Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 33
Filtrar
Más filtros

Banco de datos
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
J Youth Adolesc ; 49(7): 1481-1491, 2020 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31732836

RESUMEN

Previous studies have shown that status goals motivate direct forms of interpersonal aggression. However, status goals have been studied mostly in isolation from affection goals. It is theorized that the means by which status and affection goals are satisfied change during adolescence, which can affect aggression. This is tested in a pooled sample of (pre)adolescents (N = 1536; 49% girls; ages 10-15), by examining associations between status goals and direct aggression and the moderating role of affection goals. As hypothesized, with increasing age, status goals were more strongly associated with direct aggression. Moreover, for older adolescents, status goals were only associated with aggression when affection goals were weak. These findings support the changing relationship between status goals and direct aggression during adolescence.


Asunto(s)
Conducta del Adolescente/psicología , Agresión/psicología , Objetivos , Autoimagen , Adolescente , Niño , Víctimas de Crimen/psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Relaciones Interpersonales , Masculino , Grupo Paritario , Deseabilidad Social
2.
Behav Brain Sci ; 40: e86, 2017 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29342545

RESUMEN

We comment on the proposition "that lower temperatures and especially greater seasonal variation in temperature call for individuals and societies to adopt … a greater degree of self-control" (Van Lange et al., sect. 3, para. 4) for which we cannot find empirical support in a large data set with data-driven analyses. After providing greater nuance in our theoretical review, we suggest that Van Lange et al. revisit their model with an eye toward the social determinants of self-control.


Asunto(s)
Agresión , Autocontrol , Animales , Clima , Humanos , Spheniscidae , Violencia
3.
J Adolesc ; 45: 1-10, 2015 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26349449

RESUMEN

This study investigates the types and behavioral associations of peer status in school-bound young adults in the Netherlands. We argue that adolescent peer popularity and its link with aggressive and norm-breaking behavior result from adolescents' desire to create an image of maturity among their peers. We expect that in young adults who are approaching working life, peer status is defined by affective measures of status and prosociality rather than adverse behaviors. Analyses revealed a three cluster solution of (1) liked, (2) liked-popular and (3) neutral members of the peer group, showing that status is primarily defined by being well-liked, though popularity remains relevant. Status was primarily associated with prosocial behavior, especially for females. Peer status in young males remained associated with overt aggressive behavior.


Asunto(s)
Conducta del Adolescente/psicología , Relaciones Interpersonales , Grupo Paritario , Adolescente , Agresión/psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Países Bajos , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
4.
Behav Brain Sci ; 38: e104, 2015.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26786163

RESUMEN

In their target article, Kalisch et al. explicate an appraisal-based model to explain how people bounce back from stress. We posit that for their model, it is crucial to understand the begin-state φ (the "self") - a state that is shaped by early social thermoregulation and through the social network.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Psicológica , Medio Social , Humanos , Apoyo Social
5.
J Interpers Violence ; 38(1-2): NP772-NP795, 2023 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35343282

RESUMEN

Status and affection are both goals related to social needs. The imbalanced needs theory of aggression proposes that although aggression can be used to realize status, this strategy is detrimental for realizing affection in the same social context. Thus, to the degree that the social circles overlap in which status and affection needs are realized, it becomes more costly (in terms of affection) to achieve status via aggression. This theory was tested for different forms of aggression, in different contexts, in a sample of adults from the general population (N = 253, M age = 29.95, SD = 2.60, 78% female). Participants reported on social needs with the Interpersonal Goals Inventory and reported on general measures of physical and social aggression, as well as rule breaking, and aggression at the workplace and in intimate partner relationships. As hypothesized, status needs were associated with physical aggression when affection needs were weak. This interaction, though to a lesser degree, also extended to social forms of aggression and rule breaking. At the workplace, aggression was only related to weak affection needs, whereas aggression in intimate partner relationships was, as expected, unrelated to both social needs. Together, these findings support the results of an earlier test of the imbalanced needs theory of aggression in adolescence, and encourage more research into the link between aggression and the satisfaction of social needs.


Asunto(s)
Agresión , Relaciones Interpersonales , Adulto , Adolescente , Humanos , Femenino , Masculino , Parejas Sexuales , Medio Social
6.
Aggress Behav ; 37(5): 417-29, 2011.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21688275

RESUMEN

The role of physical and relational aggression in adolescents' friendship selection was examined in a longitudinal sample of 274 Chilean students from 5th and 6th grade followed over 1 year. Longitudinal social network modeling (SIENA) was used to study selection processes for aggression while influence processes were controlled for. Furthermore, the effects of network characteristics (i.e., reciprocity and transitivity), gender, and social status on friendship selection were examined. The starting assumption of this study was that selection effects based on aggression might have been overestimated in previous research as a result of failing to consider influence processes and alternative characteristics that steer friendship formation. The results show that selection effects of both physical and relational aggression disappeared when network effects, gender, and social status were taken into account. Particularly gender and perceived popularity appeared to be far more important determinants of friendship selection over time than aggression. Moreover, a peer influence effect was only found for relational aggression, and not for physical aggression. These findings suggest that similarity in aggression among befriended adolescents can be considered to be mainly a by-product rather than a leading dimension in friendship selection.


Asunto(s)
Agresión/psicología , Amigos/psicología , Relaciones Interpersonales , Caracteres Sexuales , Apoyo Social , Adolescente , Conducta del Adolescente/psicología , Niño , Chile , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Personalidad , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
7.
Child Dev ; 81(2): 480-6, 2010.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20438454

RESUMEN

To understand the complex nature of bullies' acceptance and rejection, this article considered goal-framing effects of status and affection as they relate to the gender of the bully (male vs. female bullies), the target (male vs. female victims), and the evaluator (acceptance and rejection from male vs. female classmates). The hypotheses were tested with data from a social network questionnaire conducted in 26 elementary school classes (N = 481 children; M(age) = 10.5 years). The findings revealed that bullies were only rejected by those for whom they were a potential threat and that bullies generally chose their victims so as to minimize loss of affection by choosing victims who were not likely to be defended by significant others.


Asunto(s)
Víctimas de Crimen/psicología , Identidad de Género , Rechazo en Psicología , Conducta Social , Identificación Social , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Grupo Paritario , Psicometría , Deseabilidad Social , Técnicas Sociométricas , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
8.
Br J Soc Psychol ; 49(Pt 1): 175-87, 2010 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19397843

RESUMEN

In two studies we show that people make environments norm-relevant and this increases the likelihood that environments influence norm-relevant judgments. When people see environments without having people on their mind, this effect does not occur. Specifically, when exposed to an environment (a library), people's perceived importance of environment-relevant norms (be silent in libraries) increases, when the concept of 'people' is primed compared to when this is not the case. The impact on normative judgments of priming significant others (Study 1) is stronger than priming people in general (Study 2). Additional effects on conformism and public self-consciousness are discussed, as well as implications for future studies.


Asunto(s)
Relaciones Interpersonales , Juicio , Autoimagen , Conducta Social , Percepción Social , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Medio Social , Adulto Joven
9.
Dev Psychol ; 45(2): 419-30, 2009 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19271828

RESUMEN

This study examined the possible risk-buffering and risk-enhancing role of family characteristics on the association between temperament and early adolescent externalizing and internalizing problems, adjusted for familial vulnerability for psychopathology and early childhood problem behavior. Furthermore, it explored whether these effects were specific or conditional for either internalizing or externalizing problems or more generic for psychopathology. Data on temperament (frustration and fearfulness) and family characteristics (overprotection, rejection, emotional warmth, and socioeconomic status) came from a large longitudinal Dutch population sample of early adolescents (n = 2,149; M age = 13.55 years; 51.2% girls). Hypotheses on the direction and the specificity of the effects were derived from a goal-framing approach. The findings indicate that family characteristics can either buffer or enhance the temperamental risk in the development of psychopathology. Analyses on the direction of these effects resulted in a descriptive classification of domain-specific, conditional, and generic factors that promote or protect the development of psychopathology. Implications of the results are discussed, and directions for future research are given.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos de la Conducta Infantil/psicología , Control Interno-Externo , Responsabilidad Parental/psicología , Desarrollo de la Personalidad , Medio Social , Temperamento , Adolescente , Niño , Estudios de Cohortes , Femenino , Humanos , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Países Bajos , Estudios Prospectivos , Psicopatología , Factores de Riesgo , Socialización , Factores Socioeconómicos
10.
Aggress Behav ; 35(1): 57-67, 2009.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18925635

RESUMEN

The literature suggests that status goals are one of the driving motivations behind bullying behavior, yet this conjecture has rarely if ever been examined empirically. This study assessed status goals in three ways, using dyadic network analysis to analyze the relations and goals among 10-11 and 14-15 year olds in 22 school classes (N boys=225; N girls=277). As a validation bullies were contrasted with victims. Bullies had direct status goals (measured with the Interpersonal Goal Inventory for Children) and showed dominance as measured with proactive aggression. Moreover, as predicted from a goal perspective, bullying behavior was related to prestige in terms of perceived popularity. In contrast, victims lacked status goals, were only reactively aggressive, and low on prestige. That being popular is not the same as being liked could be shown by the fact that bullies were just as rejected as victims by their classmates. Eighth-grade bullies had more direct status goals than fourth-grade bullies, possibly indicating that striving for the popularity component of status increases in early adolescence.


Asunto(s)
Agresión/psicología , Inventario de Personalidad , Conducta Social , Clase Social , Percepción Social , Adolescente , Niño , Víctimas de Crimen/psicología , Femenino , Finlandia , Humanos , Relaciones Interpersonales , Masculino , Grupo Paritario , Población Urbana
11.
Sci Data ; 6(1): 32, 2019 04 17.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30996323

RESUMEN

In the Human Penguin Project (N = 1755), 15 research groups from 12 countries collected body temperature, demographic variables, social network indices, seven widely-used psychological scales and two newly developed questionnaires (the Social Thermoregulation and Risk Avoidance Questionnaire (STRAQ-1) and the Kama Muta Frequency Scale (KAMF)). They were collected to investigate the relationship between environmental factors (e.g., geographical, climate etc.) and human behaviors, which is a long-standing inquiry in the scientific community. More specifically, the present project was designed to test principles surrounding the idea of social thermoregulation, which posits that social networks help people to regulate their core body temperature. The results showed that all scales in the current project have sufficient to good psychometrical properties. Unlike previous crowdsourced projects, this dataset includes not only the cleaned raw data but also all the validation of questionnaires in 9 different languages, thus providing a valuable resource for psychological scientists who are interested in cross-national, environment-human interaction studies.


Asunto(s)
Regulación de la Temperatura Corporal , Medio Social , Temperatura Corporal , Regulación de la Temperatura Corporal/fisiología , Clima , Demografía , Humanos , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
12.
J Abnorm Child Psychol ; 36(8): 1289-99, 2008 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18607717

RESUMEN

This study examined to what extent bullying behavior of popular adolescents is responsible for whether bullying is more or less likely to be accepted or rejected by peers (popularity-norm effect) rather than the behavior of all peers (class norm). Specifically, the mean level of bullying by the whole class (class norm) was split into behavior of popular adolescents (popularity-norm) and behavior of non-popular adolescents (non-popularity-norm), and examined in its interaction with individual bullying on peer acceptance and peer rejection. The data stem from a peer-nominations subsample of TRAILS, a large population-based sample of adolescent boys and girls (N = 3,312). The findings of multilevel regression analyses demonstrated that the negative impact of individual bullying on peer acceptance and the positive impact on peer rejection were particularly weakened by bullying by popular adolescents. These results place the class-norm effects found in previous person-group dissimilarity studies in a different light, suggesting that particularly bullying by popular adolescents is related to the social status attached to bullying.


Asunto(s)
Agresión/psicología , Relaciones Interpersonales , Grupo Paritario , Rechazo en Psicología , Conducta Social , Clase Social , Adolescente , Adulto , Estudios Transversales , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
13.
Pers Soc Psychol Bull ; 34(8): 1047-56, 2008 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18493030

RESUMEN

Two studies tested the conditions under which an environment (e.g., library, restaurant) raises the relevance of environment-specific social norms (e.g., being quiet, using table manners). As hypothesized, the relevance of such norms is raised when environments are goal relevant ("I am going there later") and when they are humanized with people or the remnants of their presence (e.g., a glass of wine on a table). Two studies show that goal-relevant environments and humanized environments raise the perceived importance of norms (Study 1) and the intention to conform to norms (Study 2). Interestingly, in both studies, these effects reach beyond norms related to the environments used in the studies.


Asunto(s)
Restaurantes , Conducta Social , Medio Social , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
14.
Dev Psychol ; 43(6): 1377-1389, 2007 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18020818

RESUMEN

The relation between bullying and helping and same-gender and cross-gender peer acceptance and peer rejection was examined in a sample of preadolescents aged 11 and 12 years (N=1,065). The authors tested predictions from a gender-homophily approach vs. predictions from a goal-framing approach in which acceptance and rejection are seen as being generated by approach and avoidance goals, respectively. For preadolescents, both approaches predicted a central role for gender, but the gender-homophily approach predicted symmetrical effects for acceptance and rejection, whereas the goal-framing approach predicted strong asymmetries. The data supported the goal-framing approach. The most important findings were that for preadolescents, acceptance is much more frequent and much more gendered than rejection; the absolute impact of helping on acceptance is much larger than that of bullying (and vice versa for rejection); for acceptance, there is a prototypicality effect (i.e., boys accept bullying girls better than nonbullying girls, and girls accept helping boys better than nonhelping boys); and for acceptance, there is a cross-gender ignorance effect (i.e., boys ignore helping in girls, and girls ignore bullying in boys).


Asunto(s)
Agresión/psicología , Conducta , Identidad de Género , Relaciones Interpersonales , Grupo Paritario , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Psicológicos , Caracteres Sexuales
15.
Psychol Aging ; 21(2): 281-90, 2006 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16768575

RESUMEN

In this study the authors investigated how satisfaction levels of affection, behavioral confirmation, and status, as three human social needs, relate to age, physical loss, and subjective well-being. Results (N=883, aged 65 to 98 years) revealed that (a) affection was relatively high and status was relatively low in all age and loss groups; behavioral confirmation showed negative age and loss effects but was better predicted by loss; (b) the three needs relate differentially to indicators of subjective well-being: affection and behavioral confirmation relate positively to life satisfaction; status and behavioral confirmation relate positively to positive affect and negatively to negative affect. It is concluded that the need for behavioral confirmation is more difficult to satisfy with high physical loss, but none of the three social needs becomes less important with advancing age.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/psicología , Actitud Frente a la Salud , Satisfacción Personal , Apoyo Social , Afecto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Conducta , Femenino , Necesidades y Demandas de Servicios de Salud , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Psicológicos
16.
Dev Psychol ; 41(4): 672-82, 2005 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16060813

RESUMEN

Research on bullying and victimization largely rests on univariate analyses and on reports from a single informant. Researchers may thus know too little about the simultaneous effects of various independent and dependent variables, and their research may be biased by shared method variance. The database for this Dutch study was large (N = 1,065) and rich enough to allow multivariate analysis and multi-source information. In addition, the effect of familial vulnerability for internalizing and externalizing disorders was studied. Gender, aggressiveness, isolation, and dislikability were most strongly related to bullying and victimization. Among the many findings that deviated from or enhanced the univariate knowledge base were that not only victims and bully/victims but bullies as well were disliked and that parenting was unrelated to bullying and victimization once other factors were controlled.


Asunto(s)
Agresión/psicología , Víctimas de Crimen , Dominación-Subordinación , Grupo Paritario , Niño , Hijo de Padres Discapacitados/psicología , Estudios de Cohortes , Femenino , Humanos , Control Interno-Externo , Masculino , Análisis Multivariante , Responsabilidad Parental/psicología , Estudios Prospectivos , Rechazo en Psicología , Factores de Riesgo , Factores Sexuales , Deseabilidad Social , Aislamiento Social , Socialización , Técnicas Sociométricas
17.
Front Psychol ; 6: 464, 2015.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25954223

RESUMEN

Beyond breathing, the regulation of body temperature-thermoregulation-is one of the most pressing concerns for many animals. A dysregulated body temperature has dire consequences for survival and development. Despite the high frequency of social thermoregulation occurring across many species, little is known about the role of social thermoregulation in human (social) psychological functioning. We outline a theory of social thermoregulation and reconsider earlier research on people's expectations of their social world (i.e., attachment) and their prediction of the social world. We provide support and outline a research agenda that includes consequences for individual variation in self-regulatory strategies and capabilities. In our paper, we discuss physiological, neural, and social processes surrounding thermoregulation. Emphasizing social thermoregulation in particular, we appeal to the economy of action principle and the hierarchical organization of human thermoregulatory systems. We close with future directions of a crucial aspect of human functioning: the social regulation of body temperature.

18.
J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci ; 59(9): M962-5, 2004 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15472162

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Selecting elderly persons who need geriatric interventions and making accurate treatment decisions are recurring challenges in geriatrics. Chronological age, although often used, does not seem to be the best selection criterion. Instead, the concept of frailty, which indicates several concurrent losses in resources, can be used. METHODS: The predictive values of chronological age and frailty were investigated in a large community sample of persons aged 65 years and older, randomly drawn from the register of six municipalities in the northern regions of the Netherlands (45% of the original addressees). The participants' generative capacity to sustain well-being (i.e., self-management abilities) was used as the main outcome measure. RESULTS: When using chronological age instead of frailty, both too many and too few persons were selected. Furthermore, frailty related more strongly (with beta values ranging from -.25 to -.39) to a decline in the participants' self-management abilities than did chronological age (with beta values ranging from -.06 to -.14). Chronological age added very little to the explained variances of all outcomes once frailty was included. CONCLUSIONS: Using frailty as the criterion to select older persons at risk for interventions may be better than selecting persons based only on their chronological age.


Asunto(s)
Anciano Frágil , Factores de Edad , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Femenino , Humanos , Evaluación de Resultado en la Atención de Salud
19.
PLoS One ; 9(4): e95421, 2014.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24759901

RESUMEN

Rejection can convey that one is seen as inferior and not worth bothering with. Is it possible for people to feel vicariously rejected in this sense and have reactions that are similar to those following personal rejection, such as feeling humiliated, powerless, and angry? A study on personal rejection was followed by two main studies on vicarious group-based rejection. It was found that merely observing rejection of ingroup members can trigger feelings of humiliation that are equally intense as those experienced in response to personal rejection. Moreover, given that the rejection is explicit, vicariously experienced feelings of humiliation can be accompanied by powerlessness and anger. Potentially, this combination of emotions could be an important source of offensive action against rejecters.


Asunto(s)
Ira/fisiología , Emociones/fisiología , Rechazo en Psicología , Adolescente , Conducta Peligrosa , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
20.
PLoS One ; 8(6): e65137, 2013.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23755182

RESUMEN

Contrary to what is often assumed, order is not the strongest context for encouraging normative behavior. The strongest context effect on normative behavior comes from cues that clearly convey other people's respect for norms. Ironically, this show of respect necessitates some contrasting disrespect that is being restored. Using civic virtues (such as helping behavior) as a prototype of normative behavior, the three field experiments described in this paper reveal the impact of normative cues on civic virtues. Results show that the strongest effect on making people follow prosocial norms in public places emanates from seeing order being restored, rather than just order being present. The robust and surprisingly large effects show that observing other people's respect for one particular norm (as evidenced in their restoring physical order) makes it more likely that the onlooker follows other norms as well. This implies that prosocial behavior has the highest chance of spreading when people observe order being restored. There are clear policy implications: create low cost "normative respect cues" wherever it is desirable to increase conformity to norms.


Asunto(s)
Conducta de Ayuda , Conformidad Social , Control de la Conducta , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Conducta de Masa , Motivación
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA