Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 15 de 15
Filtrar
1.
Psychol Sci ; 33(3): 343-353, 2022 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35191784

RESUMEN

The contributions of gonadal hormones to the development of human behavioral sex differences are subjects of intense scientific and social interest. Isolated gonadotropin-releasing-hormone deficiency (IGD) is a rare endocrine disorder that can reveal a possible role of early gonadal hormones. IGD is characterized by low or absent gonadal hormone production after the first trimester of gestation, but external genitalia and hence gender of rearing are concordant with chromosomal and gonadal sex. We investigated recalled childhood gender nonconformity in men (n = 65) and women (n = 32) with IGD and typically developing men (n = 463) and women (n = 1,207). Men with IGD showed elevated childhood gender nonconformity, particularly if they also reported undescended testes at birth, a marker of low perinatal androgens. Women with IGD did not differ from typically developing women. These results indicate that early androgen exposure after the first trimester contributes to male-typical gender-role behaviors in childhood.


Asunto(s)
Andrógenos , Identidad de Género , Femenino , Humanos , Recién Nacido , Masculino , Recuerdo Mental
2.
Horm Behav ; 127: 104886, 2021 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33202246

RESUMEN

Circulating gonadal hormones have been linked to variation in the structure and function of the adult human brain, raising the question of how cognition is affected by sex hormones in adulthood. The impacts of progestogens and estrogens are of special interest due to the widespread use of hormone supplementation. Multiple studies have analyzed relationships between ovarian hormones and mental rotation performance, one of the largest known cognitive sex differences; however, results are conflicting. These discrepancies are likely due in part to modest sample sizes and reliance on self-report measures to assess menstrual cycle phase. The present study aimed to clarify the impact of progestogens and estrogens on visuospatial cognition by relating mental rotation task performance to salivary hormone concentrations. Across two studies totaling 528 naturally-cycling premenopausal women, an internal meta-analysis suggested a small, positive effect of within-subjects changes in progesterone on MRT performance (estimate = 0.44, p = 0.014), though this result should be interpreted with caution given multiple statistical analyses. Between-subjects differences and within-subject changes in estradiol did not significantly predict MRT. These results shed light on the potential cognitive effects of endogenous and exogenous hormone action, and the proximate mechanisms modulating spatial cognition.


Asunto(s)
Cognición/fisiología , Hormonas Esteroides Gonadales/metabolismo , Ciclo Menstrual , Adolescente , Adulto , Estradiol/análisis , Estradiol/metabolismo , Femenino , Hormonas Esteroides Gonadales/análisis , Humanos , Ciclo Menstrual/metabolismo , Ciclo Menstrual/psicología , Progesterona/análisis , Progesterona/metabolismo , Saliva/química , Saliva/metabolismo , Caracteres Sexuales , Memoria Espacial/fisiología , Navegación Espacial/fisiología , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas , Adulto Joven
3.
Arch Sex Behav ; 50(8): 3377-3383, 2021 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34518958

RESUMEN

Male sexual orientation is influenced by environmental and complex genetic factors. Childhood gender nonconformity (CGN) is one of the strongest correlates of homosexuality with substantial familiality. We studied brothers in families with two or more homosexual brothers (409 concordant sibling pairs in 384 families, as well as their heterosexual brothers), who self-recalled their CGN. To map loci for CGN, we conducted a genome-wide linkage scan (GWLS) using SNP genotypes. The strongest linkage peaks, each with significant or suggestive two-point LOD scores and multipoint LOD score support, were on chromosomes 5q31 (maximum two-point LOD = 4.45), 6q12 (maximum two-point LOD = 3.64), 7q33 (maximum two-point LOD = 3.09), and 8q24 (maximum two-point LOD = 3.67), with the latter not overlapping with previously reported strongest linkage region for male sexual orientation on pericentromeric chromosome 8. Family-based association analyses were used to identify associated variants in the linkage regions, with a cluster of SNPs (minimum association p = 1.3 × 10-8) found at the 5q31 linkage peak. Genome-wide, clusters of multiple SNPs in the 10-6 to 10-8 p-value range were found at chromosomes 5p13, 5q31, 7q32, 8p22, and 10q23, highlighting glutamate-related genes. This is the first reported GWLS and genome-wide association study on CGN. Further increasing genetic knowledge about CGN and its relationships to male sexual orientation should help advance our understanding of the biology of these associated traits.


Asunto(s)
Identidad de Género , Estudio de Asociación del Genoma Completo , Ligamiento Genético , Heterosexualidad , Homosexualidad Masculina/genética , Humanos , Masculino , Hermanos
4.
Horm Behav ; 121: 104712, 2020 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32059854

RESUMEN

Experiments in male rodents demonstrate that sensitivity to the organizational effects of steroid hormones decreases across the pubertal window, with earlier androgen exposure leading to greater masculinization of the brain and behavior. Similarly, some research suggests the timing of peripubertal exposure to sex steroids influences aspects of human psychology, including visuospatial cognition. However, prior studies have been limited by small samples and/or imprecise measures of pubertal timing. We conducted 4 studies to clarify whether the timing of peripubertal hormone exposure predicts performance on male-typed tests of spatial cognition in adulthood. In Studies 1 (n = 1095) and 2 (n = 173), we investigated associations between recalled pubertal age and spatial cognition in typically developing men, controlling for current testosterone levels in Study 2. In Study 3 (n = 51), we examined the relationship between spatial performance and the age at which peripubertal hormone replacement therapy was initiated in a sample of men with Isolated GnRH Deficiency. Across Studies 1-3, effect size estimates for the relationship between spatial performance and pubertal timing ranged from. -0.04 and -0.27, and spatial performance was unrelated to salivary testosterone in Study 2. In Study 4, we conducted two meta-analyses of Studies 1-3 and four previously published studies. The first meta-analysis was conducted on correlations between spatial performance and measures of the absolute age of pubertal timing, and the second replaced those correlations with correlations between spatial performance and measures of relative pubertal timing where available. Point estimates for correlations between pubertal timing and spatial cognition were -0.15 and -0.12 (both p < 0.001) in the first and second meta-analyses, respectively. These associations were robust to the exclusion of any individual study. Our results suggest that, for some aspects of neural development, sensitivity to gonadal hormones declines across puberty, with earlier pubertal hormone exposure predicting greater sex-typicality in psychological phenotypes in adulthood. These results shed light on the processes of behavioral and brain organization and have implications for the treatment of IGD and other conditions wherein pubertal timing is pharmacologically manipulated.


Asunto(s)
Cognición/fisiología , Hormonas Esteroides Gonadales/fisiología , Pubertad/fisiología , Conducta Espacial/fisiología , Esteroides/sangre , Adolescente , Desarrollo del Adolescente/fisiología , Adulto , Factores de Edad , Animales , Encéfalo/crecimiento & desarrollo , Niño , Femenino , Hormonas Esteroides Gonadales/sangre , Humanos , Masculino , Neurogénesis/fisiología , Pubertad/sangre , Pubertad/psicología , Maduración Sexual/fisiología , Esteroides/fisiología , Testosterona/sangre , Factores de Tiempo , Adulto Joven
5.
Arch Sex Behav ; 49(7): 2461-2468, 2020 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31950380

RESUMEN

We examined whether recalled childhood gender nonconformity and self-reported adult gender nonconformity is familial, using data from 1154 families selected for having at least two homosexual brothers. Specifically, we examined the extent to which homosexual men's variation in gender nonconformity runs in families by examining pairs of genetic brothers who were both homosexual (N = 672-697 full sibling concordant pairs). We also examined similarity between homosexual and heterosexual brothers (N = 79-82 full sibling discordant pairs). Consistent with past studies, concordant pairs yielded modest positive correlations consistent with moderate genetic and/or familial environmental effects on gender nonconformity. Unlike results of smaller past studies, discordant pairs also yielded modest positive, though nonsignificant, correlations. Our results support the feasibility of supplementing genetic studies of male sexual orientation with analyses of gender nonconformity variation.


Asunto(s)
Identidad de Género , Heterosexualidad/psicología , Homosexualidad Masculina/psicología , Humanos , Masculino
6.
Proc Biol Sci ; 283(1829)2016 04 27.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27122553

RESUMEN

In many primates, including humans, the vocalizations of males and females differ dramatically, with male vocalizations and vocal anatomy often seeming to exaggerate apparent body size. These traits may be favoured by sexual selection because low-frequency male vocalizations intimidate rivals and/or attract females, but this hypothesis has not been systematically tested across primates, nor is it clear why competitors and potential mates should attend to vocalization frequencies. Here we show across anthropoids that sexual dimorphism in fundamental frequency (F0) increased during evolutionary transitions towards polygyny, and decreased during transitions towards monogamy. Surprisingly, humans exhibit greater F0 sexual dimorphism than any other ape. We also show that low-F0 vocalizations predict perceptions of men's dominance and attractiveness, and predict hormone profiles (low cortisol and high testosterone) related to immune function. These results suggest that low male F0 signals condition to competitors and mates, and evolved in male anthropoids in response to the intensity of mating competition.


Asunto(s)
Haplorrinos/fisiología , Preferencia en el Apareamiento Animal/fisiología , Conducta Sexual Animal/fisiología , Conducta Sexual/fisiología , Vocalización Animal/fisiología , Adolescente , Animales , Evolución Biológica , Tamaño Corporal/fisiología , Femenino , Haplorrinos/anatomía & histología , Haplorrinos/clasificación , Humanos , Hidrocortisona/fisiología , Masculino , Modelos Biológicos , Filogenia , Caracteres Sexuales , Testosterona/fisiología , Adulto Joven
7.
Horm Behav ; 71: 16-21, 2015 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25857930

RESUMEN

The human face displays a wealth of information, including information about dominance and fecundity. Dominance and fecundity are also associated with lower concentrations of the stress hormone cortisol, suggesting that cortisol may negatively predict facial dominance and attractiveness. We digitally photographed 61 women's faces, had these images rated by men and women for dominance, attractiveness, and femininity, and explored relationships between these perceptions and women's salivary cortisol concentrations. In a first study, we found that women with more dominant-appearing, but not more attractive, faces had lower cortisol levels. These associations were not due to age, ethnicity, time since waking, testosterone, or its interaction with cortisol. In a second study, composite images of women with low cortisol were perceived as more dominant than those of women with high cortisol significantly more often than chance by two samples of viewers, with a similar but non-significant trend in a third sample. However, data on perceptions of attractiveness were mixed; low-cortisol images were viewed as more attractive by two samples of US viewers and as less attractive by a sample of Mexican viewers. Our results suggest that having a more dominant-appearing face may be associated with lower stress and hence lower cortisol in women, and provide further evidence regarding the information content of the human face.


Asunto(s)
Cara , Hidrocortisona/metabolismo , Predominio Social , Adolescente , Femenino , Feminidad , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Deseabilidad Social , Percepción Social , Testosterona/metabolismo , Adulto Joven
8.
Horm Behav ; 63(1): 13-9, 2013 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23159480

RESUMEN

In many species, females are more sexually attractive to males near ovulation. Some evidence suggests a similar pattern in humans, but methodological limitations prohibit firm conclusions at present, and information on physiological mechanisms underlying any such pattern is lacking. In 202 normally-cycling women, we explored whether women's attractiveness changed over the cycle as a function of two likely candidates for mediating these changes: estradiol and progesterone. We scheduled women to attend one session during the late follicular phase and another during the mid-luteal phase. At each session, facial photographs, voice recordings and saliva samples were collected. All photographs and voice recordings were subsequently rated by men for attractiveness and by women for flirtatiousness and attractiveness to men. Saliva samples were assayed for estradiol and progesterone. We found that progesterone and its interaction with estradiol negatively predicted vocal attractiveness and overall (facial plus vocal) attractiveness to men. Progesterone also negatively predicted women's facial attractiveness to men and female-rated facial attractiveness, facial flirtatiousness and vocal attractiveness, but not female-rated vocal flirtatiousness. These results strongly suggest a pattern of increased attractiveness during peak fertility in the menstrual cycle and implicate estradiol and progesterone in driving these changes.


Asunto(s)
Estradiol/análisis , Ciclo Menstrual/metabolismo , Progesterona/análisis , Voz/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Belleza , Cara , Femenino , Fertilidad/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Saliva/química
9.
Arch Sex Behav ; 41(5): 1127-43, 2012 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22733154

RESUMEN

Whether women's orgasm is an adaptation is arguably the most contentious question in the study of the evolution of human sexuality. Indeed, this question is a veritable litmus test for adaptationism, separating those profoundly impressed with the pervasive and myriad correspondences between organisms' phenotypes and their conditions of life from those who apply the "onerous concept" of adaptation with more caution, skepticism or suspicion. Yet, the adaptedness of female orgasm is a question whose answer will elucidate mating dynamics in humans and nonhuman primates. There are two broad competing explanations for the evolution of orgasm in women: (1) the mate-choice hypothesis, which states that female orgasm has evolved to function in mate selection and (2) the byproduct hypothesis, which states that female orgasm has no evolutionary function, existing only because women share some early ontogeny with men, in whom orgasm is an adaptation. We review evidence for these hypotheses and identify areas where relevant evidence is lacking. Although additional research is needed before firm conclusions can be drawn, we find that the mate-choice hypothesis receives more support. Specifically, female orgasm appears to have evolved to increase the probability of fertilization from males whose genes would improve offspring fitness.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Biológica/fisiología , Evolución Biológica , Orgasmo/fisiología , Reproducción/fisiología , Coito/fisiología , Femenino , Aptitud Genética , Humanos , Matrimonio , Fenotipo
10.
Psychoneuroendocrinology ; 134: 105431, 2021 Sep 27.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34601343

RESUMEN

Ovarian estrogens may influence the development of the human brain and behavior, but there are few opportunities to test this possibility. Isolated GnRH deficiency (IGD) is a rare endocrine disorder that could provide evidence for the role of estrogens in organizing sexually differentiated phenotypes: Unlike typical development, development in individuals with IGD is characterized by low or absent gonadal hormone production after the first trimester of gestation. Because external genitalia develop in the first trimester, external appearance is nevertheless concordant with gonadal sex in people with IGD. We therefore investigated the effects of gonadal hormones on sexual orientation by comparing participants with IGD (n = 97) to controls (n = 1670). Women with IGD reported lower male-attraction compared with typically developing women. In contrast, no consistent sexuality differences between IGD and typically developing men were evident. Ovarian hormones after the first trimester appear to influence female-typical dimensions of sexual orientation.

11.
Psychoneuroendocrinology ; 119: 104733, 2020 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32563936

RESUMEN

Evidence suggests that psychosexuality in humans is modulated by both organizational effects of prenatal and peripubertal sex steroid hormones, and by activational effects of circulating hormones in adulthood. Experimental work in male rodents indicates that sensitivity to androgen-driven organization of sexual motivation decreases across the pubertal window, such that earlier puberty leads to greater sex-typicality. We test this hypothesis in typically developing men (n = 231) and women (n = 648), and in men (n = 72) and women (n = 32) with isolated GnRH deficiency (IGD), in whom the precise timing of peripubertal hormone exposure can be ascertained via the age at which hormone replacement therapy (HRT) was initiated. Psychosexuality was measured with the Sexual Desire Inventory-2 (SDI-2) and Sociosexual Orientation Inventory-Revised (SOI-R). In both sexes, earlier recalled absolute pubertal timing predicted higher psychosexuality in adulthood, although the magnitude of these associations varied with psychosexuality type and group (i.e., typically developing and IGD). Results were robust when controlling for circulating steroid hormones in typically developing participants. Age of initiation of HRT in men with IGD negatively predicted SOI-R. We discuss the clinical implications of our findings for conditions in which pubertal timing is medically altered.


Asunto(s)
Hormona Liberadora de Gonadotropina/deficiencia , Enfermedades Hipotalámicas , Libido/fisiología , Pubertad/fisiología , Maduración Sexual/fisiología , Adolescente , Desarrollo del Adolescente/fisiología , Adulto , Factores de Edad , Femenino , Hormonas Esteroides Gonadales/sangre , Humanos , Enfermedades Hipotalámicas/sangre , Enfermedades Hipotalámicas/diagnóstico , Enfermedades Hipotalámicas/fisiopatología , Enfermedades Hipotalámicas/psicología , Masculino , Pronóstico , Conducta Sexual/psicología , Factores de Tiempo , Adulto Joven
12.
Sci Rep ; 7(1): 16950, 2017 12 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29217827

RESUMEN

Family and twin studies suggest that genes play a role in male sexual orientation. We conducted a genome-wide association study (GWAS) of male sexual orientation on a primarily European ancestry sample of 1,077 homosexual men and 1,231 heterosexual men using Affymetrix single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) arrays. We identified several SNPs with p < 10-5, including regions of multiple supporting SNPs on chromosomes 13 (minimum p = 7.5 × 10-7) and 14 (p = 4.7 × 10-7). The genes nearest to these peaks have functions plausibly relevant to the development of sexual orientation. On chromosome 13, SLITRK6 is a neurodevelopmental gene mostly expressed in the diencephalon, which contains a region previously reported as differing in size in men by sexual orientation. On chromosome 14, TSHR genetic variants in intron 1 could conceivably help explain past findings relating familial atypical thyroid function and male homosexuality. Furthermore, skewed X chromosome inactivation has been found in the thyroid condition, Graves' disease, as well as in mothers of homosexual men. On pericentromeric chromosome 8 within our previously reported linkage peak, we found support (p = 4.1 × 10-3) for a SNP association previously reported (rs77013977, p = 7.1 × 10-8), with the combined analysis yielding p = 6.7 × 10-9, i.e., a genome-wide significant association.


Asunto(s)
Homosexualidad Masculina/genética , Polimorfismo de Nucleótido Simple , Cromosomas Humanos Par 13/genética , Cromosomas Humanos Par 14/genética , Femenino , Estudio de Asociación del Genoma Completo , Humanos , Masculino , Proteínas de la Membrana/genética , Receptores de Tirotropina/genética , Inactivación del Cromosoma X
13.
Twin Res Hum Genet ; 9(3): 467-72, 2006 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16790159

RESUMEN

Do women experience orgasm because this trait was shaped by natural selection to augment female fitness? Or are women merely the lucky recipients of developmental patterns favored by selection to produce orgasm in males? A recent and widely publicized book by Elisabeth Lloyd (2005a) contends that there is insufficient evidence to validate any of the adaptive explanations yet proposed for female orgasm. We agree. But our reading of the data differs from Lloyd's. In this essay, we outline why, unlike Caton (2006), whose review of Lloyd's book appeared previously in this journal, we are not persuaded by Lloyd's argument that female orgasm is a nonadaptive byproduct of orgasm in men. We hold this view because we disagree with the criteria Lloyd uses to evaluate evolutionary hypotheses, and because we believe Lloyd defines female orgasm too narrowly, ignoring critical information about its affective aspects.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Fisiológica , Evolución Biológica , Orgasmo/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos
14.
Twin Res Hum Genet ; 8(1): 27-33, 2005 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15836807

RESUMEN

This study reports on genetic and environmental influences on the frequency of orgasm in women during sexual intercourse, during other sexual contact with a partner, and during masturbation. Participants were drawn from the Australian Twin Registry, and recruited from a large, partly longitudinal twin-family study. Three thousand and eighty women responded to the anonymous self-report questionnaire, including 667 complete monozygotic (MZ) pairs and 377 complete dizygotic (DZ) same-sex pairs, 366 women from complete DZ opposite-sex pairs, and 626 women whose co-twins did not participate. Significant twin correlations were found for both MZ and DZ twin pairs for all three items of interest. Age effects were statistically significant for some items. Models incorporating additive genetic, shared and nonshared environmental influences provided the best fit for Items 1 and 3, while a model with additive and nonadditive genetic influences along with nonshared environment fitted the data from Item 2. While an independent pathway model fits the data most par-simoniously, a common pathway model incorporating additive genetic (A), shared environment (C), and unique environment (E) effects cannot be ruled out. Overall, genetic influences account for approximately 31% of the variance of frequency of orgasm during sexual intercourse, 37% of the variance of frequency of orgasm during sexual contact other than during intercourse, and 51% of the variance of frequency of orgasm during masturbation. Following Baker (1996), we speculate that this additive genetic variance might arise from frequency-dependent selection for a variety of female sexual strategies.


Asunto(s)
Orgasmo , Ambiente , Femenino , Genética Conductual , Humanos , Análisis Multivariante
15.
Behav Neurosci ; 124(2): 278-87, 2010 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20364887

RESUMEN

The ratio of the lengths of the second and fourth fingers (2D:4D) may serve as a marker for prenatal androgen signaling. Because people are typically unaware of their 2D:4D, its use allows possible effects of early sex hormone regimes and socialization to be disentangled. We conducted a meta-analysis on relationships between 2D:4D and sexual orientation in men and women in 18 independent samples of men and 16 independent samples of women. Collectively, these samples comprised 1,618 heterosexual men, 1,693 heterosexual women, 1,503 gay men, and 1,014 lesbians. In addition to identifying the normative heterosexual sex difference in 2D:4D for both hands, we found that heterosexual women had higher (more feminine) left- and right-hand 2D:4D than did lesbians, but we found no difference between heterosexual and gay men. Moderator analyses suggested that ethnicity explained some between-studies variation in men. These results add to a literature suggesting that early sex hormone signaling affects sexual orientation in women, and highlight the need for further research exploring the relationships among 2D:4D, sexual orientation, and ethnicity in men.


Asunto(s)
Dedos/anatomía & histología , Caracteres Sexuales , Conducta Sexual/fisiología , Femenino , Heterosexualidad/fisiología , Homosexualidad Femenina/estadística & datos numéricos , Homosexualidad Masculina/estadística & datos numéricos , Humanos , Masculino
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA