Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 54
Filtrar
Más filtros

Banco de datos
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry ; 27(7): 849-859, 2018 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29143155

RESUMEN

Language disorder (LD) and social-emotional and behavioural (SEB) difficulties are common childhood problems that often co-occur. While there is clear evidence of these associations from clinical samples, less is known about community samples. This paper examines these associations in children aged 4-7 years from a community-based longitudinal study. 771 families provided questionnaire and assessment data at 4, 5 and 7 years. Parent-reported SEB difficulties were measured at each point (SDQ). Child language was directly assessed at 4 (CELF-P2), 5 and 7 years (CELF-4). Linear regression analysis was used to compare cross-sectional differences in mean SDQ scores between children with and without LD at each time point. Linear regression was then used to examine how patterns of language development (language disordered at three time points; never disordered; disordered at one or two time points, i.e. 'unstable' group) related to SEB difficulties at each age, adjusted for potential confounders, as in the previous analyses. Higher hyperactivity/inattention scores were associated with LD at each age. In fully adjusted models, there was little difference in mean emotional symptoms scores between children with and without LD. The 'never' LD group had lower mean SDQ scores at each time point than the 'unstable' group. Findings highlight that children with persistent LD from preschool to early primary school may be more likely to have concomitant SEB difficulties, particularly behavioural difficulties. Those with unstable LD may also have co-occurring SEB difficulties, showing a need for education and health professionals to monitor early language and SEB development.


Asunto(s)
Emociones/fisiología , Lenguaje , Trastornos Mentales/psicología , Salud Mental/tendencias , Niño , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino
2.
J Paediatr Child Health ; 51(1): 61-4, 2015 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25586846

RESUMEN

Since the Journal of Paediatrics and Child Health was first published, there has been substantial change in the field of autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) with an exponential increase in the amount of funded and published research. In this paper, we focus on regression in children with ASD, a phenomenon that remains poorly understood. We discuss the implications of what we know about regression in ASD for the way we think about ASD more broadly and for paediatric practice.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos Generalizados del Desarrollo Infantil/fisiopatología , Progresión de la Enfermedad , Niño , Desarrollo Infantil , Trastornos Generalizados del Desarrollo Infantil/diagnóstico , Trastornos Generalizados del Desarrollo Infantil/psicología , Preescolar , Humanos , Pronóstico
3.
Int J Lang Commun Disord ; 50(4): 488-98, 2015 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25630989

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Early identification of Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is currently limited by the absence of reliable biological markers for the disorder, as well as the reliability of screening and assessment tools for children aged between 6 and 18 months. Ongoing research has demonstrated the importance of early social communication skills in differentiating children later diagnosed with ASD from their typically developing (TD) peers, but researchers have not yet investigated whether these differences can be detected using community-ascertained systematic observation data as early as 12 months. AIMS: To investigate whether differences in early social communication skills can be detected at 12 months of age, comparing children later diagnosed with ASD, and TD peers; and to determine whether differences remain when groupings are based on age of subsequent ASD diagnosis. METHODS & PROCEDURES: From a prospective community-ascertained sample, we collected data on children in early life, then conducted retrospective analyses for those children who were later diagnosed with ASD by the age of 7 years, compared with matched TD peers. We analysed standardized observational data of early communication skills, collected using the Communication and Symbolic Behavior Scales-Developmental Profile (CSBS-DP) Behavior Sample, when participants were 12 months of age. OUTCOMES & RESULTS: Children in the ASD group exhibited significantly lower social communication skills than the TD group, including on the Total score and Social and Symbolic Composite scores of the CSBS-DP Behavior Sample. Differences on the Total score and Social Composite were also detected for both early and late ASD diagnosis groups when compared with the TD group. CONCLUSIONS & IMPLICATIONS: These findings give further support for the importance of social communication in assessing children at risk of ASD as early as 12 months of age. Future research could evaluate the sensitivity and specificity of direct observation of these early communication skills as diagnostic indicators for ASD at 12 months, and investigate whether it is possible to distinguish between ASD and other high-risk groups (e.g. developmental delay) at this age.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno del Espectro Autista/diagnóstico , Trastorno de Comunicación Social/diagnóstico , Factores de Edad , Niño , Preescolar , Diagnóstico Precoz , Femenino , Humanos , Lactante , Masculino , Grupo Paritario , Estudios Prospectivos , Valores de Referencia , Estudios Retrospectivos
4.
BMC Pediatr ; 14: 234, 2014 Sep 22.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25241772

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Universal developmental surveillance programs aimed at early identification and targeted early intervention significantly improve short- and long-term outcomes in children at risk of developmental disorders. However, a significant challenge remains in providing sufficiently rigorous research and robust evidence to inform policy and service delivery. This paper describes the methods of the 'Watch Me Grow' study that aims to maximise accurate early detection of children with developmental disorders through a partnership formed between policy makers, service providers and researchers. METHODS/DESIGN: A mixed methods study design was developed consisting of: (1) a qualitative study of parents and health service providers to investigate barriers and enablers of developmental surveillance; (2) recruitment of a birth cohort and their longitudinal follow-up to 18 months of age to: a) assess risk factors for not accessing existing developmental surveillance programs and b) estimate the prevalence of children identified with developmental risk; (3) comparison of surveillance outcomes with a reference standard at 18 months of age to assess the diagnostic test accuracy of existing and alternative developmental surveillance tools; and (4) comparison of developmental surveillance models to inform policy recommendations. Data linkage will be used to determine the uptake and representativeness of the study participant group versus non-participants. DISCUSSION: The Watch Me Grow study is expected to provide a collaborative opportunity to enhance universal developmental surveillance for early accurate identification of developmental risk. This will also provide quality evidence about identification of developmental risk and access to services to be embedded in existing practice with linkages to policy development.


Asunto(s)
Desarrollo Infantil , Discapacidades del Desarrollo/prevención & control , Australia , Estudios de Cohortes , Discapacidades del Desarrollo/diagnóstico , Humanos , Lactante , Recién Nacido , Estudios Longitudinales , Factores de Riesgo
5.
J Paediatr Child Health ; 50(5): 341-6, 2014 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24422663

RESUMEN

Continuing from part 1, part 2 of the autism spectrum disorders review explores clinical practice and service delivery aspects of autism spectrum disorders including current assessment approaches in Australia, family-centred models of care, and key service structure and delivery issues. Treatments including behavioural interventions, established and emergent medication, and complementary and alternative therapies are discussed. The key role of paediatricians as both individual child and family care providers and advocates, as well as agents of service reform in Australia, is evident. Much still needs to be done.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos Generalizados del Desarrollo Infantil/diagnóstico , Trastornos Generalizados del Desarrollo Infantil/terapia , Servicios de Salud del Niño , Australia , Niño , Servicios de Salud del Niño/métodos , Servicios de Salud del Niño/organización & administración , Terapia Combinada , Política de Salud , Necesidades y Demandas de Servicios de Salud , Humanos , Pediatría , Rol del Médico , Resultado del Tratamiento
6.
J Paediatr Child Health ; 50(5): 335-40, 2014 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24383615

RESUMEN

This review paper describes our current perspective of autism spectrum disorders (ASD), taking into account past, current and future classification systems and the evolving definitions of ASD. International prevalence rates from 1965 to 2012 are presented and key issues, including whether there is an epidemic of autism and what this means in terms of thinking about possible causes of autism, are discussed. Also discussed is the need for high quality national data collection in Australia and the evidence, and lack of evidence, for the many theoretical causes of ASD. The lack of robust classification of autism along with limited high quality evidence base about its prevalence and possible causes leaves ample space for future discoveries.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos Generalizados del Desarrollo Infantil , Australia/epidemiología , Niño , Trastornos Generalizados del Desarrollo Infantil/clasificación , Trastornos Generalizados del Desarrollo Infantil/diagnóstico , Trastornos Generalizados del Desarrollo Infantil/epidemiología , Trastornos Generalizados del Desarrollo Infantil/etiología , Humanos , Prevalencia , Factores de Riesgo
7.
J Paediatr Child Health ; 50(9): 693-700, 2014 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24909517

RESUMEN

AIM: The aim of this study is to identify social communication skills in infancy which predict autism spectrum disorder (ASD) diagnosis by 7 years as compared with children with other developmental difficulties or typical development from within a population sample. METHODS: Children with an ASD (n = 41), developmental delay (n = 28), language impairment (n = 47) and typical development (n = 41) were drawn from a large, longitudinal community sample following children from 8 months to 7 years of age, the Early Language in Victoria Study. At 7 years of age, early social communication skills at 8, 12 and 24 months from the Communication and Symbolic Behavior Scales Infant-Toddler Checklist and the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventory: Words and Gestures were compared between groups and used to predict ASD diagnosis. RESULTS: Significant predictors of ASD diagnosis were found from 8 months, predominantly focused on gesture use and communicative behaviours, such as requesting and joint attention. While comparisons between children with ASD and children with language impairment and typical development revealed differences from 8 months of age, the developmental delay group did not differ significantly from ASD on any measure until 24 months of age. At 24 months, children with ASD had lower Communication and Symbolic Behavior Scales Use of Communication scores as compared with all other groups. CONCLUSIONS: The capacity to identify early markers of ASD should facilitate awareness of the risk of an ASD as compared with other developmental problems and point to the need for further developmental assessment, monitoring and provision of early intervention if indicated.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno Autístico/diagnóstico , Trastornos de la Comunicación/diagnóstico , Discapacidades del Desarrollo/diagnóstico , Conducta Social , Niño , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Lactante , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Padres , Estudios Prospectivos
8.
J Paediatr Child Health ; 49(3): 193-8, 2013 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23437778

RESUMEN

AIM: To explore the link between pre-school children's general home computer use and their letter knowledge. METHODS: As part of the Early Language in Victoria Study, a community cohort of 1539 four-year-old children was tested on letter knowledge as well as on non-verbal intelligence, oral language, articulation and phonological awareness. Performance on these measures was examined in relation to parent-questionnaire responses exploring home literacy environment and the amount of time children spent using the computer. RESULTS: A positive correlation between computer use and letter knowledge was found, and this association was still evident after controlling for other cognitive and environmental factors known to predict the development of letter knowledge in young children. CONCLUSIONS: Greater computer use in pre-school children appears to have a positive association with emerging literacy development. Future research needs to examine the nature of that association.


Asunto(s)
Computadores/estadística & datos numéricos , Lenguaje , Fonética , Lectura , Vocabulario , Preescolar , Escolaridad , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
9.
Child Psychiatry Hum Dev ; 44(2): 247-64, 2013 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22820873

RESUMEN

This study evaluated a 6-session group parenting program, Tuning into Kids (TIK), as treatment for young children (aged 4.0-5.11 years) with behavior problems. TIK targets parent emotion socialization (parent emotion awareness, regulation and emotion coaching skills). Fifty-four parents, recruited via a child behavior clinic, were randomized into intervention (TIK) or waitlist (clinical treatment as usual). Parents reported emotion awareness/regulation, emotion coaching, empathy and child behavior (pre-intervention, post-intervention, 6-month follow-up); teachers reported child behavior and observers rated parent-child emotion coaching and child emotion knowledge (pre-intervention, follow-up). Data were analyzed using growth curve modeling and ANCOVA. Parents in both conditions reported less emotional dismissiveness and reduced child behavior problems; in the intervention group, parents also reported greater empathy and had improved observed emotion coaching skills; their children had greater emotion knowledge and reduced teacher-reported behavior problems. TIK appears to be a promising addition to treatment for child behavior problems.


Asunto(s)
Terapia Conductista/métodos , Trastornos de la Conducta Infantil/terapia , Conducta Infantil/psicología , Emociones , Relaciones Padres-Hijo , Padres/psicología , Trastornos de la Conducta Infantil/psicología , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Responsabilidad Parental/psicología , Padres/educación , Socialización , Resultado del Tratamiento
10.
Infant Ment Health J ; 33(4): 386-399, 2012 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28520171

RESUMEN

Infants seek contingent, companionable interactions with others. Infants in a Neonatal Intensive Care Unit (NICU), while receiving care that optimizes their chances of survival, often do not have the kind of interactions that are optimal for their social development. Live music therapy (MT) with infants is an intervention that aims for contingent, social interaction between therapist and infant. This study, with a limited numbers of infants, examined the effectiveness of an MT intervention in the NICU at The Royal Children's Hospital Melbourne. Two groups of late pre-term and full-term infants were recruited to the study; one was given MT and the other was not. A healthy group of infants not given MT served as an additional control. The effect of MT was indexed using two measures reflecting infant social engagement: the Neurobehavioral Assessment of the Preterm Infant (NAPI) and the Alarm Distress Baby Scale (ADBB). Results suggest that the MT intervention used at The Royal Children's Hospital Melbourne supports infants' neurobehavioral development. In particular, hospitalized infants who received MT were better able to maintain self-regulation during social interaction with an adult, were less irritable and cried less, and were more positive in their response to adult handling, when compared with infants who did not receive the intervention. These are important prerequisites for social interaction and development. Further and larger scale research using MT with this population is indicated.

11.
J Paediatr Child Health ; 47(7): 405-7, 2011 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20598076

RESUMEN

Children's development is influenced by the quantity and quality of father involvement (FI) in their care. High levels of FI are associated with significant and highly desirable outcomes for children and families. In order to foster increased positive FI, dissemination of evidence-based knowledge regarding its importance and value is needed. Evidence summarised here suggests that fathers and mothers alike need to be educated about the fact that positive paternal involvement is highly beneficial to children.


Asunto(s)
Relaciones Padre-Hijo , Responsabilidad Parental , Conducta Paterna , Niño , Desarrollo Infantil , Protección a la Infancia , Humanos
12.
Int J Lang Commun Disord ; 46(4): 489-94, 2011.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21771224

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Relationships between child language development and temperament have been little studied in young children, although it is known that children with language impairment are at risk in a number of domains of psychosocial development involving temperamental influences. AIMS: To investigate the relationships between temperament and language development, along with child behavioural adjustment and maternal psychosocial factors. METHODS & PROCEDURES: A sample of 4-year-old children with language impairment was compared with typically developing children, from a large community cohort in a longitudinal study, on three temperament dimensions, behavioural and emotional problems, and maternal factors. Participants were part of a large community cohort involved in a longitudinal study. OUTCOMES & RESULTS: While the groups did not differ significantly on temperamental shyness/sociability, children with language impairment showed more negative dispositions on the persistence/self-regulation factor, and on overall temperamental difficultness. Behavioural problems were elevated in the language impairment group and were associated with temperament in both groups. Maternal measures of education level, reading and vocabulary skills were significantly lower in the language impairment group. CONCLUSIONS & IMPLICATIONS: Generally the language impairment group showed a constellation of developmental disadvantages which add to the existing developmental vulnerability conferred by the presence of language impairment. Poorer child temperament self-regulation and behavioural adjustment are strong risk factors for school learning, while lower mother education and literacy contribute further disadvantage. Clinicians managing language impairment in children need to be aware of the whole package of risk factors which are common in this population.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos de la Conducta Infantil/psicología , Conducta Infantil/psicología , Trastornos del Desarrollo del Lenguaje/psicología , Relaciones Madre-Hijo , Personalidad , Trastornos de la Conducta Infantil/epidemiología , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Desarrollo del Lenguaje , Trastornos del Desarrollo del Lenguaje/epidemiología , Masculino , Lectura , Características de la Residencia , Factores de Riesgo , Apoyo Social
13.
Clin Psychol Rev ; 85: 101984, 2021 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33607568

RESUMEN

The study of temperament in Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) has the potential to provide insight regarding variability in the onset, nature, and course of both core and co-morbid symptoms. The aim of this systematic review was to integrate existing findings concerning temperament in the context of ASD. Searches of Medline, PsychInfo and Scopus databases identified 64 relevant studies. As a group, children and adolescents with ASD appear to be temperamentally different from both typically developing and other clinical non-ASD groups, characterized by higher negative affectivity, lower surgency, and lower effortful control at a higher-order level. Consistent with research on typically developing children, correlational findings and emerging longitudinal evidence suggests that lower effortful control and higher negative affect are associated with increased internalizing and externalizing problems in ASD samples. Longitudinal studies suggest there may be temperamental differences between high familial risk infants who do and do not develop ASD from as early as 6-months of age. Limitations of existing research are highlighted, and possible directions for future research to capitalize on the potential afforded through the study of temperament in relation to ASD are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno del Espectro Autista , Problema de Conducta , Adolescente , Niño , Humanos , Lactante , Temperamento
14.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry ; 51(12): 1342-50, 2010 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20735794

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: This study evaluated a new prevention and early intervention parenting program: Tuning in to Kids. The program aims to improve emotion socialization practices in parents of preschool children and is based on research evidence that parents' responses to, and coaching of, their children's emotions influence emotional and behavioral functioning in children. METHODS: Two hundred and sixteen primary caregiver parents of children aged 4.0-5.11 years were randomized into an intervention or waitlist control group. Parents in the intervention condition attended a 6-session group parenting program plus two booster sessions. Assessment occurred pre-intervention, post-intervention and at six-month follow-up. Questionnaires assessed parent emotion awareness and regulation, parent beliefs and practices of emotion socialization (emotion dismissing, emotion coaching, empathy) and child behavior (parent and teacher report). Observation of emotion socialization practices and child emotional knowledge was conducted pre-intervention and at follow-up with 161 parent-child dyads. RESULTS: Parents in the intervention condition reported significant improvements in their own emotion awareness and regulation, increases in emotion coaching, and decreases in emotionally dismissive beliefs and behaviors. There were increases in parents' observed use of emotion labels and discussion of causes and consequences of emotions with their children. Child emotional knowledge improved, and reductions in child behavior problems were reported by parents and teachers. CONCLUSIONS: This study provides support for the efficacy of a parenting intervention targeting parent emotion socialization practices that lead to improved child emotional knowledge and behavior. This preventative intervention targeting parents' own emotion awareness and regulation, as well as emotional communication in parent-child relationships, is a promising addition to available parenting programs.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Infantil/psicología , Intervención Educativa Precoz , Emociones , Relaciones Padres-Hijo , Responsabilidad Parental/psicología , Padres/educación , Conducta Social , Adulto , Niño , Preescolar , Servicios Comunitarios de Salud Mental/métodos , Intervención Educativa Precoz/métodos , Femenino , Estudios de Seguimiento , Humanos , Masculino , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Resultado del Tratamiento
15.
Int J Lang Commun Disord ; 45(5): 572-85, 2010.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19886849

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Parent report instruments are frequently used for the identification of both 'at-risk' children and to support the diagnosis of communication delay. Whilst the evidence is strong for the accuracy of parent report of vocabulary between 2 and 3 years, there are fewer studies that have considered the ability of parents to report on early communication behaviours in 12-month-old infants. AIMS: To investigate the validity of the underlying structure of the Communication and Symbolic Behaviour Scales-Developmental Profile (CSBS-DP) for each of the direct observation and parent reports of communication behaviour in infants at 12 months of age. METHODS & PROCEDURES: Participants were 1725 infants, already participating in a longitudinal study of language development, whose parents completed the Infant-Toddler Checklist from the CSBS-DP. Seven hundred and twenty-eight (728) of these infants also completed the Behaviour Sample from the CSBS-DP. The structure of the CSBS-DP was examined using confirmatory factor analyses (CFA) of the Behaviour Sample and the Infant-Toddler Checklist. Correlations between the Infant-Toddler Checklist and the Behaviour Sample on the total, composite, and subscale scores were also calculated. OUTCOMES & RESULTS: Confirmatory factor analysis of the CSBS-DP Behaviour Sample replicated previous work conducted during the development of the instrument, but on a larger and younger cohort of Australian infants. The data provided support for at least three factors, broadly representing Social, Speech, and Symbolic communication skills, with some evidence that the speech factor could be further split into sub-factors representing Sounds and Words. There was support for a three-factor structure for the Infant-Toddler Checklist. Moderate correlations were found between results from the Behaviour Sample and the Infant-Toddler Checklist. CONCLUSIONS & IMPLICATIONS: As measures of early communication skill for young infants, the CSBS-DP Behaviour Sample and the Infant-Toddler Checklist are valid clinical tools for measuring constructs broadly representing Social, Speech, and Symbolic communication skills. The Speech composite in particular emerged as a strong factor under the Behaviour Sample.


Asunto(s)
Comunicación , Trastornos del Desarrollo del Lenguaje/prevención & control , Tamizaje Masivo/métodos , Pruebas Psicológicas , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Lenguaje Infantil , Análisis Factorial , Femenino , Humanos , Lactante , Masculino , Comunicación no Verbal , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Victoria
16.
Autism ; 13(1): 43-57, 2009 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19176576

RESUMEN

In order to examine hypothesized underlying neurocognitive processes in repetitive behaviour, children and adolescents (7-16 years) with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD) were compared on a range of executive function (EF) measures. Performance on neuropsychological tests assessing executive functioning showed a trend for children with ASD to perform poorly on tasks requiring generation of multiple responses, while children with OCD tended to demonstrate impairments on a task requiring inhibition. Parental ratings on a questionnaire measure of EF indicated impairments in both groups relative to controls. Relationships between questionnaire and performance measures of EF were generally weak. There was some limited support for a relationship between EF and repetitive behaviour, but effects tended to be small and variable across groups and measures.


Asunto(s)
Síndrome de Asperger/diagnóstico , Trastorno Autístico/diagnóstico , Trastornos Generalizados del Desarrollo Infantil/diagnóstico , Trastornos del Conocimiento/diagnóstico , Trastorno Obsesivo Compulsivo/diagnóstico , Conducta Estereotipada , Adolescente , Síndrome de Asperger/psicología , Trastorno Autístico/psicología , Niño , Trastornos Generalizados del Desarrollo Infantil/psicología , Trastornos del Conocimiento/psicología , Diagnóstico Diferencial , Femenino , Humanos , Inhibición Psicológica , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Trastorno Obsesivo Compulsivo/psicología , Solución de Problemas , Conducta Verbal
17.
J Autism Dev Disord ; 48(10): 3282-3295, 2018 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29705923

RESUMEN

This study used a prospective community-based sample to describe patterns and predictors of language development from 4 to 7 years in verbal children (IQ ≥ 70) with autism spectrum disorder (ASD; n = 26-27). Children with typical language (TD; n = 858-861) and language impairment (LI; n = 119) were used for comparison. Children with ASD and LI had similar mean language scores that were lower on average than children with TD. Similar proportions across all groups had declining, increasing and stable patterns. Language progressed at a similar rate for all groups, with progress influenced by IQ and language ability at 4 years rather than social communication skills or diagnosis of ASD. These findings inform advice for parents about language prognosis in ASD.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno del Espectro Autista/diagnóstico , Trastorno del Espectro Autista/epidemiología , Trastornos del Desarrollo del Lenguaje/diagnóstico , Trastornos del Desarrollo del Lenguaje/epidemiología , Desarrollo del Lenguaje , Escalas de Wechsler , Trastorno del Espectro Autista/psicología , Niño , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Trastornos del Desarrollo del Lenguaje/psicología , Pruebas del Lenguaje/normas , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Padres/psicología , Valor Predictivo de las Pruebas , Estudios Prospectivos , Habilidades Sociales , Escalas de Wechsler/normas
18.
J Autism Dev Disord ; 37(2): 251-9, 2007 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16865546

RESUMEN

Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) and children with Obsessive Compulsive Disorder (OCD) were compared on a range of repetitive behaviours. Parents reported similar levels of sameness behaviour and repetitive movements in the clinical groups, although children with OCD engaged in more repetitive behaviour focussed around routines and rituals. Children with OCD reported more compulsions and obsessions than children with ASD; both groups reported more compulsions and obsessions than a typically developing comparison group. Types of compulsions and obsessions tended to be less sophisticated in children with ASD than those with OCD. Sameness behaviour was more prevalent in younger children with OCD, but for children with ASD, age was not significantly related to sameness behaviour, repetitive movements, compulsions, or obsessions.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno Autístico/epidemiología , Trastornos del Conocimiento/epidemiología , Trastorno Obsesivo Compulsivo/epidemiología , Trastorno de Movimiento Estereotipado/epidemiología , Trastorno Autístico/diagnóstico , Niño , Trastornos del Conocimiento/diagnóstico , Manual Diagnóstico y Estadístico de los Trastornos Mentales , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Trastorno Obsesivo Compulsivo/diagnóstico , Prevalencia , Índice de Severidad de la Enfermedad , Trastorno de Movimiento Estereotipado/diagnóstico
20.
Autism ; 21(3): 344-356, 2017 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27178996

RESUMEN

We compared loss and gain in communication from 1 to 2 years in children later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder (n = 41), language impairment (n = 110) and in children with typical language development at 7 years (n = 831). Participants were selected from a prospective population cohort study of child language (the Early Language in Victoria Study). Parent-completed communication tools were used. As a group, children with autism spectrum disorder demonstrated slower median skill gain, with an increasing gap between trajectories compared to children with typical development and language impairment. A proportion from all groups lost skills in at least one domain (autism spectrum disorder (41%), language impairment (30%), typical development (26%)), with more children with autism spectrum disorder losing skills in more than one domain (autism spectrum disorder (47%), language impairment (15%, p = 0.0003), typical development (16%, p < 0.001)). Loss was most common for all groups in the domain of 'emotion and eye gaze' but with a higher proportion for children with autism spectrum disorder (27%; language impairment (12%, p = 0.03), typical development (14%, p = 0.03)). A higher proportion of children with autism spectrum disorder also lost skills in gesture (p = 0.01), sounds (p = 0.009) and understanding (p = 0.004) compared to children with typical development but not with language impairment. These findings add to our understanding of early communication development and highlight that loss is not unique to autism spectrum disorder.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno del Espectro Autista/psicología , Desarrollo del Lenguaje , Padres/psicología , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Lactante , Masculino , Vocabulario
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA