Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 47
Filtrar
Más filtros

Banco de datos
País/Región como asunto
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35790649

RESUMEN

A systematic review and meta-analysis was conducted where we evaluated the effects of Parent Management Training (PMT), Parent-Child Interaction Therapy (PCIT) and PMT combined with child cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) using data from 25 RCTs on children with clinical levels of disruptive behavior (age range 2-13 years). Results showed that PMT (g = 0.64 [95% CI 0.42, 0.86]) and PCIT (g = 1.22 [95% CI 0.75, 1.69]) were more effective than waiting-list (WL) in reducing parent-rated disruptive behavior, and PMT also in improving parental skills (g = 0.83 [95% CI 0.67, 0.98]) and child social skills (g = 0.49 [95% CI 0.30, 0.68]). PCIT versus WL had larger effects in reducing disruptive behavior than PMT versus WL. In the few studies found, the addition of child CBT to PMT did not yield larger effects than PMT or WL. These results support offering PMT to children with clinical levels of disruptive behavior and highlight the additional benefits of PCIT for younger ages.

2.
Horm Behav ; 109: 64-70, 2019 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30742831

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Hypospadias is a common malformation of the male external genitalia that results in urethral displacement with different levels of severity. Male genital development during the fetal period is dependent on androgen function, while the etiology of hypospadias differs and can be multifactorial. The psychosocial outcome is sometimes affected, but according to several studies acceptable. The question of whether hypospadias is associated with differences in psychosexual development has been investigated previously, with mixed results. There are no previous investigations of cognitive abilities in men with hypospadias. OBJECTIVE: The aim of this study was to investigate whether hypospadias is associated with differences in performance on cognitive tests and/or gender role behavior. PARTICIPANTS: Eighty-six men with hypospadias were compared to male and female controls from the general population. PROCEDURE: Cognitive tasks, previously shown to yield group level sex differences and questions regarding self-reported childhood gender role behavior, were administered either at an outpatient clinic visit or via online participation. RESULTS: The cognitive performance of men and women in the control groups differed significantly in the expected directions. Men and women also differed on self-reported childhood gender role behavior questions. There were no significant differences between men with and without hypospadias on any of the measures. Men with proximal hypospadias performed slightly lower on many of the cognitive tasks in comparison to men with distal hypospadias and controls. CONCLUSION: In general, hypospadias is not associated with differences in performance on cognitive tests that typically yield sex differences or with altered gender role behavior in childhood. Further studies on cognitive abilities in boys and men with proximal hypospadias are warranted.


Asunto(s)
Conducta/fisiología , Cognición/fisiología , Identidad de Género , Hipospadias/psicología , Adulto , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Niño , Estudios Transversales , Femenino , Estudios de Seguimiento , Humanos , Hipospadias/fisiopatología , Hipospadias/cirugía , Masculino , Autoinforme , Caracteres Sexuales , Adulto Joven
3.
Infancy ; 24(3): 356-367, 2019 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32677193

RESUMEN

This research examined how caregiver experience (female primary caregiver or distributed caregiving with mom and dad) influenced 10-, 14-, and 16-month-olds' visual preferences and attention toward internal facial features of female-male face pairs, and how these behaviors related to novelty preferences in a face recognition task and speed and accuracy on a visual search task. In the visual preference task, infants visually preferred male faces, regardless of caregiver experience. Despite similarities in visual preferences, infants' attention toward females and males' internal facial features was related for infants with distributed caregiving only. Infants' performance across face processing tasks most often correlated for those with female primary caregivers. Results further our understanding of how infants with female primary caregivers display specialized processing of female faces, and how infants with distributed caregiving show similarities in their attention to female and male facial features.

4.
Hippocampus ; 28(2): 151-163, 2018 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29171897

RESUMEN

The hippocampus (HC) interacts with distributed brain regions to support memory and shows significant volume reductions in aging, but little is known about age effects on hippocampal whole-brain structural covariance. It is also unclear whether the anterior and posterior HC show similar or distinct patterns of whole-brain covariance and to what extent these are related to memory functions organized along the hippocampal longitudinal axis. Using the multivariate approach partial least squares, we assessed structural whole-brain covariance of the HC in addition to regional volume, in young, middle-aged and older adults (n = 221), and assessed associations with episodic and spatial memory. Based on findings of sex differences in both memory and brain aging, we further considered sex as a potential modulating factor of age effects. There were two main covariance patterns: one capturing common anterior and posterior covariance, and one differentiating the two regions by capturing anterior-specific covariance only. These patterns were differentially related to associative memory while unrelated to measures of single-item memory and spatial memory. Although patterns were qualitatively comparable across age groups, participants' expression of both patterns decreased with age, independently of sex. The results suggest that the organization of hippocampal structural whole-brain covariance remains stable across age, but that the integrity of these networks decreases as the brain undergoes age-related alterations.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/fisiología , Encéfalo/fisiología , Hipocampo/fisiología , Memoria/fisiología , Adulto , Anciano , Asociación , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Femenino , Hipocampo/diagnóstico por imagen , Humanos , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Vías Nerviosas/fisiología , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Adulto Joven
5.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 111(32): 11673-8, 2014 Aug 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25071201

RESUMEN

Cognitive gender differences and the reasons for their origins have fascinated researchers for decades. Using nationally representative data to investigate gender differences in cognitive performance in middle-aged and older populations across Europe, we show that the magnitude of these differences varies systematically across cognitive tasks, birth cohorts, and regions, but also that the living conditions and educational opportunities individuals are exposed to during their formative years are related to their later cognitive performance. Specifically, we demonstrate that improved living conditions and less gender-restricted educational opportunities are associated with increased gender differences favoring women in some cognitive functions (i.e., episodic memory) and decreases (i.e., numeracy) or elimination of differences in other cognitive abilities (i.e., category fluency). Our results suggest that these changes take place due to a general increase in women's cognitive performance over time, associated with societal improvements in living conditions and educational opportunities.


Asunto(s)
Cognición/fisiología , Caracteres Sexuales , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Estudios de Cohortes , Comparación Transcultural , Escolaridad , Europa (Continente) , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Memoria Episódica , Persona de Mediana Edad , Psicometría , Condiciones Sociales
6.
Am J Geriatr Psychiatry ; 23(9): 885-96, 2015 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25746486

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: To test a life-course model of cognitive reserve in dementia and examine if school grades around age 10 years, formal educational attainment, and lifetime occupational complexity affect the risk of dementia in old age. METHODS: 7,574 men and women from the Uppsala Birth Cohort Multigenerational Study were followed for 21 years. Information on school performance, formal education, and occupational attainment was collected prospectively from elementary school archives and population censuses. Dementia diagnosis was extracted from the two Swedish registers. Discrete-time Cox proportional hazard models were estimated. RESULTS: Dementia was diagnosed in 950 individuals (12.5%). Dementia risk was lower among individuals with higher childhood school grades (hazard ratio [HR]: 0.79; 95% confidence interval [CI]: 0.68 to 0.93) and was lower among individuals in data-complex occupations (HR: 0.77; 95% CI: 0.64 to 0.92). Professional/university education predicted lower risk of dementia in minimally adjusted models (HR: 0.74; 95% CI: 0.60 to 0.91), although the effect faded with adjustment for occupational complexity. Lowest risk was found in the group with both higher childhood school performance and high occupational complexity with data (HR: 0.61; 95% CI: 0.50 to 0.75). Importantly, high occupational complexity could not compensate for the effect of low childhood grades. In contrast, dementia risk was reduced in those with higher school grades, irrespective of occupational complexity. CONCLUSION: Higher childhood school performance is protective of dementia risk, particularly when preserved through complex work environments in adulthood, although it will remain protective even in the absence of later-life educational or occupational stimulation.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/psicología , Reserva Cognitiva , Demencia/psicología , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Demencia/diagnóstico , Demencia/epidemiología , Escolaridad , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Ocupaciones , Sistema de Registros , Factores de Riesgo , Suecia/epidemiología
7.
Neuroimage ; 99: 215-25, 2014 Oct 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24857714

RESUMEN

Sex differences in episodic and spatial memory are frequently observed, suggesting that there may be sex-related structural differences in the hippocampus (HC). Earlier findings are inconsistent, possibly due to a known variability along the hippocampal longitudinal axis. Here, we assessed potential sex differences in hippocampal volume and structural covariance with the rest of the brain in young men and women (N=76), considering the anterior (aHC) and posterior (pHC) hippocampus separately. Women exhibited a larger pHC than men adjusted for brain size. Using partial least squares, we identified two significant patterns of structural covariance of the aHC and pHC. The first included brain areas that covaried positively and negatively in volume with both the aHC and pHC in men, but showed greater covariance with the aHC than pHC in women. The second pattern revealed distinct structural covariance of the aHC and pHC that showed a clear difference between men and women: in men the pHC showed reliable structural covariance with the medial and lateral parietal lobes and the prefrontal cortex, whereas in women the aHC showed reliable structural covariance with the anterior temporal lobe bilaterally. This pattern converges with resting state functional connectivity of the aHC and pHC and suggests that these hippocampal sections interact with different brain regions, consistent with a division of labor with regards to episodic and spatial memory. Our findings lend support to a division of the HC into an anterior and posterior part and identify sex as a potential moderating factor when investigating hippocampal structure and connectivity.


Asunto(s)
Hipocampo/anatomía & histología , Adulto , Cognición/fisiología , Femenino , Sustancia Gris/anatomía & histología , Humanos , Masculino , Aprendizaje por Laberinto/fisiología , Memoria Episódica , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Caracteres Sexuales , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Adulto Joven
8.
Perspect Psychol Sci ; : 17456916231202685, 2024 Jan 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38170215

RESUMEN

Some studies show that living conditions, such as economy, gender equality, and education, are associated with the magnitude of psychological sex differences. We systematically and quantitatively reviewed 54 articles and conducted new analyses on 27 meta-analyses and large-scale studies to investigate the association between living conditions and psychological sex differences. We found that sex differences in personality, verbal abilities, episodic memory, and negative emotions are more pronounced in countries with higher living conditions. In contrast, sex differences in sexual behavior, partner preferences, and math are smaller in countries with higher living conditions. We also observed that economic indicators of living conditions, such as gross domestic product, are most sensitive in predicting the magnitude of sex differences. Taken together, results indicate that more sex differences are larger, rather than smaller, in countries with higher living conditions. It should therefore be expected that the magnitude of most psychological sex differences will remain unchanged or become more pronounced with improvements in living conditions, such as economy, gender equality, and education.

9.
Memory ; 20(4): 321-31, 2012.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22364145

RESUMEN

Previous research suggests that the own-race bias (ORB) in memory for faces is a result of other-race faces receiving less visual attention at encoding. As women typically display an own-gender bias in memory for faces and men do not, we investigated whether face gender and sex of viewer influenced visual attention and memory for own- and other-race faces, and if preferential viewing of own-race faces contributed to the ORB in memory. Participants viewed pairs of female or male own- and other-race faces while their viewing time was recorded. Afterwards, they completed a surprise memory test. We found that (1) other-race males received the initial focus of attention, (2) own-race faces were viewed longer than other-race faces over time, although the difference was larger for female faces, and (3) even though longer viewing time increased the probability of remembering a face, it did not explain the magnified ORB in memory for female faces. Importantly, these findings highlight that face gender moderates attentional responses to and memory for own- and other-race faces.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Cara , Memoria , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Grupos Raciales/psicología , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Modelos Logísticos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Caracteres Sexuales , Adulto Joven
10.
J Speech Lang Hear Res ; 65(12): 4608-4622, 2022 12 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36399792

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: We aimed to cross-sectionally describe the impact of stuttering on persons who stutter (PWS): children, adolescents, and young adults. Based on previous research on PWS and psychosocial health in the general population, we hypothesized that (a) the adverse impact of stuttering in PWS would be larger among adolescents than children and young adults and that (b) females, especially adolescent females, would report being more adversely impacted by their stuttering than males. METHOD: We pooled samples of Swedish PWS, obtaining 162 individuals (75 females and 87 males), aged 7-30 years. We measured the impact of stuttering using age-relevant versions of the Overall Assessment of the Speaker's Experience of Stuttering (OASES). The relationship between OASES score, age, and sex was described using a polynomial model with an interaction term between age and sex to allow for potential differences between females and males' age-related curves. RESULTS: The average trends were that (a) the impact of stuttering was greater for the adolescents than for the children and young adults, and (b) females, especially adolescent females, were on average more impacted by their stuttering than males. Taking self-reported speech fluency into account did not change this pattern. CONCLUSIONS: In line with findings on psychosocial health, communication attitude, and self-esteem in the general population, the impact of stuttering seems to be particularly adverse among adolescents, especially female adolescents. Thus, clinicians need to be aware of the risk that young girls who stutter may develop a negative attitude to speech and communication, and this should also be communicated to caregivers and teachers. SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL: https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.21554877.


Asunto(s)
Tartamudeo , Adulto Joven , Masculino , Adolescente , Niño , Humanos , Femenino , Adulto , Estudios Transversales , Tartamudeo/psicología , Lenguaje , Autoimagen , Habla
11.
J Fluency Disord ; 67: 105822, 2021 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33348210

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: The aim of this cross-sectional study was to explore the impact and experience of stuttering, and attitude to communication for female and male teenagers who stutter (TWS) in comparison with teenagers with no stutter (TWNS). METHODS: The Swedish version of the Overall Assessment of the Speaker's Experience of Stuttering (OASES-T-S), was administered to 56 TWS, 13-17 years old (26 females, 30 males). An adapted version of OASES, Attitude to Speech and Communication (ASC), was administered to 233 TWNS. The impact scores were analyzed in relation to sex in TWS and TWNS. RESULTS: Female TWS reported that stuttering had a greater impact on their life than it did on male TWS (on average 0.5 higher impact scores; Hedges' g = 0.87). The differences did not seem to be caused by differences in severity of overt stuttering symptoms. Female and male TWS particularly differed on items related to difficulties in day-to-day communication and affective/behavioral reactions to stuttering. Such sex differences were not as pronounced among TWNS, suggesting that teenage women may be more vulnerable to the negative impact of stuttering than men the same age. CONCLUSION: Female teenagers report more negative experiences and a greater tendency to use avoidance strategies than male teenagers.


Asunto(s)
Tartamudeo , Adolescente , Estudios Transversales , Femenino , Humanos , Lenguaje , Masculino , Habla , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
13.
Front Psychol ; 11: 613, 2020.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32362856

RESUMEN

Men as a group have been shown to have larger variances than women in several areas pertaining to both biological and psychological traits, but no investigation has been performed in regard to episodic memory. We conducted an analysis on sex differences in episodic memory variance on 535 studies, representing 962,946 individuals, conducted between 1973 and 2013. Results showed that men had larger variances than women in verbal episodic memory tasks as well as episodic memory tasks having to do with spatial locations. Women, on the other hand, had larger variance than men for tasks involving remembering routes. These effects were for the most part small, and exploratory analyses suggest that they might come about, at least in part, because of measures not sufficiently controlled for ceiling effects. This means that the effects should be interpreted with caution and that further research on sex differences in episodic memory variance is needed.

14.
Front Psychol ; 11: 554, 2020.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32273865

RESUMEN

Sweden consistently ranks at the top of international assessments of gender equality, but paradoxically exhibits marked horizontal gender segregation in the labor market. By combining administrative and respondent-collected data, this study investigates whether occupational attributes are associated with sex distribution in Swedish occupations over a 10-year period between 2002 and 2011. Results show that the proportion of women was higher, on average, in occupations high in people orientation and verbal demands and lower in occupations high in things orientation and numerical demands. Mixed linear models showed a trend for desegregation during this period, as the proportion of women in people-oriented occupations has declined and a trend for an increase in the proportion of women in numerically demanding occupations was observed. Occupational attributes aid the understanding of gender segregation but patterns of segregation seem to change over time.

15.
Ageing Res Rev ; 64: 101184, 2020 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32992046

RESUMEN

Individual differences in cognitive performance increase with advancing age, reflecting marked cognitive changes in some individuals along with little or no change in others. Genetic and lifestyle factors are assumed to influence cognitive performance in ageing by affecting the magnitude and extent of age-related brain changes (i.e., brain maintenance or atrophy), as well as the ability to recruit compensatory processes. The purpose of this review is to present findings from the Betula study and other longitudinal studies, with a focus on clarifying the role of key biological and environmental factors assumed to underlie individual differences in brain and cognitive ageing. We discuss the vital importance of sampling, analytic methods, consideration of non-ignorable dropout, and related issues for valid conclusions on factors that influence healthy neurocognitive ageing.


Asunto(s)
Betula , Envejecimiento Cognitivo , Envejecimiento , Encéfalo , Humanos , Estudios Longitudinales
16.
Transgend Health ; 5(4): 246-257, 2020.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33376803

RESUMEN

Purpose: Pubertal suppression is standard of care for early pubertal transgender youth to prevent the development of undesired and distressing secondary sex characteristics incongruent with gender identity. Preliminary evidence suggests pubertal suppression improves mental health functioning. Given the widespread changes in brain and cognition that occur during puberty, a critical question is whether this treatment impacts neurodevelopment. Methods: A Delphi consensus procedure engaged 24 international experts in neurodevelopment, gender development, puberty/adolescence, neuroendocrinology, and statistics/psychometrics to identify priority research methodologies to address the empirical question: is pubertal suppression treatment associated with real-world neurocognitive sequelae? Recommended study approaches reaching 80% consensus were included in the consensus parameter. Results: The Delphi procedure identified 160 initial expert recommendations, 44 of which ultimately achieved consensus. Consensus study design elements include the following: a minimum of three measurement time points, pubertal staging at baseline, statistical modeling of sex in analyses, use of analytic approaches that account for heterogeneity, and use of multiple comparison groups to minimize the limitations of any one group. Consensus study comparison groups include untreated transgender youth matched on pubertal stage, cisgender (i.e., gender congruent) youth matched on pubertal stage, and an independent sample from a large-scale youth development database. The consensus domains for assessment includes: mental health, executive function/cognitive control, and social awareness/functioning. Conclusion: An international interdisciplinary team of experts achieved consensus around primary methods and domains for assessing neurodevelopmental effects (i.e., benefits and/or difficulties) of pubertal suppression treatment in transgender youth.

17.
PLoS One ; 14(4): e0214945, 2019.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31009465

RESUMEN

Sex differences in episodic memory have been reported. We investigate (1) the existence of sex differences in verbal and other episodic memory tasks in 54 countries, and (2) the association between the time- and country-specific social progress indicators (a) female to male ratio in education and labor force participation, (b) population education and employment, and (c) GDP per capita, and magnitude of sex differences in verbal episodic memory tasks. Data were retrieved from 612 studies, published 1973-2013. Results showed that females outperformed (Cohen's d > 0) males in verbal (42 out of 45 countries) and other (28 out of 45 countries) episodic memory tasks. Although all three social progress indicators were, separately, positively associated with the female advantage in verbal episodic memory performance, only population education and employment remained significant when considering the social indicators together. Results suggest that women's verbal episodic memory performance benefits more than men's from education and employment.


Asunto(s)
Cognición , Memoria Episódica , Caracteres Sexuales , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estudios Retrospectivos , Factores Socioeconómicos
18.
Psychol Bull ; 145(8): 785-821, 2019 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31180695

RESUMEN

To remember what one did yesterday is an example of an everyday episodic memory task, in which a female advantage has sometimes been reported. Here, we quantify the impact of sex on episodic memory performance and investigate whether the magnitude of the sex difference is modified by study-, task-, and sample-specific moderators. Analyses were based on 617 studies conducted between 1973 and 2013 with 1,233,921 participants. A 5-level random-effects meta-analysis showed an overall female advantage in episodic memory (g = 0.19, 95% CI [0.17, 0.21]). The material to be remembered affected the magnitude of this advantage, with a female advantage for more verbal tasks, such as words, sentences, and prose (g = 0.28, 95% CI [0.25, 0.30]), nameable images (g = 0.16, 95% CI [0.11, 0.22]), and locations (g = 0.16, 95% CI [0.11, 0.21]), and a male advantage in more spatial tasks, such as abstract images (g = -0.20, 95% CI [-0.35, -0.05]) and routes (g = -0.24, 95% CI [-0.35, -0.12]). Furthermore, there was a female advantage for materials that cannot easily be placed along the verbal-spatial continuum, such as faces (g = 0.26, 95% CI [0.20, 0.33]), and odor, taste, and color (g = 0.37, 95% CI [0.18, 0.55]). These differences have remained stable since 1973. For verbal episodic memory tasks, differences were larger in Europe, North America, Oceania, and South America than in Asia, and smaller in childhood and old age than for other ages. Taken together, results suggest that men may use their spatial advantage in spatially demanding episodic memory tasks, whereas women do well in episodic memory tasks that are verbalizable and tasks that are neither verbal nor spatial, such as remembering faces and odors/tastes/colors. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Memoria Episódica , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Estadísticos , Caracteres Sexuales , Factores Sexuales
19.
Psychoneuroendocrinology ; 98: 233-241, 2018 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29884451

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Many questions regarding the mechanisms behind sex differences in cognitive abilities are still unanswered. On a group level, men typically outperform women on certain spatial tasks, whereas women perform better on certain tests of memory and verbal ability. The prevailing theories concerning the biological predispositions for these and other differences in behaviour and brain function focus on early and prolonged exposure to sex hormones. There is, however, evidence of direct effects of sex chromosomes on sex-typical behaviour in other species. OBJECTIVES: To study the influence of sex hormones and sex chromosomes on cognition in women with Complete androgen insensitivity (CAIS) and Gonadal dysgenesis (GD). METHODS: Eighteen women with CAIS, 6 women with 46,XYGD, and 7 women with 46,XXGD were compared with age-matched male and female controls on tests of spatial and verbal abilities, memory functions, and emotion recognition. RESULTS: Women with CAIS, XYGD, and XXGD performed similar to female controls on cognitive tasks. However, on a test of emotion recognition, women with XXGD outperformed the other groups, whereas women with CAIS and XYGD performed similar to male controls. CONCLUSION: Our results support theories of androgen effects on cognitive abilities and suggest that factors related to sex chromosomes may influence emotion recognition. Implications of an atypical sex hormone situation and sex chromosome variation are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Síndrome de Resistencia Androgénica/psicología , Cognición/fisiología , Disgenesia Gonadal/psicología , Adulto , Síndrome de Resistencia Androgénica/metabolismo , Andrógenos , Emociones/fisiología , Femenino , Disgenesia Gonadal/metabolismo , Hormonas Esteroides Gonadales/metabolismo , Hormonas Esteroides Gonadales/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Caracteres Sexuales
20.
Menopause ; 14(3 Pt 1): 425-31, 2007.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17279058

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: In a population-based sample of women (45, 50, or 55 years old), behavioral data and blood serum were collected concurrently, enabling us (1) to investigate cognitive differences among premenopausal, perimenopausal, and postmenopausal groups of women and (2) to evaluate the relationship between blood estrogen levels and cognitive performance. DESIGN: Groups of premenopausal (n = 129), perimenopausal (n = 58), and postmenopausal (n = 55) women were tested on tasks assessing episodic and semantic memory, verbal fluency, visuospatial performance, and face recognition. Blood serum was collected concurrently for analyses of estrogen levels. RESULTS: With inclusion of controls for age and education, results showed that there were no differences in cognitive performance among premenopausal, perimenopausal, and postmenopausal groups of women. In addition, there were no associations between blood estrogen levels and cognitive performance. CONCLUSIONS: These results do not support the hypothesis that estrogen or menopausal status affects cognitive performance in middle-aged women.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Cognición , Estrógenos/sangre , Menopausia/sangre , Recuerdo Mental , Factores de Edad , Estudios de Cohortes , Femenino , Hormona Folículo Estimulante/sangre , Humanos , Persona de Mediana Edad , Análisis Multivariante , Salud de la Mujer
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA