Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 34
Filtrar
Más filtros

Banco de datos
País/Región como asunto
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Am J Obstet Gynecol ; 229(3): 294.e1-294.e14, 2023 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36863645

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Preeclampsia, a hypertensive pregnancy disorder, is a leading cause of maternal and fetal morbidity and mortality, with remote cardio- and cerebrovascular implications. After preeclampsia, women may report serious disabling cognitive complaints, especially involving executive function, but the extent and time course of these complaints are unknown. OBJECTIVE: This study aimed to determine the impact of preeclampsia on perceived maternal cognitive functioning decades after pregnancy. STUDY DESIGN: This study is part of a cross-sectional case-control study named Queen of Hearts (ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT02347540), a collaboration study of 5 tertiary referral centers within the Netherlands investigating long-term effects of preeclampsia. Eligible participants were female patients aged ≥18 years after preeclampsia and after normotensive pregnancy between 6 months and 30 years after their first (complicated) pregnancy. Preeclampsia was defined as new-onset hypertension after 20 weeks of gestation along with proteinuria, fetal growth restriction, or other maternal organ dysfunction. Women with a history of hypertension, autoimmune disease, or kidney disease before their first pregnancy were excluded. Attenuation of higher-order cognitive functions, that is, executive function, was measured with the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function for Adults. Crude and covariate-adjusted absolute and relative risks of clinical attenuation over time after (complicated) pregnancy were determined with moderated logistic and log-binomial regression. RESULTS: This study included 1036 women with a history of preeclampsia and 527 women with normotensive pregnancies. Regarding overall executive function, 23.2% (95% confidence interval, 19.0-28.1) of women experienced clinically relevant attenuation after preeclampsia, as opposed to 2.2% (95% confidence interval, 0.8-6.0) of controls immediately after childbirth (adjusted relative risk, 9.20 [95% confidence interval, 3.33-25.38]). Group differences diminished yet remained statistically significant (P < .05) at least 19 years postpartum. Regardless of history of preeclampsia, women with lower educational attainment, mood or anxiety disorders, or obesity were especially at risk. Neither severity of preeclampsia, multiple gestation, method of delivery, preterm birth, nor perinatal death was related to overall executive function. CONCLUSION: After preeclampsia, women were 9 times more likely to experience clinical attenuation of higher-order cognitive functions as opposed to after normotensive pregnancy. Despite overall steady improvement, elevated risks persisted over decades after childbirth.


Asunto(s)
Hipertensión , Preeclampsia , Nacimiento Prematuro , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Recién Nacido , Masculino , Embarazo , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Cognición , Estudios Transversales , Preeclampsia/epidemiología , Adolescente
2.
Eur Psychiatry ; 67(1): e18, 2024 Feb 14.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38351594

RESUMEN

Adults with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) often struggle with emotion regulation (ER), impacting their empathic skills and relationships. ADHD medication might not be as effective for ER issues as for ADHD symptoms. Microdosing (MD) psychedelics has shown promise for ADHD treatment and previous studies reported social-emotional benefits. Two online prospective studies investigated MD effects on ER and empathy in adults with severe ADHD symptoms across three assessments: baseline, two-, and four-week post-initiation. Study 1 examined adults initiating MD on their own (n = 233, n = 64, and n = 44) and found positive effects on ER (cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression) and aspects of empathy (perspective-taking and personal distress). Study 2, including a control group and an ADHD symptom scale, compared individuals only MD (n = 180, n = 50, and n = 38) to individuals using conventional ADHD medication (n = 37, n = 27, and n = 28). After 4 weeks, ADHD symptoms were lower in the MD group. Only improvements in expressive suppression persisted after adding the control group. This study indicates the positive effects of MD psychedelics on ADHD symptoms and ER in adults with severe ADHD symptoms while lacking evidence for effects on empathy.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad , Regulación Emocional , Alucinógenos , Adulto , Humanos , Empatía , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/psicología , Estudios Prospectivos , Alucinógenos/uso terapéutico
3.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol ; : 1-17, 2024 Jun 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38836516

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION: Design fluency (DF) tasks are commonly used to assess executive functions such as attentional control, cognitive flexibility, self-monitoring and strategy use. Next to the total number of correct designs, the standard outcome of a DF task, clustering and switching can help disentangle the processes underlying DF performance. We present the first longitudinal study of 4-8-year-old children's developmental DF trajectories. METHOD: At initial enrollment, children (n = 228) were aged between 4.05 and 6.88 years (M = 5.18, SD = 0.59) and attended Dutch primary schools. The DF task was administered at three time points, each time point separated by approximately 1 year. Data were analyzed using mixed regression for total number of correct designs and switching, and mixed logistic regression analysis for clustering. RESULTS: The total number of correct designs increased linearly across the three time points. Across all time points, children made very few clusters, and most clusters consisted of only 3 designs. Clustering only increased at the third assessment compared to the two previous assessments. Switching increased up to the second assessment, but not after that. The number of switches was highly correlated with the total number of correct designs at all time points (r = 0.78 to r = 0.85). These developmental trajectories were similar for all children regardless of their baseline age. Normative data are given for the total number of correct designs and switching. CONCLUSIONS: Children as of age 4 onwards can perform a DF task. For children as young as 4-8 years old, computing clustering, and switching measures is of limited value to study cognitive processes underlying DF performance, next to the total number of correct designs. There were no sex differences on any of the DF outcomes. Level of parental education (LPE) was positively associated with the total number of correct designs and switching.

4.
BMJ Open ; 14(3): e077534, 2024 Mar 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38443087

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION: Pre-eclampsia is a hypertensive disorder affecting up to 8% of pregnancies. After pre-eclampsia, women are at increased risk of cognitive problems, and cerebrovascular and cardiovascular disorders. These sequelae could result from microvascular dysfunction persisting after pre-eclampsia. This study will explore differences in cerebral and myocardial microvascular function between women after pre-eclampsia and women after normotensive gestation. We hypothesise that pre-eclampsia alters cerebral and myocardial microvascular functions, which in turn are related to diminished cognitive and cardiac performance. METHODS AND ANALYSIS: The cross-sectional 'DEcreased Cognitive functiON, NEurovascular CorrelaTes and myocardial changes in women with a history of pre-eclampsia' (DECONNECT) pilot study includes women after pre-eclampsia and controls after normotensive pregnancy between 6 months and 20 years after gestation. We recruit women from the Queen of Hearts study, a study investigating subclinical heart failure after pre-eclampsia. Neuropsychological tests are employed to assess different cognitive domains, including attention, processing speed, and cognitive control. Cerebral images are recorded using a 7 Tesla MRI to assess blood-brain barrier integrity, perfusion, blood flow, functional and structural networks, and anatomical dimensions. Cardiac images are recorded using a 3 Tesla MRI to assess cardiac perfusion, strain, dimensions, mass, and degree of fibrosis. We assess the effect of a history of pre-eclampsia using multivariable regression analyses. ETHICS AND DISSEMINATION: This study is approved by the Ethics Committee of Maastricht University Medical Centre (METC azM/UM, NL47252.068.14). Knowledge dissemination will include scientific publications, presentations at conferences and public forums, and social media. TRIAL REGISTRATION NUMBER: NCT02347540.


Asunto(s)
Preeclampsia , Femenino , Humanos , Embarazo , Cognición , Estudios Transversales , Miocardio , Proyectos Piloto
5.
Child Neuropsychol ; : 1-12, 2023 Jun 22.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37345982

RESUMEN

The triple pathway model suggests that different neuropsychological factors underlie symptoms of inattention (i.e., time, cognition and/or motivation problems). However, screening instruments asking individuals to judge the link between these neuropsychological factors and inattention are lacking. The recently developed screening questionnaire, PASSC, aims to examine these factors possibly causing inattention by asking parents to indicate to what extent their child experiences inattention symptoms and to what extent different neuropsychological factors explain this inattention. The present study extends prior validation research of the PASSC by examining associations between PASSC inattention explained by time, cognition, and/or motivation and children's performance on tests measuring these same three constructs. Results indicated positive correlations between PASSC inattention explained by time and less accurate performance on a time discrimination test, and between PASSC inattention explained by cognition and more working memory errors as well as higher attention switching costs. Furthermore, children whose parents indicated that their inattention was best explained by cognition showed higher switching costs than children whose inattention was best explained by motivation. This support for construct validity of the PASSC is limited to two PASSC explanations (i.e., time, cognition) and a subset of tests (i.e., time discrimination, attention switching, memory span). Future research should focus on integrating PASSC and performance test results to differentiate between children with attention problems but different underlying neuropsychological problems. Concluding, the PASSC can be a promising screening tool to identify inattention in children and the underlying explanation indicated by parents.

6.
Front Psychiatry ; 14: 1233585, 2023.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37915796

RESUMEN

Background: Microdosing (MD), repeatedly taking psychedelics in small, non-hallucinogenic amounts, has been practiced by individuals to relieve attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms. Generally, adults diagnosed with ADHD have lower levels of mindfulness and differ in personality structure from non-ADHD adults. How MD affects mindfulness and personality in adults with ADHD remains unexplored. Aim: This study aimed to investigate the effects of 4 weeks of MD on mindfulness and personality traits in adults diagnosed with ADHD and those experiencing severe ADHD symptoms. It was expected that mindfulness and the personality traits conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness, and openness would increase and neuroticism would decrease after 4 weeks of MD compared to baseline. It was explored if using conventional ADHD medication alongside MD and/or having comorbidities influenced MD-induced effects. Methods: An online prospective naturalistic design was used to measure participants before MD initiation and 2 and 4 weeks later. Validated self-report measures were used assessing mindfulness (15-item Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire) and personality traits (10-item version of the Big Five Inventory) at three time points. Results: The sample included n = 233, n = 66, and n = 44 participants at the three time points, respectively. Trait mindfulness, specifically description and non-judging of inner experience, was increased, and neuroticism was decreased after 4 weeks of MD compared to baseline. The remaining personality traits remained unchanged. Using conventional medication and/or having comorbid diagnoses did not change the MD-induced effects on mindfulness and personality traits after 4 weeks. Conclusion: MD induced changes in otherwise stable traits. Future placebo-controlled studies are warranted to confirm whether these changes occur in a controlled setting.

7.
Brain Sci ; 12(11)2022 Nov 14.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36421868

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Intelligence scores in males with Duchenne Muscular Dystrophy (DMD) and Becker Muscular Dystrophy (BMD) remain a major issue in clinical practice. We performed a literature review and meta-analysis to further delineate the intellectual functioning of dystrophinopathies. METHOD: Published, peer-reviewed articles assessing intelligence, using Wechsler Scales, of males with DMD or BMD were searched from 1960 to 2022. Meta-analysis with random-effects models was conducted, assessing weighted, mean effect sizes of full-scale IQ (FSIQ) scores relative to normative data (Mean = 100, Standard Deviation = 15). Post hoc we analysed differences between performance and verbal intelligence scores. RESULTS: 43 studies were included, reporting data on 1472 males with dystrophinopathies; with FSIQ scores available for 1234 DMD (k = 32) and 101 BMD (k = 7). DMD males score, on average, one standard deviation below average (FSIQ = 84.76) and significantly lower than BMD (FSIQ = 92.11). Compared to a previous meta-analysis published in 2001, we find, on average, significantly higher FSIQ scores in DMD. CONCLUSION: Males with Duchenne have, on average, significantly lower FSIQ scores than BMD males and the general population. Clinicians must consider lower intelligence in dystrophinopathies to ensure good clinical practice.

8.
Brain Sci ; 12(10)2022 Oct 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36291303

RESUMEN

This study aims to investigate distractibility quantified by recording and analyzing eye movements during task-irrelevant distraction in children with and without ADHD and in children with and without neurological disorders. Gaze behavior data and press latencies of 141 participants aged 6−17 that were collected during a computerized distraction paradigm with task-irrelevant stimuli (IDistrack) were analyzed. Children using attention-regulating medication were excluded from participation. Data were analyzed for subgroups that were formed based on the presence of neurological disorders and the presence of ADHD separately. Participants with ADHD and participants with neurological disorders spent less time fixating on the target stimuli compared to their peers without ADHD (p = 0.025) or their peers without neurological disorders (p < 0.001). Participants with and without ADHD had equal press latencies (p = 0.79). Participants with neurological disorders had a greater press latency compared to their typically developing peers (p < 0.001). Target fixation duration shows a significant association with parent-reported attention problems (r = −0.39, p < 0.001). We conclude that eye tracking during a distraction task reveals potentially valid clinical information that may contribute to the assessment of dysfunctional attentional processes. Further research on the validity and reliability of this paradigm is recommended.

9.
Laterality ; 16(2): 207-26, 2011 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20509093

RESUMEN

A behaviour-based lateral preference instrument (the Lateral Preferences Questionnaire; LPQ; Van Strien, 1992, 2001) was administered to a large sample of school-aged children. The aims of the present study were twofold: (i) to evaluate the factor structure and the psychometric properties of the LPQ, and (ii) to evaluate the effects of age, gender, and mean level of parental education on lateral preferences and lateral consistency. Two factor models had an excellent fit with the data. In the first model the LPQ items were considered to be indicators of four different lateral preference factors (the hand, foot, eye, and ear preference factors). In the second model the LPQ items were considered to be indicators of four lateral preference factors, which were in turn expected to load on a single underlying general lateral preference factor. The psychometric properties of the derived hand and eye preference scales of the LPQ were good to excellent, and the psychometric properties of the foot and ear preference scales were acceptable. Lateral preferences and lateral consistency were not affected by age, gender, or mean level of parental education.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/fisiología , Dominancia Cerebral/fisiología , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Destreza Motora/fisiología , Adolescente , Envejecimiento/psicología , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
10.
Child Neuropsychol ; 27(8): 1117-1132, 2021 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34114931

RESUMEN

The present study examined psychometric properties of a recently developed parent report screening questionnaire, i.e., Parent ADHD Screening questionnaire: Signaling the Core explanation underlying behavioral symptoms (PASSC). The PASSC aims to measure (1) ADHD symptoms and (2) what parents view to be the main underlying explanation(s) of these symptoms. The PASSC questions 3 (potential) underlying explanations based on the triple pathway model (TPM): i.e., time, cognition and/or motivation problems. Parents of 1166 Dutch children aged 4-12 filled in the PASSC, as well as 2 questionnaires measuring time, cognition and motivation (i.e., the FTF and the SPSRQ-C). Reliability of the PASSC is good, indicated by high internal consistency of the sumscores. Principal component analyses supported the distinction between inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity symptoms as defined in the DSM-5, and the distinction between the 3 TPM explanations given by parents for inattention, but not for hyperactivity-impulsivity symptoms. The majority of parents selected one and the same explanation for inattention problems of their child, most often being cognition (31.2%) and motivation (28.2%). PASSC validity was further supported by positive associations between the explanation sumscores for inattention symptoms and other parent questionnaires measuring the same constructs (i.e., time, cognition and motivation; convergent validity), although we found no evidence for discriminant validity. Groups (based on age group, sex and ADHD diagnosis) differed on the PASSC sumscores in the expected directions. Concluding, the PASSC is a promising tool to assess a child's ADHD symptoms as well as the parent view on (potential) explanation(s) of inattention.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/diagnóstico , Niño , Cognición , Humanos , Psicometría , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Instituciones Académicas , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
11.
Child Neuropsychol ; 26(2): 189-218, 2020 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31328631

RESUMEN

The study aims to establish demographically corrected, pediatric norms for the computerized Delayed Matching to Sample (DMS) test, a measure of "visual matching ability and short-term visual recognition memory, for non-verbalisable problems". The DMS was administered to n = 184 children aged 5.10 to 14.5 years old. The DMS is a 4-choice recognition task of non-verbal, abstract patterns. The child has "to select, among four different choice patterns, the one that matches a complex visual pattern presented," i.e., (the target stimulus). The DMS consists of two conditions: a) the overt condition in which the target stimulus and four choice patterns are shown simultaneously and b) the covert condition, in which the choice patterns are shown after the target pattern is covered. The DMS test provides three outcome measures: the accuracy score (i.e., the number of correct patterns selected), latency (i.e., the response speed) and the probability of making an error after an incorrect response. These outcome measures were calculated for both conditions and for both conditions combined. Results showed that demographic variables, such as age, sex, and/or level of parental education (LPE) affected scores on these outcome measures. Based on these data, demographically corrected norms were established for all outcome measures, per condition and for both conditions combined.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Cognición/fisiología , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Adolescente , Niño , Preescolar , Demografía , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Evaluación de Resultado en la Atención de Salud , Tiempo de Reacción
12.
Neuropsychologia ; 124: 236-245, 2019 02 18.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30528585

RESUMEN

Childhood and adolescence represent sensitive developmental periods for brain networks implicated in a range of complex skills, including executive functions (EF; inhibitory control, working memory, and cognitive flexibility). As a consequence, these skills may be particularly vulnerable to injuries sustained during these sensitive developmental periods. The present study investigated 1) whether age at injury differentially affects EF 6 months and 2 years after TBI in children aged 5-15 years, and 2) whether the association between brain lesions and EF depend on age at injury. Children with TBI (n = 105) were categorized into four age-at-injury groups based on previous studies and proposed timing of cerebral maturational spurts: early childhood (5-6 years, n = 14), middle childhood (7-9 years, n = 24), late childhood (10-12 years, n = 52), and adolescence (13-15 years, n = 15). EF were assessed with performance-based tasks and a parent-report of everyday EF. TBI patients' EF scores 6 months and 2 years post-injury were compared to those of typically developing (TD) controls (n = 42). Brain lesions were identified using susceptibility weighted imaging (SWI). Results indicated that inhibitory control performance 2 years post-injury was differentially affected by the impact of TBI depending on age at injury. Follow-up analyses did not reveal significant differences within the age groups, preventing drawing strong conclusions regarding the contribution of age at injury to EF outcome after TBI. Tentatively, large effect sizes suggest that vulnerability is most apparent in early childhood and adolescence. Everyday inhibitory control behaviour was worse for children with TBI than TD children across childhood and adolescence at the 2-year assessment. There was no evidence for impairment in working memory or cognitive flexibility after TBI at the group level. Given small group sizes, findings from analyses into correlations between EF and SWI lesions should be interpreted with caution. Extent, number and volume of brain lesions correlated with adolescent everyday EF behaviour 6 months post-injury. Taken together, the results emphasize the need for long-term follow-up after paediatric TBI during sensitive developmental periods given negative outcomes 2-year post injury. Inhibitory control seems to be particular vulnerable to the impact of TBI. Findings of associations between EF and SWI lesions need to be replicated with larger samples.


Asunto(s)
Lesiones Traumáticas del Encéfalo/patología , Lesiones Traumáticas del Encéfalo/psicología , Desarrollo Infantil , Función Ejecutiva , Adolescente , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador/métodos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Estudios Prospectivos
13.
Neuropsychology ; 22(1): 74-84, 2008 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18211157

RESUMEN

In this study, the authors aimed to examine 4 domains of executive functioning in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)--namely interference control, concept shifting, verbal fluency, and verbal working memory. Four groups of participants were included: (a) adults diagnosed with ADHD (ADHD-super(-); n = 20), (b) adults diagnosed with both ADHD and 1 or more comorbid disorder(s) (ADHD-super(+); n = 22), (c) adults referred for ADHD because of ADHD symptomatology but not diagnosed as such (non-ADHD; n = 34), and (d) healthy controls (n = 136). ADHD-related deficits (independent of comorbidity) were revealed for concept shifting and verbal working memory. In addition, the ADHD-super(+) and non-ADHD groups displayed deficits in terms of general processing speed. Given that these deficits were not found in the ADHD-super(-) group, the authors contend that these deficits are likely attributable to comorbidity rather than ADHD itself. Contrary to the authors' expectations, these findings do not correspond with the cognitive subtype hypothesis.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/fisiopatología , Inhibición Psicológica , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Procesos Mentales/fisiología , Aprendizaje Verbal/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Solución de Problemas , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología
14.
Child Neuropsychol ; 14(3): 195-210, 2008 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17852129

RESUMEN

The development of three aspects of selective attention was studied in 451 Dutch schoolchildren attending second to sixth grade. Selective attention was measured with the d2 Test of attention. The largest age differences were found for processing speed that continued to improve until the sixth grade. Impulsivity, as measured by the percentage of errors of commission, decreased until the fourth grade. Inattention, measured by the percentage of errors of omission, was stable in all grades. Processing speed and impulsivity were correlated with the score on the Attention Problems subscale of the Child Behavior Checklist. These results imply that selective attention continues to develop, at least, until the end of elementary school. The findings are support for a step-wise model of cognitive development (P. Anderson, 2002).


Asunto(s)
Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/epidemiología , Atención , Trastornos del Conocimiento/epidemiología , Conducta Impulsiva/epidemiología , Adolescente , Distribución por Edad , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/psicología , Niño , Conducta Infantil/psicología , Trastornos del Conocimiento/psicología , Estudios Transversales , Femenino , Humanos , Conducta Impulsiva/psicología , Masculino , Países Bajos/epidemiología , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas , Factores de Tiempo
15.
J Atten Disord ; 11(6): 664-76, 2008 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17712171

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: To examine the specificity of deficits in focused attention and sustained attention in adults with ADHD and to evaluate the effect of comorbidity. METHOD: Twenty-eight adults with ADHD without comorbidity were compared with 28 ADHD outpatients with comorbidity. Two control groups were used: 68 adults referred for ADHD but with another psychopathology rather than ADHD (non-ADHD) and 28 healthy controls. All participants completed attention tests of the Amsterdam Neuropsychological Tasks program. RESULTS: Both ADHD groups demonstrated a sustained attention deficit relative to the control groups, as indicated by a disproportionate deterioration of speed fluctuation with time-on-task reflecting temporal lapses in attention. Only the ADHD+ group showed focused attention deficits in that they were less able to ignore irrelevant information. CONCLUSION: These findings show that adults with ADHD have specific deficits in sustained attention. Additional deficits in focused attention are confined to outpatients with ADHD and comorbidity.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/diagnóstico , Atención/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción , Adulto , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/tratamiento farmacológico , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/epidemiología , Estimulantes del Sistema Nervioso Central/uso terapéutico , Trastornos del Conocimiento/diagnóstico , Trastornos del Conocimiento/epidemiología , Femenino , Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Índice de Severidad de la Enfermedad
16.
Dev Neurorehabil ; 21(2): 101-107, 2018 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28152329

RESUMEN

The assessment of intellectual abilities is intensive, time-consuming, and might be considered burdensome for patients. We examined psychometric qualities of short forms (SFs) of the Wechsler Intelligence Scales for Children (WISC-third edition) and for adults (WAIS-fourth edition), in children (n = 986; Mage = 10.9) and adults (n = 324; Mage = 40.9) with neurological disorders. SF estimates were compared with Full Scale IQ (FSIQ), obtained by a complete administration, for the entire sample and for the subgroups FSIQ < 80 and FSIQ ≥ 80. The FSIQ was correctly identified within ± 7 points in 86% of children and 87% of adults. There were, however, some differences regarding the optimal SF subtest combination between subgroups. Although clinical inferences should not be made, SFs may be useful in research settings to obtain a global estimate of intelligence, and in clinical settings to screen periodically for possible intellectual deterioration.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedades del Sistema Nervioso/diagnóstico , Escalas de Wechsler/normas , Adolescente , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Inteligencia , Masculino , Psicometría/normas , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados
17.
Acta Psychol (Amst) ; 179: 78-88, 2017 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28738243

RESUMEN

We investigated two components of proactive cognitive control, response facilitation and response inhibition, in an adult lifespan sample (N=544, age range=18-91years) by administering two response-preparation tasks: a procue task, primarily involving facilitation, and an anticue task, involving both facilitation and inhibition. Cues in both tasks corresponded with the index and middle fingers of either the left or right hand. After a random preparation interval (PI) of 100-850ms following the onset of the cue signal, a single-target stimulus indicated the required response. Where procues were spatially aligned with the two fingers of the responding hand, anticues consistently indicated the two fingers of the opposite hand, requiring a remapping of cue location and response hand. This remapping requires inhibition to suppress the automatic activation of the ipsilateral responses. Previous research revealed typical reaction time (RT) profiles for procues and anticues as a function of PI. Whereas procues generate RT benefits (relative to a neutral-cue condition) already at short PIs, which increase with longer PIs, anticues generate RT costs at short PIs and RT benefits at longer PIs. Our results showed that, in the anticue task, older participants needed more preparation time to turn RT costs into RT benefits than younger participants, revealing an age-related deficit of response inhibition. Moreover, in both tasks, older participants were less able to increase RT benefits with longer PIs, revealing a deficit of response facilitation. We conclude that both facilitatory and inhibitory impairments contribute to age-related deficiencies in proactive cognitive control.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/fisiología , Cognición/fisiología , Inhibición Psicológica , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Señales (Psicología) , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Pruebas Psicológicas , Adulto Joven
18.
Eur J Paediatr Neurol ; 21(2): 336-343, 2017 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27707654

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: The aim of the current study was to investigate whether total intelligence scores (FSIQ) and/or a discrepancy in intelligence can predict behavioral or emotional problems in children with neurological deficiencies. METHOD: The population consists of children with neurological deficiencies (N = 610, ranging from 6 to 17 years), referred due to concerns on the (educational) development of the child to a tertiary outpatient clinic. All children were tested with the Dutch Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children - third edition (WISC-III-NL). A VIQ-PIQ discrepancy score was calculated by subtracting the performance capacities of the verbal capacities. The effects of demographic variables, FSIQ, and the VIQ-PIQ discrepancy on two parent-rated questionnaires measuring behavior and emotions in children were analyzed with linear and logistic regression models. RESULTS AND CONCLUSION: The VIQ-PIQ discrepancy was not predictive of behavioral or emotional problems recorded on the above-mentioned parent-rated questionnaires. The FSIQ score, age, and sex were predictive to some extent: increases in age and FSIQ led to a decrease of reported problems, and boys showed more problems than girls. Children with neurological deficiencies had on average significantly higher verbal capacities than performance capacities, in line with the neuropsychological principle that language survives brain damage whereas performance capacities are more affected.


Asunto(s)
Síntomas Afectivos/complicaciones , Síntomas Afectivos/psicología , Pruebas de Inteligencia , Enfermedades del Sistema Nervioso/complicaciones , Enfermedades del Sistema Nervioso/psicología , Padres , Problema de Conducta/psicología , Adolescente , Envejecimiento/psicología , Niño , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Valor Predictivo de las Pruebas , Caracteres Sexuales , Escalas de Wechsler
19.
Child Neuropsychol ; 22(7): 818-36, 2016.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26095321

RESUMEN

This study examines inter-individual differences in how presentation modality affects verbal learning performance. Children aged 5 to 16 performed a verbal learning test within one of three presentation modalities: pictorial, auditory, or textual. The results indicated that a beneficial effect of pictures exists over auditory and textual presentation modalities and that this effect increases with age. However, this effect is only found if the information to be learned is presented once (or at most twice) and only in children above the age of 7. The results may be explained in terms of single or dual coding of information in which the phonological loop is involved. Development of the (sub)vocal rehearsal system in the phonological loop is believed to be a gradual process that begins developing around the age of 7. The developmental trajectories are similar for boys and girls. Additionally, auditory information and textual information both seemed to be processed in a similar manner, namely without labeling or recoding, leading to single coding. In contrast, pictures are assumed to be processed by the dual coding of both the visual information and a (verbal) labeling of the pictures.


Asunto(s)
Desarrollo del Adolescente/fisiología , Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Desarrollo Infantil/fisiología , Individualidad , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Lectura , Aprendizaje Verbal/fisiología , Adolescente , Niño , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
20.
Dev Psychol ; 52(6): 960-71, 2016 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27124653

RESUMEN

We investigated the effects of age on proactive and reactive cognitive control in a large population sample of 809 individuals, ranging in age between 5 and 97 years. For that purpose, we used an anticue paradigm, which required a consistent remapping of cue location and response hand: Left-sided cues required right-hand responses and vice versa. After a random preparation interval of 100-850 ms, these anticues were followed by a target stimulus, which prompted a response with the index or middle finger of 1 of 2 hands. A neutral control condition involved uninformative cues, indicating all 4 possible response locations. The primary outcome measure was the difference between neutral and anticue reaction time (RT). Negative values indicated RT costs of the anticue, relative to the neutral condition, reflecting reactive cognitive control. Positive values indicated RT benefits, reflecting proactive cognitive control. Results were twofold. First, the switch from RT costs to benefits took place at longer preparation intervals in the youngest and oldest age groups than in the intermediate age groups. Second, irrespective of preparation interval, anticue performance followed an inverted U-shaped trajectory as a function of age, with a relatively steep improvement during childhood and adolescence, relative stability between 26 and 60 years, and a slightly accelerating decline into old age. Both patterns of results suggest an age-related transition from a primarily reactive, to a primarily proactive mode of cognitive control in early life and back again from a primarily proactive, to a primarily reactive mode of control in later life. (PsycINFO Database Record


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/psicología , Dedos , Lateralidad Funcional , Inhibición Psicológica , Tiempo de Reacción , Adolescente , Adulto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Envejecimiento/fisiología , Niño , Preescolar , Cognición/fisiología , Señales (Psicología) , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Femenino , Dedos/fisiología , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Pruebas Psicológicas , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Adulto Joven
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA