Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 24
Filtrar
Más filtros

Banco de datos
Tipo del documento
País de afiliación
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
J Clin Psychol ; 73(5): 536-546, 2017 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28170102

RESUMEN

The psychological treatment of choice for obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) is exposure and response prevention (ERP). However, the training required for practitioners to be proficient in delivering ERP is not readily available, thereby rendering the treatment inaccessible to most patients. Self-directed ERP (sERP) programs designed to increase the accessibility of ERP have not proven effective, perhaps because patients find it difficult to comply with exposure exercises without the guidance of a clinician. Research on cognitive bias modification (CBM) suggests that CBM may help individuals approach feared situations. In this case study, a patient with OCD completed a 7-week treatment program that combines sERP with CBM. Treatment led to a significant decrease in OCD symptoms and functional impairment. Results suggest that this novel treatment, which requires only an initial couple of sessions with a clinician trained in ERP, has the potential to increase the accessibility of ERP for patients with OCD.


Asunto(s)
Terapia Cognitivo-Conductual/métodos , Terapia Implosiva/métodos , Trastorno Obsesivo Compulsivo/terapia , Autocuidado/métodos , Femenino , Humanos , Persona de Mediana Edad
2.
Am J Geriatr Psychiatry ; 23(4): 416-422, 2015 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24953872

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: Hoarding disorder (HD) is a chronic condition associated with moderate to severe impairment in health and functioning. HD has been primarily studied in midlife adults, and there is limited research on HD in late life. METHODS: In this review, we summarize research on the presentation and characteristics of HD and hoarding symptoms in older adults, including evidence for associated impairment in daily functioning, physical health, and cognitive function. Finally, we review the evidence available for intervention outcomes for treating HD in older adults. RESULTS: Geriatric HD is characterized by severe functional impairment, medical and psychiatric comorbidities, and cognitive dysfunction. CONCLUSION: There is a lack of randomized controlled trials investigating evidence-based treatments for geriatric HD.


Asunto(s)
Demencia/epidemiología , Trastorno de Acumulación/epidemiología , Enfermedades de Inicio Tardío/epidemiología , Actividades Cotidianas/psicología , Edad de Inicio , Envejecimiento/psicología , Enfermedad Crónica , Comorbilidad , Estado de Salud , Trastorno de Acumulación/diagnóstico , Trastorno de Acumulación/psicología , Trastorno de Acumulación/terapia , Humanos , Enfermedades de Inicio Tardío/diagnóstico , Enfermedades de Inicio Tardío/psicología , Enfermedades de Inicio Tardío/terapia , Prevalencia , Evaluación de Síntomas
3.
Cogn Emot ; 29(7): 1210-23, 2015.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25355423

RESUMEN

Poor regulation of emotions may involve impaired attention control. In the current paper, we report the results of two studies examining the interaction of anxiety, attention control and cognitive load. In Study I, using a performance-based task to assess attention control, we examined whether anxiety is associated with impaired attention control, and whether these effects are influenced by working memory load. In Study II, we examined these effects in patients with a diagnosis of generalised anxiety disorder (GAD) compared to non-anxious control (NAC) participants. Results of Study I showed that high anxiety was associated with increased attention control, that is decreased interference from distractors, but only under high cognitive load. These results were replicated in Study II such that individuals with GAD showed increased attention control relative to NACs, but only under high cognitive load. These results help clarify previous predictions regarding the effect of anxiety on attention control.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos de Ansiedad/psicología , Ansiedad/psicología , Atención , Cognición , Ajuste Emocional , Orientación , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Ansiedad/diagnóstico , Trastornos de Ansiedad/diagnóstico , Señales (Psicología) , Discriminación en Psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Desempeño Psicomotor , Tiempo de Reacción , Valores de Referencia , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Adulto Joven
4.
Behav Modif ; 47(6): 1242-1268, 2023 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30943758

RESUMEN

Although efficacious treatments exist for anxiety disorders, issues remain regarding how best to conceptualize and measure purported change processes in clinical research. In the current study, we examined the relationship between treatment-specific (exposure therapy, attention bias modification [ABM]) as well as more general change processes with symptoms within a transdiagnostic sample using mixed models. Results indicated that slope of self-efficacy across treatment and between-session habituation across identical exposures was associated with slope of symptom change. Although slope of anxiety ratings within session was not associated with slope of symptom change, it did interact with other candidate exposure processes to predict symptoms. Purported ABM change processes were not associated with outcome. Our use of mixed models exemplifies an emerging trend in this research aimed at minimizing loss of data through aggregation, and our results highlight the utility of integrating treatment-specific as well as more general change processes in mechanistic research.


Asunto(s)
Terapia Cognitivo-Conductual , Terapia Implosiva , Humanos , Terapia Cognitivo-Conductual/métodos , Trastornos de Ansiedad/terapia , Ansiedad/terapia , Resultado del Tratamiento
5.
Depress Anxiety ; 29(3): 243-9, 2012 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21948383

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: The ability to attend to relevant visual information in a proficient manner is central to most day-to-day tasks. Research suggests, however, that this ability is compromised by anxiety such that anxiety results in narrowing the focus of visual attention. METHOD: In the current study (N = 58), we used the Attention Scope Task [1999: Gerontology 45:102-109] to examine the hypothesis that low-anxious individuals would be more proficient than high-anxious individuals in their scope of attention, that is, high-anxious individuals would have a larger scope of visual attention than low-anxious individuals. Additionally, we hypothesized that low-anxious individuals would be more proficient than high-anxious individuals in their ability to expand their scope of attention. RESULTS: Results revealed that, compared to low-anxious individuals, high-anxious individuals were impaired only in their ability to expand their scope of attention from a small area to a larger one. Inclusion of a depressed control group in the study revealed that our findings are specific to the effect of anxiety and not depressive symptoms. CONCLUSION: Thus, high-anxious individuals do not appear to have a smaller absolute scope of attention but instead seem to have difficulty expanding their attention scope dynamically. We discuss our results in relation to cognitive inflexibility in anxiety.


Asunto(s)
Ansiedad/psicología , Atención/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Autoinforme , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Adulto Joven
6.
Depress Anxiety ; 27(9): 807-12, 2010 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20821800

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: In this study, we examined executive control of attention in individuals with contamination-related obsessive-compulsive (OC) symptoms using a modified version of the Eriksen flanker task. The task indexes one's ability to resolve attentional conflict between different responses and to ignore task distracters. METHODS: For this study, we modified the original flanker task using affective words to examine the effect of threat-relevant stimuli on executive control of attention. Consistent with research on information processing biases in individuals with OC symptoms, we hypothesized that the flanker interference effect (i.e., difference in response latencies between incongruent and congruent flanker trials) will be greater for threat-related flankers in individuals with OC symptoms (n=32), relative to a control group (n=36). RESULTS: Results of our study were consistent with our hypothesis: The interference effect for threat flankers was greater in individuals with OC symptoms than in those low in symptoms. Moreover, there was no differential interference effect in the low and high symptom groups for neutral flankers. CONCLUSIONS: These findings suggest that the presence of threat-relevant distracters disrupts executive control of attention in individuals with contamination-related OC symptoms. These results are consistent with extant research on attentional biases in individuals with clinical and subclinical symptoms of OCD.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Miedo , Inhibición Psicológica , Trastorno Obsesivo Compulsivo/psicología , Adolescente , Señales (Psicología) , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Trastorno Obsesivo Compulsivo/diagnóstico , Trastorno Obsesivo Compulsivo/fisiopatología , Pruebas Psicológicas , Tiempo de Reacción , Autoinforme , Adulto Joven
7.
Behav Cogn Psychother ; 38(1): 1-14, 2010 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19852877

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: The belief that we can control our thoughts is not inevitably adaptive, particularly when it fuels mental control activities that have ironic unintended consequences. The conviction that the mind can and should be controlled can prompt people to suppress unwanted thoughts, and so can set the stage for the intrusive return of those very thoughts. An important question is whether or not these beliefs about the control of thoughts can be reduced experimentally. One possibility is that behavioral experiments aimed at revealing the ironic return of suppressed thoughts might create a lesson that could reduce unrealistic beliefs about the control of thoughts. AIMS: The present research assessed the influence of the thought suppression demonstration on beliefs about the control of thoughts in a non-clinical sample, and among individuals with obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). METHOD: In Study 1, we assessed the effect of the thought suppression demonstration on beliefs about the control of thoughts among low and high obsessive individuals in the non-clinical population (N = 62). In Study 2, we conducted a similar study with individuals with OCD (N = 29). RESULTS: Results suggest that high obsessive individuals in the non-clinical population are able to learn the futility of suppression through the thought suppression demonstration and to alter their faulty beliefs about the control of thoughts; however, for individuals with OCD, the demonstration may be insufficient for altering underlying beliefs. CONCLUSIONS: For individuals with OCD, the connection between suppressing a neutral thought in the suppression demonstration and suppressing a personally relevant obsession may need to be stated explicitly in order to affect their obsessive beliefs.


Asunto(s)
Inhibición Psicológica , Trastorno Obsesivo Compulsivo/psicología , Represión Psicológica , Pensamiento , Adolescente , Adulto , Atención , Concienciación , Cultura , Femenino , Humanos , Juicio , Masculino , Recuerdo Mental , Persona de Mediana Edad , Trastorno Obsesivo Compulsivo/diagnóstico , Inventario de Personalidad/estadística & datos numéricos , Psicometría , Adulto Joven
8.
Psychol Assess ; 31(8): 1052-1061, 2019 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31070448

RESUMEN

Behavioral measures are increasingly used to assess suicidal thoughts and behaviors. Some measures, such as the Suicide Stroop Task, have yielded mixed findings in the literature. An understudied feature of these behavioral measures has been their psychometric properties, which may affect the probability of detecting significant effects and reproducibility. In the largest investigation of its kind, we tested the internal consistency and concurrent validity of the Suicide Stroop Task in its current form, drawing from seven separate studies (N = 875 participants, 64% female, aged 12 to 81 years). Results indicated that the most common Suicide Stroop scoring approach, interference scores, yielded unacceptably low internal consistency (rs = -.09-.13) and failed to demonstrate concurrent validity. Internal consistency coefficients for mean reaction times (RTs) to each stimulus type ranged from rs = .93-.94. All scoring approaches for suicide-related interference demonstrated poor classification accuracy (AUCs = .52-.56) indicating that scores performed near chance in their ability to classify suicide attempters from nonattempters. In the case of mean RTs, we did not find evidence for concurrent validity despite our excellent reliability findings, highlighting that reliability does not guarantee a measure is clinically useful. These results are discussed in the context of the wider implications for testing and reporting psychometric properties of behavioral measures in mental health research. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Test de Stroop/estadística & datos numéricos , Suicidio/psicología , Adolescente , Adulto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Psicometría , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Suicidio/estadística & datos numéricos , Adulto Joven
9.
Conscious Cogn ; 17(1): 114-24, 2008 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17339120

RESUMEN

An unwanted thought appears to be cued easily by reminders in the environment but often the thought itself seems to cue nothing more than the desire to eliminate it from consciousness. This unusual asymmetry in the way unwanted thoughts are linked to other thoughts was the focus of the present research. Participants who were asked to suppress a thought or to concentrate on it completed a task assessing the influence of priming on reaction time (RT) for word/non-word judgments. Results revealed that suppression under cognitive load produced asymmetric priming: Priming with the associate of a suppressed word speeded RT for the suppressed word, but priming with a suppressed word did not speed RT for associated words. These findings suggest that thought suppression induces an unusual form of cognitive accessibility in which movement of activation toward the suppressed thought from associates is facilitated but movement of activation away from the suppressed thought to associates is undermined.


Asunto(s)
Asociación , Señales (Psicología) , Pensamiento , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Control Interno-Externo , Masculino , Conducta Obsesiva/psicología , Tiempo de Reacción , Semántica
10.
Behav Res Ther ; 45(8): 1957-65, 2007 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17074302

RESUMEN

This study proposes and tests a theoretical model suggesting that the propensity to suppress unwanted thoughts is associated with an increased presence and frequency of self-injurious thoughts and behaviors (SITB). In the model, propensity to suppress unwanted thoughts is hypothesized to be a cognitive mediator of the relationship between emotional reactivity and SITB, and is expected to be related to the extent to which SITB is initiated to escape from aversive emotions. Results of this cross-sectional study of adolescents (N=87) revealed that the self-reported propensity to suppress unwanted thoughts is associated with the presence and frequency of non-suicidal self-injury (NSSI), suicidal ideation, and suicide attempts. Furthermore, thought suppression partially mediates the relationship between emotional reactivity and the frequency of NSSI and suicidal ideation. Finally, adolescents with a higher tendency to suppress unwanted thoughts report engaging in NSSI in order to reduce aversive emotions rather than for social communication. Results are discussed within the framework of the negative reinforcement function of SITB.


Asunto(s)
Represión Psicológica , Conducta Autodestructiva/psicología , Adaptación Psicológica , Adolescente , Conducta del Adolescente , Emociones , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Psicológicos , Psicometría , Refuerzo en Psicología , Intento de Suicidio/psicología
11.
Arch Suicide Res ; 21(1): 33-51, 2017 Jan 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26984769

RESUMEN

This study explores whether four sessions of attention bias modification (ABM) decreases suicide-specific attentional bias. We conducted two experiments where suicide ideators completed either a Training or Control version of ABM, a computer-based intervention intended to target attentional bias. Suicide-specific attentional bias was measured using adapted Stroop and probe discrimination tasks. The first experiment with community-based suicide ideators did not show that ABM impacts attentional bias or suicidal ideation. The second experiment with clinically severe suicidal inpatients yielded similar results. Post-hoc findings suggest that the type of attentional bias targeted by the current intervention may differ from the type that marks suicide risk. There remains little to no evidence that the ABM intervention changes suicide-specific attentional bias or suicidal ideation.


Asunto(s)
Sesgo Atencional , Terapia Cognitivo-Conductual/métodos , Ideación Suicida , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Resultado del Tratamiento , Adulto Joven
12.
J Anxiety Disord ; 43: 14-22, 2016 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27472452

RESUMEN

This is the first randomised controlled trial to evaluate a protocol for cognitive behaviour therapy (CBT) for a Specific Phobia of Vomiting (SPOV) compared with a wait list and to use assessment scales that are specific for a SPOV. METHOD: 24 participants (23 women and 1 man) were randomly allocated to either 12 sessions of CBT or a wait list. RESULTS: At the end of the treatment, CBT was significantly more efficacious than the wait list with a large effect size (Cohen's d=1.53) on the Specific Phobia of Vomiting Inventory between the two groups after 12 sessions. Six (50%) of the participants receiving CBT achieved clinically significant change compared to 2 (16%) participants in the wait list group. Eight (58.3%) participants receiving CBT achieved reliable improvement compared to 2 (16%) participants in the wait list group. CONCLUSIONS: A SPOV is a condition treatable by CBT but further developments are required to increase efficacy.


Asunto(s)
Terapia Cognitivo-Conductual/métodos , Trastornos Fóbicos/terapia , Vómitos/psicología , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Trastornos Fóbicos/psicología , Proyectos Piloto , Resultado del Tratamiento , Adulto Joven
13.
Cognit Ther Res ; 39(4): 424-440, 2015 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26366021

RESUMEN

Exposure and Response Prevention (ERP) is the most effective treatment for OCD but it is not accessible to most patients. Attempts to increase the accessibility of ERP via self-directed ERP (sERP) programs such as computerized delivery and bibliotherapy have met with noncompliance, presumably because patients find the exposure exercises unacceptable. Previous research suggests that Cognitive Bias Modification (CBM) interventions may help individuals approach feared situations. The goal of the current study was to test the efficacy of a treatment program for OCD that integrates sERP with CBM. Twenty-two individuals meeting diagnostic criteria for OCD enrolled in our 7-week treatment program. Results suggest that sERP with CBM led to significant reduction of OCD symptoms and functional impairment. Indeed, the magnitude of the effect of this novel treatment, that requires only an initial session with a clinician trained in ERP for OCD, was comparable to that of the gold standard clinician-administered ERP. Moreover, preliminary evidence suggests that CBM interventions targeting interpretation bias may be most effective, whereas those targeting attention and working memory bias may not be so.

14.
J Abnorm Psychol ; 111(2): 302-12, 2002 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12003451

RESUMEN

Clinical lore suggests that depression is associated with frequent and intense crying. To test these postulations empirically, a standardized cry-evoking stimulus was presented to depressed and nondepressed participants, and their likelihood of crying and the magnitude of crying-related changes in their emotion experience, behavior, and autonomic physiology were compared. Unexpectedly, crying was no more likely in depressed than in nondepressed participants. Within the nondepressed group, participants who cried exhibited increases in the report and display of sadness and had greater cardiac and electrodermal activation than did participants who did not cry. There was less evidence of this crying-related emotional activation within the depressed group. The lack of emotional activation among clinically depressed participants who cried provides a tantalizing clue concerning how emotions are dysregulated in this disorder.


Asunto(s)
Llanto , Trastorno Depresivo Mayor/psicología , Adulto , Nivel de Alerta , Trastorno Depresivo Mayor/diagnóstico , Femenino , Respuesta Galvánica de la Piel , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Inventario de Personalidad , Rol , Sexo
15.
Emotion ; 13(3): 478-84, 2013 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23163714

RESUMEN

We used the Approach-Avoidance Task (AAT) to examine the role of automatic action tendencies. We hypothesized that, after manipulation of automatic action tendencies, participants would be more likely to approach feared objects when compared with participants in a control condition. Participants were instructed to push or pull a joystick, resulting in contamination-related and neutral pictures moving progressively away from or toward them, respectively. We manipulated approach by building a contingency between the arm movement and the picture type in the active condition but not in the control condition. Consistent with our hypothesis, participants in the active manipulation group showed facilitated automatic approach tendencies and reduced avoidance tendencies for contamination-related stimuli and completed more steps approaching their feared objects in a behavioral approach test compared with participants in the control group. Our results suggest that automatic action tendencies may play an important role in the maintenance of fear-related behavioral avoidance.


Asunto(s)
Miedo/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Estado de Conciencia/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Trastornos Fóbicos/psicología , Adulto Joven
16.
Cognit Ther Res ; 37(2)2013 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24179287

RESUMEN

In two experiments we examined the psychometric properties of a new measure of interpretation bias in individuals with obsessive-compulsive symptoms (OCs). In Experiment 1, 38 individuals high in OC symptoms, 34 individuals high in anxiety and dysphoric symptoms, and 31 asymptomatic individuals completed the measure. Results revealed that the Word Sentence Association Test for OCD (WSAO) can differentiate those with OC symptoms from both a matched anxious/dysphoric group and a non-anxious/non-dysphoric group. In a second experiment, we tested the predictive validity of the WSAO using a performance-based behavioral approach test of contamination fears, and found that the WSAO was a better predictor of avoidance than an established measure of OC washing symptoms (Obsessive Compulsive Inventory-Revised, washing subscale). Our results provide preliminary evidence for the reliability and validity of the WSAO as well as its usefulness in predicting response to behavioral challenge above and beyond OC symptoms, depression, and anxiety.

17.
Cognit Ther Res ; 36(5): 537-547, 2012 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23125469

RESUMEN

Individuals with social phobia report experiencing recurrent negative images of themselves in social situations. However, research on the role of visual imagery in social phobia has relied exclusively on self-report measures. In the first study, we used a visual image generation task with social-threat and neutral stimuli to test the hypothesis that individuals with Generalized Social Phobia (GSP, n = 32) are more efficient at generating images related to social-threat words than are non-anxious-controls (NACs, n = 28). We found that, contrary to our hypothesis, the GSP and NAC groups did not differ in speed of generating images related to social-threat words. However, the GSP group was significantly slower than the NAC group at generating images related to neutral words. To further examine the generation of neutral images, we conducted a second study using a well-validated neutral image generation task, and found that the GSP group (n = 24) was slower to generate neutral images than were the NAC (n = 21) and anxious-control (AC, n = 15) groups, which did not differ from each other. Taken together, findings from the two studies suggest that social phobia is characterized by less efficient generation of neutral images.

18.
J Abnorm Psychol ; 119(1): 136-42, 2010 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20141250

RESUMEN

In the current study, we evaluated the effectiveness of attention training in individuals with subclinical obsessive-compulsive symptoms. We hypothesized that after completing attention training, participants would be more likely to complete steps in a hierarchy approaching their feared contaminant compared with participants in the control condition. Participants completed a probe detection task by identifying letters replacing one member of a pair of words (neutral or contamination related). We trained attention by building a contingency between the location of the contamination-related word in the active condition and not in the control condition. Participants in the active group showed a significant reduction in attention bias for threat and completed significantly more steps when approaching their feared objects compared with participants in the control group. Our results suggest that attention disengagement training may facilitate approaching feared objects in individuals with obsessive-compulsive symptoms.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Miedo , Trastorno Obsesivo Compulsivo/psicología , Trastorno Obsesivo Compulsivo/terapia , Enseñanza/métodos , Adolescente , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
19.
Behav Res Ther ; 48(10): 1058-62, 2010 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20650448

RESUMEN

We used an approach-avoidance task (AAT) to examine response to threatening stimuli in 20 individuals high in contamination-related obsessive-compulsive symptoms (HCs) and 21 individuals low in contamination-related obsessive-compulsive symptoms (LCs). Participants were instructed to respond to contamination-related and neutral pictures by pulling a joystick towards themselves or by pushing it away from themselves. Moving the joystick changed the size of the image to simulate approaching or distancing oneself from the object. Consistent with our hypothesis, the HC group was significantly slower in pulling contamination-related pictures than in pulling neutral pictures, whereas in the LC group there was no difference between speed of pulling contamination-related pictures and neutral pictures. Contrary to our hypothesis, we did not find support for faster pushing away of contamination-related pictures than neutral pictures by the HC group. Moreover, the degree of avoidance of contamination-related stimuli when pulling - but not when pushing - was significantly correlated with self-reported contamination-related obsessive-compulsive symptoms. These results suggest a biased behavioral response for threatening objects in individuals high in contamination fears only when inhibiting the prepotent response to avoid threatening stimuli and not when performing a practiced avoidance response. Thus, our results validate the use of the AAT as a measure of inhibited and uninhibited automatic avoidance reactions to emotional information in individuals with contamination-related obsessive-compulsive symptoms.


Asunto(s)
Reacción de Prevención , Trastorno Obsesivo Compulsivo/psicología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Adaptación Psicológica , Adolescente , Análisis de Varianza , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Represión Psicológica , Adulto Joven
20.
J Anxiety Disord ; 24(7): 657-62, 2010 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20510577

RESUMEN

Cognitive theorists propose that attentional biases for threatening information play an important role in the development and maintenance of anxiety disorders. If attentional biases for threat figure in the maintenance of anxiety, then the experimental reduction of the bias for threat (attention training) should reduce anxiety. We randomly assigned 41 spider-fearful individuals to receive either attention training (n=20) or control procedures (n=21). We used a modified dot-probe discrimination paradigm with photographs of spiders and cows to train attention. Training reduced attentional bias for spiders, but only temporarily. Although both groups declined in spider fear and avoidance, reduction in attentional bias did not produce significantly greater symptom reduction in the training group than in the control group. However, reduction in attentional bias predicted reduction in self-reported fear for the training group. The reduction in attentional bias for threat may have been insufficiently robust to produce symptom reduction greater than that produced by exposure to a live spider and spider photographs alone. Alternatively, attention training may be an unsuitable intervention for spider fear.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Terapia Cognitivo-Conductual/métodos , Trastornos Fóbicos/terapia , Arañas , Adolescente , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Animales , Miedo/psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Trastornos Fóbicos/psicología , Estimulación Luminosa , Tiempo de Reacción , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Terapéutica
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA