Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 37
Filtrar
Más filtros

Banco de datos
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
J Res Adolesc ; 33(2): 641-655, 2023 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36717971

RESUMEN

This longitudinal two-wave cross-national study investigated whether intentions, friends' substance use, and parent-adolescent substance-use specific communication predict adolescent alcohol and cannabis use 1 year later, while estimating reversed links. The temporal order between these two substances was also examined. We used multi-group cross-lagged panel modeling on data from 2 ethnically and socioeconomically diverse samples: Sint Maarten (N = 350; Mage  = 14.19) and the Netherlands (N = 602; Mage  = 13.50). Results showed that in the Netherlands, cannabis use predicts more subsequent problems (alcohol use, intention to use cannabis, and affiliation with cannabis-using friends). But for Sint Maarten, alcohol use predicts more subsequent problems (cannabis use, intention to use alcohol, and affiliation with alcohol-using friends). These opposing results demonstrate that caution is warranted when generalizing results across countries.


Asunto(s)
Cannabis , Trastornos Relacionados con Sustancias , Humanos , Adolescente , Intención , Trastornos Relacionados con Sustancias/epidemiología , Grupo Paritario , Padres , Etanol , Comunicación
2.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 206: 105099, 2021 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33631633

RESUMEN

Internalization of external rules is a behavioral manifestation of moral development during childhood, and its development has come to be understood from the view of a complex parenting-by-temperament process. To examine this developmental process, the current research investigated how maternal parenting behaviors and child effortful control foretell internalization throughout early to middle childhood with two longitudinal samples of Chinese mother-child dyads. In Study 1 (N = 226), maternal respect for autonomy and negative control during free plays at 15 months of age were observed. At 25 months, child cool and hot effortful control were measured with a Stroop-like categorization task and an externally imposed delay task. At 37 months, observed internalization of maternal rules was assessed. Results showed that for toddlers with high levels of cool effortful control, maternal respect for autonomy positively predicted later internalization. In Study 2 (N = 88), maternal respect for autonomy and negative control during free plays at 38 months of age were coded. At 60 months, child cool and hot effortful control were measured with a Stroop-like inhibition task and a delay-of-gratification task. Observed internalization of maternal and experimenter rules and mother-reported internalization in everyday life were assessed at 60 and 84 months. Results showed that for children low on either cool or hot effortful control, maternal respect for autonomy negatively predicted later internalization during childhood. Together, the current findings support an age-relevant goodness-of-fit model for internalization development in Chinese children throughout the first 7 years of life.


Asunto(s)
Responsabilidad Parental , Temperamento , Niño , China , Femenino , Humanos , Recién Nacido , Madres , Relaciones Padres-Hijo
3.
J Youth Adolesc ; 49(3): 693-705, 2020 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31863339

RESUMEN

Social neurodevelopmental imbalance models posit that peer presence causes heightened adolescent risk-taking particularly during early adolescence. Evolutionary theory suggests that these effects would be most pronounced in males. However, the small but growing number of experimental studies on peer presence effects in adolescent risky decision-making showed mixed findings, and the vast majority of such studies did not test for the above-described gender and adolescent phase moderation effects. Moreover, most of those studies did not assess the criterion validity of the employed risky decision-making tasks. The current study was designed to investigate the abovementioned hypotheses among a sample of 327 ethnically-diverse Dutch early and mid-adolescents (49.80% female; Mage = 13.61). No main effect of peer presence on the employed risky-decision making task (i.e., the stoplight game) was found. However, the results showed a gender by peer presence moderation effect. Namely, whereas boys and girls engaged in equal levels of risks when they completed the stoplight game alone, boys engaged in more risk-taking than girls when they completed this task together with two same-sex peers. In contrast, adolescent phase did not moderate peer presence effects on risk-taking. Finally, the results showed that performance on the stoplight game predicted self-reported real-world risky traffic behavior, alcohol use and delinquency. Taken together, using a validated task, the present findings demonstrate that individual differences (i.e., gender) can determine whether the social environment (i.e., peer presence) affect risk-taking in early- and mid-adolescents. The finding that performance on a laboratory risky decision-making task can perhaps help identify adolescents that are vulnerable to diverse types of heightened risk behaviors is an important finding for science as well as prevention and intervention efforts.


Asunto(s)
Conducta del Adolescente/psicología , Conducta Peligrosa , Toma de Decisiones , Grupo Paritario , Asunción de Riesgos , Medio Social , Adolescente , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estudios Prospectivos , Psicología del Adolescente , Autoinforme , Factores Sexuales
4.
Dev Psychopathol ; 31(2): 741-758, 2019 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30175699

RESUMEN

This study used a combination of microlevel observation data and longitudinal questionnaire data to study the relationship between differential reactivity and differential susceptibility, guided by three questions: (a) Does a subset of children exist that is both more likely to respond with increasingly negative emotions to increasingly negative emotions of mothers and with increasingly positive emotions to increasingly positive emotions of mothers ("emotional reactivity")? (b) Is emotional reactivity associated with temperament markers and rearing environment? (c) Are children who show high emotional reactivity "for better and for worse" also more susceptible to parenting predicting child behavior across a year? A total of 144 Dutch children (45.3% girls) aged four to six participated. Latent profile analyses revealed a group of average reactive children (87%) and a group that was emotionally reactive "for better and for worse" (13%). Highly reactive children scored higher on surgency and received lower levels of negative parenting. Finally, associations of negative and positive parenting with externalizing and prosocial behavior were similar (and nonsignificant) for highly reactive children and average reactive children. The findings suggest that children who are emotionally reactive "for better and for worse" within parent-child interactions are not necessarily more susceptible to parenting on a developmental time scale.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Infantil/psicología , Emociones , Relaciones Padres-Hijo , Responsabilidad Parental/psicología , Adulto , Niño , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Temperamento
5.
J Adolesc ; 74: 221-228, 2019 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31254781

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION: Indirect reciprocity serves as a crucial component of how we interact with strangers. Two types of indirect reciprocity can be distinguished: pay-it-forward reciprocity and third party reciprocity. Pay-it-forward reciprocity refers to behaviors where people who have been treated well by others (either fairly or generously), extend that fairness or generosity to a stranger. Third-party reciprocity refers to behaviors where third-party bystanders altruistically punish those who transgress against others or kindly help the victims. The expansion of adolescents' social world increases opportunities to exercise indirect reciprocity yet very little research has focused on this topic in this age group. The current research addresses this lacuna and investigates how younger adolescents differ from older adolescents in pay-it-forward and third party reciprocity. METHODS: With incentivized economic paradigms, we investigated both types of indirect reciprocity in younger (n = 50) and older adolescents (n = 46). RESULTS: The pay-it-forward task revealed that receiving an equal (vs. unequal) distribution led both younger and older adolescents to become fairer to a third person. In the third-party task, older adolescents were more likely to devote their own resources to enforce fairness norms than younger adolescents. CONCLUSION: Our results shed light on how adolescents perceive and act in complex social settings where direct reciprocity is unrealistic. Both younger and older adolescents are capable of engaging in both forms of indirect reciprocity with older adolescents being more discriminative in their norm-enforcing behaviors.


Asunto(s)
Conducta del Adolescente/psicología , Relaciones Interpersonales , Adolescente , Factores de Edad , Altruismo , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
6.
J Adolesc ; 66: 9-18, 2018 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29723686

RESUMEN

Although studies have shown links between minority stress and mental health (e.g., Meyer, 2003), there is little research explaining this association. Research has suggested that adequate coping skills might protect youth from the negative impact of stress (Compas et al., 2017). Thus, we aimed to examine: 1) whether associations between minority stress and depressive symptoms occurred through mechanisms of perceived burdensomeness and thwarted belongingness, and 2) whether these associations were dependent on level of problem-solving coping (moderated mediation). Using an online survey of 267 sexual minority youth from the Netherlands (16-22 years; 28.8% male), the results show an indirect relationship of sexual orientation victimization and internalized homophobia with depressive symptoms occurring through perceived burdensomeness; for both males and females. Problem-solving coping skills did not significantly moderate the aforementioned indirect relationships. These results have implications for prevention and intervention work that currently focuses on social isolation rather than perceived burdensomeness.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Psicológica , Depresión/psicología , Minorías Sexuales y de Género/psicología , Adolescente , Adulto , Acoso Escolar , Víctimas de Crimen/psicología , Estudios Transversales , Depresión/epidemiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Países Bajos/epidemiología , Percepción , Conducta Sexual/psicología , Minorías Sexuales y de Género/estadística & datos numéricos , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Adulto Joven
7.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 154: 78-97, 2017 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27837656

RESUMEN

Differential susceptibility theory proposes that a subset of individuals exist who display enhanced susceptibility to both negative (risk-promoting) and positive (development-enhancing) environments. This experiment represents the first attempt to directly test this assumption by exposing children in the experimental group to both negative and positive feedback using puppet role-plays. It thereby serves as an empirical test as well as a methodological primer for testing differential susceptibility. Dutch children (N=190, 45.3% girls) between the ages of 4 and 6years participated. We examined whether negative and positive feedback would differentially affect changes in positive and negative affect, in prosocial and antisocial intentions and behavior, depending on children's negative emotionality. Results show that on hearing negative feedback, children in the experimental group increased in negative affect and decreased in positive affect more strongly than children in the control group. On hearing positive feedback, children in the experimental group tended to increase in positive affect and decrease in prosocial behavior. However, changes in response to negative or positive feedback did not depend on children's negative emotionality. Moreover, using reliable change scores, we found support for a subset of "vulnerable" children but not for a subset of "susceptible" children. The findings offer suggestions to guide future differential susceptibility experiments.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Infantil/psicología , Retroalimentación , Relaciones Padres-Hijo , Responsabilidad Parental , Teoría Psicológica , Niño , Preescolar , Emociones , Investigación Empírica , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Países Bajos
8.
J Youth Adolesc ; 46(5): 931-942, 2017 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28130692

RESUMEN

Comprehensive sexuality education and sexuality education that is inclusive to lesbian, gay, bisexual, transgender, queer, and questioning (LGBTQ) youth is thought to educate and support youth in their social relations. Despite the obligation for Dutch schools to cover sexuality education in their curricula, including the topic of sexual diversity, the content that is covered varies widely across schools. With the current study, we present an overview of the content of sexuality education as reported by a sample of 601 Dutch adolescents (58.4% female youth) from six different high schools (e.g., public, Roman Catholic, protestant, anthroposophical; grades 10-12). Further, we examine whether the content or extensiveness of sexuality education at the beginning of the school year is related to a decrease in LGBTQ name-calling and an increase in the willingness to intervene when witnessing LGBTQ name-calling at the end of the school year. Adolescents completed three surveys, spaced four months apart. The results show that anatomy, STI prevention, and relationships are covered most often in sexuality education, with less attention to sexual diversity. Our longitudinal findings show that having a wide variety of topics covered in sexuality education-not just sexual diversity-was related to an increase in perceived willingness to intervene when witnessing LGBTQ name-calling by teachers or school staff, fellow students, and youth themselves (female youth). It also predicted a decrease in the occurrence of name-calling according to females. Our findings emphasize the importance of having comprehensive sexuality education in schools; it not only educates and empowers youth but also signals a safer school climate.


Asunto(s)
Conducta del Adolescente/psicología , Educación Sexual/métodos , Minorías Sexuales y de Género/psicología , Percepción Social , Adolescente , Bisexualidad/psicología , Femenino , Homosexualidad Femenina/psicología , Humanos , Masculino , Países Bajos , Instituciones Académicas , Conducta Sexual , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Personas Transgénero/psicología , Transexualidad/psicología
9.
Arch Sex Behav ; 44(3): 743-54, 2015 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25501659

RESUMEN

This study examined whether the development of sexualized media consumption and permissive sexual attitudes would be more strongly interrelated when adolescents perceived sexualized media images as highly realistic. We used data from a three-wave longitudinal sample of 444 Dutch adolescents aged 13-16 years at baseline. Results from parallel process latent growth modeling multigroup analyses showed that higher initial levels of sexualized media consumption were associated with higher initial level of permissive sexual attitudes. Moreover, increases of sexualized media consumption over time were associated with increases of permissive sexual attitudes over time. Considering the moderation by perceived realism, we found these effects only for those who perceived sexualized media as more realistic. Findings for male and female adolescents were similar except for the relations between initial levels and subsequent development. Among male adolescents who perceived sexualized media images to be realistic, higher initial levels of permissive sexual attitudes were related to subsequent less rapid development of sexualized media consumption. For male adolescents who perceived sexualized media to be less realistic, higher initial levels of sexualized media consumption were related to a subsequent less rapid development of permissive sexual attitudes. These relations were not found for female adolescents. Overall, our results suggest that, in male and female adolescents, those with a high level of perceived realism showed a correlated development of sexualized media consumption and permissive sexual attitudes. These findings point to a need for extended information on how to guide adolescents in interpreting and handling sexualized media in everyday life.


Asunto(s)
Conducta del Adolescente , Actitud , Medios de Comunicación de Masas , Conducta Sexual/psicología , Población Blanca/psicología , Adolescente , Coito/psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Países Bajos , Percepción
10.
J Pers ; 83(2): 155-66, 2015 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24471708

RESUMEN

In this study, we examined whether parents are differentially susceptible to support from their spouse and adolescent child depending on their personality traits, and whether differences in susceptibility to support among parents, in turn, are linked to the quality of support parents give to their children. Participants in this three-wave longitudinal study were 288 two-parent Dutch families with an adolescent child. Fathers were on average 43.9 years old (SD = 3.7 years), mothers were 41.7 years old (SD = 3.3 years), and adolescents (50% girls) were 14.5 years old (SD = 0.8 years). We found that the association between support from children toward their parents and subsequent support from parents toward their children was more pronounced for parents high on Openness, for better and for worse. Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, and Emotional Stability did not emerge as markers of differences in susceptibility. Also, parents did not differ in their susceptibility to support from their spouse, nor were differences in susceptibility found a year later when using data from a third wave. We found very modest support for differential susceptibility, only for Openness, and depending on the source of perceived support and on the timing of measurement.


Asunto(s)
Relaciones Familiares/psicología , Padres/psicología , Personalidad/fisiología , Apoyo Social , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Países Bajos , Relaciones Padres-Hijo
11.
Dev Psychol ; 60(5): 978-989, 2024 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38512190

RESUMEN

While negative associations between behavioral inhibition/shyness and social competence are well established for children from Western cultures, the directions of these associations have been inconsistent for Chinese children, partly due to the ongoing social-cultural changes in China. Drawing from three samples of young Chinese children (born between 2009 and 2019), we aim at examining how inhibition/shyness predicts cooperative behaviors and prosocial behaviors throughout early childhood. In Study 1 (N = 700, children aged between 36 and 72 months), mother-reported inhibition/shyness was negatively associated with mother-reported cooperative and prosocial behaviors during the preschool years. In Study 2 (N = 251, at 6, 15, 25, and 37 months of children's ages), mother-reported inhibition/shyness in infancy was negatively associated with mother-reported cooperative behaviors but was not related to observed cooperative behaviors at the early preschool age. Infancy inhibition/shyness was negatively associated with mother-reported and observed prosocial behaviors. In Study 3 (N = 95, at 14, 25, 38, and 60 months of children's ages), the inhibition/shyness trait, assessed by both observation and maternal report, did not predict any indicators of cooperative behaviors. Early childhood inhibition/shyness, however, still predicted fewer observed and mother-reported prosocial behaviors. On balance, our research supports a negative association between early inhibition/shyness and later prosocial behaviors. The mixed findings concerning cooperative behaviors are interpreted in light of sociopolitical changes in China during the past two decades. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Conducta Infantil , Inhibición Psicológica , Timidez , Habilidades Sociales , Humanos , Preescolar , Femenino , Masculino , China , Conducta Infantil/fisiología , Niño , Conducta Cooperativa , Desarrollo Infantil/fisiología , Lactante , Conducta Social , Pueblos del Este de Asia
12.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry ; 54(8): 881-9, 2013 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23398022

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: It is well documented that friends' externalizing problems and negative parent-child interactions predict externalizing problems in adolescence, but relatively little is known about the role of siblings. This four-wave, multi-informant study investigated linkages of siblings' externalizing problems and sibling-adolescent negative interactions on adolescents' externalizing problems, while examining and controlling for similar linkages with friends and parents. METHODS: Questionnaire data on externalizing problems and negative interactions were annually collected from 497 Dutch adolescents (M = 13.03 years, SD = 0.52, at baseline), as well as their siblings, mothers, fathers, and friends. RESULTS: Cross-lagged panel analyses revealed modest unique longitudinal paths from sibling externalizing problems to adolescent externalizing problems, for male and female adolescents, and for same-sex and mixed-sex sibling dyads, but only from older to younger siblings. Moreover, these paths were above and beyond significant paths from mother-adolescent negative interaction and friend externalizing problems to adolescent externalizing problems, 1 year later. No cross-lagged paths existed between sibling-adolescent negative interaction and adolescent externalizing problems. CONCLUSIONS: Taken together, it appears that especially older sibling externalizing problems may be a unique social risk factor for adolescent externalizing problems, equal in strength to significant parents' and friends' risk factors.


Asunto(s)
Desarrollo del Adolescente , Amigos , Control Interno-Externo , Relaciones Interpersonales , Relaciones Padres-Hijo , Padres , Relaciones entre Hermanos , Adolescente , Desarrollo del Adolescente/fisiología , Niño , Femenino , Amigos/psicología , Humanos , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Modelos Psicológicos , Países Bajos , Padres/psicología , Factores de Riesgo
13.
Dev Psychol ; 58(10): 1875-1886, 2022 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35771498

RESUMEN

Based on the goodness-of-fit theory, the current research examined how parental socialization expectations and socialization practices in infancy predicted child social adjustment in the preschool year dependent on child characteristics in toddlerhood with a longitudinal sample of Chinese families. Participants were 272 Chinese mother-child dyads. Maternal socialization goals of child autonomy and obedience were assessed when the child was 6 months old. Maternal respect for autonomy and negative control were observed in free-plays at 15 months. Mothers reported child compliance and inhibitory control at 25 months and rated child externalizing behaviors at 37 months. Results showed that for children with low levels of compliance or high levels of inhibitory control, obedience socialization goals predicted more externalizing behaviors, whereas for children with high levels of compliance or low levels of inhibitory control, obedience socialization goals predicted fewer externalizing behaviors. Moreover, for children with high levels of inhibitory control, higher levels of respect for autonomy or lower levels of negative control foretold fewer externalizing behaviors. Conversely, for children with low levels of inhibitory control, lower levels of respect for autonomy or higher levels of negative control forecasted fewer externalizing behaviors. Together, our findings demonstrate that socialization expectations, socialization practices, and child characteristics are jointly predictive of social adjustment across early childhood and all the significant interactions are characterized by the pattern of a contrastive effect, therefore congruently supporting the goodness-of-fit hypotheses. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Conducta Infantil , Socialización , Niño , Preescolar , China , Femenino , Humanos , Lactante , Relaciones Madre-Hijo , Madres , Ajuste Social
14.
J Adolesc ; 34(5): 813-27, 2011 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21397317

RESUMEN

The present study examines the cross-sectional and longitudinal associations between fathers' and mothers' parenting styles and male and female delinquency using a sample of 330 Dutch families with a mid or late adolescent son or daughter (ages 14-22), followed across two measurement waves with a 5-year interval. Parenting styles of fathers and mothers were linked to delinquency. A significant parenting style by sex interaction was found: neglectful parenting was related to higher levels of delinquency in males and permissive parenting was linked to delinquency in females. A long term relationship was found between fathers' neglectful parenting style and delinquency in males. Furthermore, results revealed that levels of delinquency were the lowest in families with at least one authoritative parent and highest in families with two neglectful parents, indicating that the level of delinquency was dependent on the combination of mother's and father's parenting styles.


Asunto(s)
Delincuencia Juvenil , Responsabilidad Parental/psicología , Adolescente , Adulto , Niño , Estudios Transversales , Femenino , Humanos , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Países Bajos , Factores Sexuales , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Adulto Joven
15.
Violence Vict ; 26(5): 608-30, 2011.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22145540

RESUMEN

Behavioral, lifestyle, and relationship factors have all been identified as risk factors that increase a woman's vulnerability to sexual violence victimization. However, it remains unclear which risk factors most strongly increase young women's vulnerability to sexual violence victimization because most studies only examine a few factors simultaneously. Using a cross-sectional sample of 764 female secondary school students from eastern Ethiopia, multivariate analyses revealed that high-rejection sensitivity, having multiple sexual partners, the frequent watching of pornography, and use of alcohol or other soft drugs (Khat or shisha) are factors associated with higher levels of sexual violence victimization. The overall rates of victimization is high in this group, with 68% of the young women studied having experienced at least one instance of sexual violence victimization. Based on type of sexual perpetration, 52% of the young women were victimized by at least one instance of sexual offence, 56% by sexual assault, 25% by sexual coercion, and 15% by sexual aggression. Qualitative data gathered from interviews of extracurricular club members and school officials and focus group discussion with students were used to further augment and illustrate results from the quantitative data. Several suggestions for intervention are presented in light of these results.


Asunto(s)
Víctimas de Crimen/estadística & datos numéricos , Asunción de Riesgos , Maltrato Conyugal/estadística & datos numéricos , Estudiantes/estadística & datos numéricos , Trastornos Relacionados con Sustancias/epidemiología , Adolescente , Conducta del Adolescente/psicología , Actitud Frente a la Salud , Cortejo/psicología , Víctimas de Crimen/psicología , Estudios Transversales , Etiopía/epidemiología , Femenino , Humanos , Grupo Paritario , Prevalencia , Factores de Riesgo , Parejas Sexuales , Maltrato Conyugal/psicología , Estudiantes/psicología , Trastornos Relacionados con Sustancias/psicología , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Adulto Joven
16.
J Genet Psychol ; 182(3): 129-148, 2021.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33704026

RESUMEN

This cross-cultural study compared the prosocial behaviors of 101 Dutch, 37 urban Indian and 91 urban Chinese preschoolers, investigated (potential) cultural differences on their mothers' values and goals, and examined how mothers' values and goals relate to preschoolers' prosocial behaviors. Preschoolers' prosocial behaviors were observed in three standardized, behavioral assessments. Mothers reported on their own values and socialization goals for their children. Results showed no cultural difference in prosocial behaviors. However, Indian and Chinese mothers rated self-enhancement values as more important than Dutch mothers, and Indian mothers rated self-transcendence values and relational goals as more important than the Chinese and Dutch mothers. No difference was found on autonomous goals. These findings suggest that current cultural differences on parental socialization processes are beyond the individualistic-collectivistic dichotomy often used to classify cultures and are more reflective of the independence of these two dimensions. Mothers in urban Indian and urban Chinese societies can be categorized into an autonomous-relatedness cultural model. Additionally, there might be an ongoing shift toward an independence model in the urban, Chinese societies. Furthermore, culture moderated the association between autonomous goals and observed prosocial behaviors, with this association being significant within the Dutch sample only. No other associations between values or goals and children's prosocial behavior were found. Overall, these findings support the ecocultural model of children's prosocial development, and further suggest that young preschoolers from different cultures are more alike than different in prosocial behaviors.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Infantil , Comparación Transcultural , Conducta Social , Preescolar , China , Femenino , Objetivos , Humanos , India , Masculino , Madres , Países Bajos , Padres , Valores Sociales , Socialización , Población Urbana
17.
J Adolesc ; 33(1): 21-31, 2010 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19596148

RESUMEN

This study applied the gender role model of socialization theory, the developmental aging theory, and the topic salience perspective to the investigation of parent-child value transmissions. Specifically, we examined whether the bi-directionality and selectivity of value transmissions differed as a function of parents' and children's gender and children's developmental phase (adolescence versus emerging adulthood). Transmissions between parents and children from 402 Dutch families on the topics of work as duty and hedonism were studied across a 5-year period using structural equation modeling. As expected, we did not find convincing support for the general models of gender socialization and developmental aging. Instead, parent-child value transmissions appeared to be qualified by value salience. Particularly, high salience of work as duty for fathers was related with great paternal involvement in transmissions on this value orientation and high salience of hedonism for sons and adolescents was linked to transmissions from these groups to parents.


Asunto(s)
Desarrollo Infantil , Identidad de Género , Relaciones Intergeneracionales , Relaciones Padres-Hijo , Valores Sociales , Adolescente , Adulto , Envejecimiento , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Países Bajos , Responsabilidad Parental , Teoría Psicológica , Socialización , Adulto Joven
18.
Front Psychol ; 11: 1398, 2020.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32714246

RESUMEN

Evidence that young children display more happiness when sharing than receiving treats supports that humans, by nature, are prosocial. However, whether this "warm glow" is also found for other prosocial behaviors (instrumental helping and empathic helping) and/or in different cultures is still unclear. Dutch (studies 1 and 2) and Chinese (study 3) young children participated in a sharing task, followed by instrumental helping and empathic helping tasks in which they were praised (thanked) if they helped. Consistent results were found across three studies, showing that (1) participants displayed more happiness after giving than receiving treats; (2) toddlers displayed more happiness after instrumental helping than initially interacting with the experimenter; and (3) toddlers' happiness remained the same after positive social feedback (i.e., being thanked). Taken together, these results indicate that independent of culture, both sharing and instrumental helping are emotionally rewarding, supporting an evolutionary origin of these behaviors.

19.
J Fam Psychol ; 23(2): 146-55, 2009 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19364209

RESUMEN

Using structural equation modeling, this study investigates father-child, mother-child, and father-mother transmissions on "work-as-duty" and "hedonism" across a 5-year period when children traverse late adolescence and emerging adulthood (N = 402 families). We found bidirectional father-child and child-to-mother transmissions on work-as-duty and child-to-father and bidirectional father-mother transmissions on hedonism. In addition, we examined whether family adaptability and cohesion influence these value transmissions. Father-to-child transmission on work-as-duty occurred regardless of family system levels, whereas child-to-parent transmissions on work-as-duty occurred only within more structured families. Furthermore, a more connected family climate tended to facilitate inter- and intragenerational value transmissions, but multiple-group analyses did not reveal strong evidence. All in all, this study showed that value socialization in the family is not a one-way-traffic process with parents solely influencing their children. Late adolescents and emerging adults also serve to socialize their parents and parents socialize each other.


Asunto(s)
Desarrollo del Adolescente/fisiología , Hijos Adultos/psicología , Relaciones Familiares , Padre/psicología , Madres/psicología , Valores Sociales , Adaptación Psicológica/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Países Bajos , Relaciones Padres-Hijo , Adulto Joven
20.
J Abnorm Child Psychol ; 36(2): 223-35, 2008 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17786548

RESUMEN

We investigated trajectories of adolescent delinquent development using data from the Pittsburgh Youth Study and examined the extent to which these different trajectories are differentially predicted by childhood parenting styles. Based on self-reported and official delinquency seriousness, covering ages 10-19, we identified five distinct delinquency trajectories differing in both level and change in seriousness over time: a nondelinquent, minor persisting, moderate desisting, serious persisting, and serious desisting trajectory. More serious delinquents tended to more frequently engage in delinquency, and to report a higher proportion of theft. Proportionally, serious persistent delinquents were the most violent of all trajectory groups. Using cluster analysis we identified three parenting styles: authoritative, authoritarian (moderately supportive), and neglectful (punishing). Controlling for demographic characteristics and childhood delinquency, neglectful parenting was more frequent in moderate desisters, serious persisters, and serious desisters, suggesting that parenting styles differentiate non- or minor delinquents from more serious delinquents.


Asunto(s)
Delincuencia Juvenil/psicología , Delincuencia Juvenil/estadística & datos numéricos , Relaciones Padres-Hijo , Responsabilidad Parental/psicología , Adolescente , Conducta del Adolescente/psicología , Adulto , Autoritarismo , Niño , Maltrato a los Niños/psicología , Maltrato a los Niños/estadística & datos numéricos , Análisis por Conglomerados , Estudios de Cohortes , Crimen/psicología , Crimen/estadística & datos numéricos , Estudios de Seguimiento , Humanos , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Pennsylvania , Valor Predictivo de las Pruebas , Castigo/psicología , Índice de Severidad de la Enfermedad , Robo/psicología , Robo/estadística & datos numéricos , Violencia/psicología , Violencia/estadística & datos numéricos
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA