen" the flashed word or, instead, had responded on some other basis (knowing or guessing). Older adults showed dramatically higher false seeing by reporting the prime on incongruent trials and claiming to have seen it flashed. This was true even though a titration procedure was used to equate the performance of young and older adults on baseline trials which did not provide a biasing context. Results of Experiment 3 related age differences in false seeing to willingness to respond when given the option to withhold responses. Convergence of results with those showing higher false memory and false hearing are interpreted as evidence that older adults are less able to avoid misleading effects of context. That lessened ability may be associated with decline in frontal lobe functioning.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/fisiología , Anticipación Psicológica/fisiología , Ilusiones/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Factores de Edad , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Análisis de Varianza , Lóbulo Frontal/fisiopatología , Humanos , Memoria/fisiología , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Memoria Implícita , Adulto Joven
19.
J Abnorm Psychol ; 120(4): 911-21, 2011 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21875166

RESUMEN

Patients with bipolar disorder (BD) and schizophrenia (SZ) often show decision-making deficits in everyday circumstances. A failure to appropriately weigh immediate versus future consequences of choices may contribute to these deficits. We used the delay discounting task in individuals with BD or SZ to investigate their temporal decision making. Twenty-two individuals with BD, 21 individuals with SZ, and 30 healthy individuals completed the delay discounting task along with neuropsychological measures of working memory and cognitive function. Both BD and SZ groups discounted delayed rewards more steeply than did the healthy group even after controlling for current substance use, age, gender, and employment. Hierarchical multiple regression analyses showed that discounting rate was associated with both diagnostic group and working memory or intelligence scores. In each group, working memory or intelligence scores negatively correlated with discounting rate. The results suggest that (a) both BD and SZ groups value smaller, immediate rewards more than larger, delayed rewards compared with the healthy group and (b) working memory or intelligence is related to temporal decision making in individuals with BD or SZ as well as in healthy individuals.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno Bipolar/psicología , Recompensa , Esquizofrenia/fisiopatología , Psicología del Esquizofrénico , Trastornos Relacionados con Sustancias/psicología , Adulto , Toma de Decisiones , Diagnóstico Dual (Psiquiatría) , Manual Diagnóstico y Estadístico de los Trastornos Mentales , Femenino , Humanos , Conducta Impulsiva/psicología , Inteligencia , Masculino , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Modelos Psicológicos , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Escalas de Valoración Psiquiátrica , Análisis de Regresión , Trastornos Relacionados con Sustancias/diagnóstico , Factores de Tiempo
20.
Pers Soc Psychol Bull ; 36(10): 1397-408, 2010 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20837777

RESUMEN

People often misattribute the causes of their thoughts and feelings. The authors propose a multinomial process model of affect misattributions, which separates three component processes. The first is an affective response to the true cause of affect. The second is an affective response to the apparent cause. The third process is when the apparent source is confused for the real source. The model is validated using the affect misattribution procedure (AMP), which uses misattributions as a means to implicitly measure attitudes. The model illuminates not only the AMP but also other phenomena in which researchers wish to model the processes underlying misattributions using subjective judgments.


Asunto(s)
Afecto , Actitud , Autoimagen , Percepción Social , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Psicológicos , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Disposición en Psicología , Estados Unidos
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA