Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 5 de 5
Filtrar
1.
Cancer Res ; 61(20): 7642-6, 2001 Oct 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11606406

RESUMEN

We investigated the role of alterations of HDM2, the human homologue of murine mdm2, in the tumorigenesis and progression of cutaneous melanoma. A well-characterized cohort of 172 cases representing different points in the spectrum of melanocyte transformation (16 dysplastic nevi, 11 melanomas in situ, 107 invasive primaries, and 38 metastatic lesions), as well as 11 human melanoma cell lines were examined by immunohistochemistry and Western blotting for HDM2 protein expression, and by either Southern blotting (SB) or fluorescence in situ hybridization for HDM2 gene amplification. HDM2 overexpression, defined as >20% tumor cells showing nuclear immunoreactivity, was observed in 1 of 16 (6%) dysplastic nevi, 3 of 11 (27%) melanomas in situ, and 81 of 145 (56%) invasive primary and metastatic melanomas. Comparable frequencies of HDM2 overexpression were observed among invasive primary cases with differing tumor thicknesses as well as among the metastatic cases: 21 of 40 (53%) at < or =1.5 mm; 31 of 50 (62%) at 1.6-3.9 mm; 10 of 17 (58%) at >4 mm; and 19 of 38 (50%) metastases. HDM2 amplification was observed in 1 of 88 (1%) primary cases using fluorescence in situ hybridization, and in 0 of 12 (0%) metastatic cases that overexpressed HDM2 using SB. Melanoma cell lines expressed HDM2 protein, but there was no evidence of amplification by SB. Our data suggest that HDM2 protein overexpression is common in invasive and metastatic melanoma. Observing HDM2 overexpression in noninvasive melanoma suggests that expression of this oncogene may play an early role in melanocyte transformation. HDM2 amplification occurs infrequently, and other mechanisms that up-regulate HDM2 expression are under investigation.


Asunto(s)
Melanoma/metabolismo , Proteínas Nucleares , Proteínas Proto-Oncogénicas/genética , Neoplasias Cutáneas/genética , Transformación Celular Neoplásica/genética , Estudios de Cohortes , Síndrome del Nevo Displásico/genética , Síndrome del Nevo Displásico/metabolismo , Amplificación de Genes , Humanos , Inmunohistoquímica , Melanocitos/metabolismo , Melanocitos/patología , Melanoma/genética , Proteínas Proto-Oncogénicas/biosíntesis , Proteínas Proto-Oncogénicas c-mdm2 , Neoplasias Cutáneas/metabolismo , Células Tumorales Cultivadas
2.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 52(3): 511-24, 1987 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3572722

RESUMEN

This article explores the possibility that romantic love is an attachment process--a biosocial process by which affectional bonds are formed between adult lovers, just as affectional bonds are formed earlier in life between human infants and their parents. Key components of attachment theory, developed by Bowlby, Ainsworth, and others to explain the development of affectional bonds in infancy, were translated into terms appropriate to adult romantic love. The translation centered on the three major styles of attachment in infancy--secure, avoidant, and anxious/ambivalent--and on the notion that continuity of relationship style is due in part to mental models (Bowlby's "inner working models") of self and social life. These models, and hence a person's attachment style, are seen as determined in part by childhood relationships with parents. Two questionnaire studies indicated that relative prevalence of the three attachment styles is roughly the same in adulthood as in infancy, the three kinds of adults differ predictably in the way they experience romantic love, and attachment style is related in theoretically meaningful ways to mental models of self and social relationships and to relationship experiences with parents. Implications for theories of romantic love are discussed, as are measurement problems and other issues related to future tests of the attachment perspective.


Asunto(s)
Amor , Apego a Objetos , Adolescente , Adulto , Anciano , Ansiedad/psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Soledad , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Padres/psicología , Desarrollo de la Personalidad , Teoría Psicológica , Factores Sexuales
3.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 60(6): 861-9, 1991 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1865324

RESUMEN

Working models of attachment in marital functioning were examined. The security and accuracy of working models were measured with a new Q-sort method. Spouses with secure working models (self as relying on partner and partner as psychologically available) showed more constructive modulation of emotion and reported better marital adjustment. The accuracy of internal working models, measured with an objective index of spouses' agreement about models, was associated with independent reports of marital adjustment and observers' ratings of communication in problem-solving and confiding tasks. Behavior in communication tasks showed predictable associations with attachment security. Husbands' attachment security covaried with wives' rejection during problem solving, and wives' security covaried with quality of husbands' listening during a confiding task. A reciprocal interaction view of working models and marital functioning is supported.


Asunto(s)
Matrimonio/psicología , Modelos Psicológicos , Apego a Objetos , Autoimagen , Adaptación Psicológica , Adulto , Comunicación , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Pruebas de Personalidad
4.
Child Dev ; 59(2): 467-79, 1988 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3359865

RESUMEN

We report an investigation of the development of visual expectancies in 3.5-month-old infants. One of the infant's eyes was videorecorded as the infant watched a series of slides that were presented noncontingent on behavior. Babies were presented an alternating and an irregular series of 30 slides with a 700-msec onset duration separated by an interstimulus interval (ISI). The ISI for the alternating series was 1,100 msec, whereas the slides for the irregular series were separated by 900, 1,100, or 1,300 msec, randomly ordered. One-half of the babies saw the irregular series first, and one-half saw the regular series first. Babies in both groups provided evidence that they developed expectations for the visual events in the alternating series. Their reaction times (RTs) declined significantly from 3-5 "baseline" presentations, and their RTs were reliably faster during the alternating than the irregular series. Additionally, babies in the alternating-late group had significantly more stimulus anticipations during the alternating than during the irregular series. These findings indicate that 3.5-month-olds can detect regularity in a spatiotemporal series, will develop expectancies for events in the series, and will act on the basis of those expectancies even when those actions have no effect on the stimulus events. We believe that infants are motivated to develop expectations for noncontrollable spatiotemporal events, because these expectations permit them to bring their visual behavior under partial internal control.


Asunto(s)
Cognición , Psicología Infantil , Percepción Visual , Movimientos Oculares , Humanos , Lactante , Tiempo de Reacción , Percepción Espacial , Percepción del Tiempo , Percepción Visual/fisiología
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA