Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 43
Filtrar
Más filtros

Banco de datos
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Behav Res Methods ; 55(7): 3494-3503, 2023 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36223007

RESUMEN

Currently, the design standards for single-case experimental designs (SCEDs) are based on validity considerations as prescribed by the What Works Clearinghouse. However, there is a need for design considerations such as power based on statistical analyses. We compute and derive power using computations for (AB)k designs with multiple cases which are common in SCEDs. Our computations show that effect size has the maximum impact on power followed by the number of subjects and then the number of phase reversals. An effect size of 0.75 or higher, at least one set of phase reversals (i.e., where k > 1), and at least three subjects showed high power. The latter two conditions agree with current standards about either having at least an ABAB design or a multiple baseline design with three subjects to meet design standards. An effect size of 0.75 or higher is not uncommon in SCEDs either. Autocorrelations, the number of time-points per phase, and intraclass correlations had a smaller but non-negligible impact on power. In sum, power analyses in the present study show that conditions to meet power requirements are not unreasonable in SCEDs. The software code to compute power is available on GitHub for the use of the reader.


Asunto(s)
Proyectos de Investigación , Humanos
2.
Neuropsychol Rehabil ; 24(3-4): 528-53, 2014.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23862576

RESUMEN

We describe a standardised mean difference statistic (d) for single-case designs that is equivalent to the usual d in between-groups experiments. We show how it can be used to summarise treatment effects over cases within a study, to do power analyses in planning new studies and grant proposals, and to meta-analyse effects across studies of the same question. We discuss limitations of this d-statistic, and possible remedies to them. Even so, this d-statistic is better founded statistically than other effect size measures for single-case design, and unlike many general linear model approaches such as multilevel modelling or generalised additive models, it produces a standardised effect size that can be integrated over studies with different outcome measures. SPSS macros for both effect size computation and power analysis are available.


Asunto(s)
Proyectos de Investigación/estadística & datos numéricos , Humanos , Metaanálisis como Asunto
3.
Behav Res Methods ; 45(3): 813-21, 2013 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23239070

RESUMEN

Researchers in the single-case design tradition have debated the size and importance of the observed autocorrelations in those designs. All of the past estimates of the autocorrelation in that literature have taken the observed autocorrelation estimates as the data to be used in the debate. However, estimates of the autocorrelation are subject to great sampling error when the design has a small number of time points, as is typically the situation in single-case designs. Thus, a given observed autocorrelation may greatly over- or underestimate the corresponding population parameter. This article presents Bayesian estimates of the autocorrelation that greatly reduce the role of sampling error, as compared to past estimators. Simpler empirical Bayes estimates are presented first, in order to illustrate the fundamental notions of autocorrelation sampling error and shrinkage, followed by fully Bayesian estimates, and the difference between the two is explained. Scripts to do the analyses are available as supplemental materials. The analyses are illustrated using two examples from the single-case design literature. Bayesian estimation warrants wider use, not only in debates about the size of autocorrelations, but also in statistical methods that require an independent estimate of the autocorrelation to analyze the data.


Asunto(s)
Teorema de Bayes , Modelos Estadísticos , Interpretación Estadística de Datos , Humanos , Análisis de Regresión , Proyectos de Investigación , Tamaño de la Muestra , Sesgo de Selección
4.
Cogn Behav Ther ; 40(1): 15-33, 2011.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21337212

RESUMEN

It is essential that outcome research permit clear conclusions to be drawn about the efficacy of interventions. The common practice of nesting therapists within conditions can pose important methodological challenges that affect interpretation, particularly if the study is not powered to account for the nested design. An obstacle to the optimal design of these studies is the lack of data about the intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC), which measures the statistical dependencies introduced by nesting. To begin the development of a public database of ICC estimates, the authors investigated ICCs for a variety outcomes reported in 20 psychotherapy outcome studies. The magnitude of the 495 ICC estimates varied widely across measures and studies. The authors provide recommendations regarding how to select and aggregate ICC estimates for power calculations and show how researchers can use ICC estimates to choose the number of patients and therapists that will optimize power. Attention to these recommendations will strengthen the validity of inferences drawn from psychotherapy studies that nest therapists within conditions.


Asunto(s)
Ensayos Clínicos como Asunto/métodos , Psicoterapia/métodos , Proyectos de Investigación , Humanos , Evaluación de Resultado en la Atención de Salud , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Resultado del Tratamiento
5.
Behav Res Methods ; 43(4): 971-80, 2011 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21656107

RESUMEN

This article reports the results of a study that located, digitized, and coded all 809 single-case designs appearing in 113 studies in the year 2008 in 21 journals in a variety of fields in psychology and education. Coded variables included the specific kind of design, number of cases per study, number of outcomes, data points and phases per case, and autocorrelations for each case. Although studies of the effects of interventions are a minority in these journals, within that category, single-case designs are used more frequently than randomized or nonrandomized experiments. The modal study uses a multiple-baseline design with 20 data points for each of three or four cases, where the aim of the intervention is to increase the frequency of a desired behavior; but these characteristics vary widely over studies. The average autocorrelation is near to but significantly different from zero; but autocorrelations are significantly heterogeneous. The results have implications for the contributions of single-case designs to evidence-based practice and suggest a number of future research directions.


Asunto(s)
Proyectos de Investigación , Humanos
7.
Am J Public Health ; 98(8): 1418-24, 2008 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18556603

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVES: We reviewed published individually randomized group treatment (IRGT) trials to assess researchers' awareness of within-group correlation and determine whether appropriate design and analytic methods were used to test for treatment effectiveness. METHODS: We assessed sample size and analytic methods in IRGT trials published in 6 public health and behavioral health journals between 2002 and 2006. RESULTS: Our review included 34 articles; in 32 (94.1%) of these articles, inappropriate analytic methods were used. In only 1 article did the researchers claim that expected intraclass correlations (ICCs) were taken into account in sample size estimation; in most articles, sample size was not mentioned or ICCs were ignored in the reported calculations. CONCLUSIONS: Trials in which individuals are randomly assigned to study conditions and treatments administered in groups may induce within-group correlation, violating the assumption of independence underlying commonly used statistical methods. Methods that take expected ICCs into account should be used in reexamining past studies and planning future studies to ensure that interventions are not judged effective solely on the basis of statistical artifacts. We strongly encourage investigators to report ICCs from IRGT trials and describe study characteristics clearly to aid these efforts.


Asunto(s)
Biometría/métodos , Interpretación Estadística de Datos , Ensayos Clínicos Controlados Aleatorios como Asunto/métodos , Proyectos de Investigación , Humanos , Práctica de Salud Pública , Tamaño de la Muestra
8.
Eval Rev ; 42(2): 248-280, 2018 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30060688

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Randomized experiments yield unbiased estimates of treatment effect, but such experiments are not always feasible. So researchers have searched for conditions under which randomized and nonrandomized experiments can yield the same answer. This search requires well-justified and informative correspondence criteria, that is, criteria by which we can judge if the results from an appropriately adjusted nonrandomized experiment well-approximate results from randomized experiments. Past criteria have relied exclusively on frequentist statistics, using criteria such as whether results agree in sign or statistical significance or whether results differ significantly from each other. OBJECTIVES: In this article, we show how Bayesian correspondence criteria offer more varied, nuanced, and informative answers than those from frequentist approaches. RESEARCH DESIGN: We describe the conceptual bases of Bayesian correspondence criteria and then illustrate many possibilities using an example that compares results from a randomized experiment to results from a parallel nonequivalent comparison group experiment in which participants could choose their condition. RESULTS: Results suggest that, in this case, the quasi-experiment reasonably approximated the randomized experiment. CONCLUSIONS: We conclude with a discussion of the advantages (computation of relevant quantities, interpretation, and estimation of quantities of interest for policy), disadvantages, and limitations of Bayesian correspondence criteria. We believe that in most circumstances, the advantages of Bayesian approaches far outweigh the disadvantages.


Asunto(s)
Teorema de Bayes , Investigación Empírica , Estudios de Evaluación como Asunto , Ensayos Clínicos Controlados Aleatorios como Asunto , Sesgo , Puntaje de Propensión , Proyectos de Investigación
9.
Psychol Bull ; 132(4): 524-8; discussion 533-7, 2006 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16822163

RESUMEN

The H. Bösch, F. Steinkamp, and E. Boller meta-analysis reaches mixed and cautious conclusions about the possibility of psychokinesis. The authors argue that, for both methodological and philosophical reasons, it is nearly impossible to draw any conclusions from this body of research. The authors do not agree that any significant effect at all, no matter how small, is fundamentally important (Bösch et al., 2006, p. 517), and they suggest that psychokinesis researchers focus either on producing larger effects or on specifying the conditions under which they would be willing to accept the null hypothesis.


Asunto(s)
Cinesis , Procesos Mentales , Teoría Psicológica , Humanos , Matemática , Parapsicología/métodos , Psicología/métodos
10.
J Clin Epidemiol ; 76: 82-8, 2016 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27079848

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVES: We reanalyzed data from a previous randomized crossover design that administered high or low doses of intravenous immunoglobulin (IgG) to 12 patients with hypogammaglobulinaemia over 12 time points, with crossover after time 6. The objective was to see if results corresponded when analyzed as a set of single-case experimental designs vs. as a usual randomized controlled trial (RCT). STUDY DESIGN AND SETTINGS: Two blinded statisticians independently analyzed results. One analyzed the RCT comparing mean outcomes of group A (high dose IgG) to group B (low dose IgG) at the usual trial end point (time 6 in this case). The other analyzed all 12 time points for the group B patients as six single-case experimental designs analyzed together in a Bayesian nonlinear framework. RESULTS: In the randomized trial, group A [M = 794.93; standard deviation (SD) = 90.48] had significantly higher serum IgG levels at time six than group B (M = 283.89; SD = 71.10) (t = 10.88; df = 10; P < 0.001), yielding a mean difference of MD = 511.05 [standard error (SE) = 46.98]. For the single-case experimental designs, the effect from an intrinsically nonlinear regression was also significant and comparable in size with overlapping confidence intervals: MD = 495.00, SE = 54.41, and t = 495.00/54.41 = 9.10. Subsequent exploratory analyses indicated that how trend was modeled made a difference to these conclusions. CONCLUSIONS: The results of single-case experimental designs accurately approximated results from an RCT, although more work is needed to understand the conditions under which this holds.


Asunto(s)
Agammaglobulinemia/tratamiento farmacológico , Investigación Biomédica/métodos , Inmunoglobulinas/administración & dosificación , Ensayos Clínicos Controlados Aleatorios como Asunto , Proyectos de Investigación , Estadística como Asunto/métodos , Administración Intravenosa , Teorema de Bayes , Relación Dosis-Respuesta a Droga , Humanos , Factores de Tiempo
11.
J Appl Behav Anal ; 49(3): 656-73, 2016 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27174301

RESUMEN

The published literature often underrepresents studies that do not find evidence for a treatment effect; this is often called publication bias. Literature reviews that fail to include such studies may overestimate the size of an effect. Only a few studies have examined publication bias in single-case design (SCD) research, but those studies suggest that publication bias may occur. This study surveyed SCD researchers about publication preferences in response to simulated SCD results that show a range of small to large effects. Results suggest that SCD researchers are more likely to submit manuscripts that show large effects for publication and are more likely to recommend acceptance of manuscripts that show large effects when they act as a reviewer. A nontrivial minority of SCD researchers (4% to 15%) would drop 1 or 2 cases from the study if the effect size is small and then submit for publication. This article ends with a discussion of implications for publication practices in SCD research.


Asunto(s)
Sesgo de Publicación , Proyectos de Investigación , Investigadores/psicología , Humanos , Investigadores/estadística & datos numéricos , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
12.
J Clin Epidemiol ; 76: 18-46, 2016 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26272791

RESUMEN

N-of-1 trials are a useful tool for clinicians who want to determine the effectiveness of a treatment in a particular individual. The reporting of N-of-1 trials has been variable and incomplete, hindering their usefulness in clinical decision making and by future researchers. This document presents the CONSORT (Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials) extension for N-of-1 trials (CENT 2015). CENT 2015 extends the CONSORT 2010 guidance to facilitate the preparation and appraisal of reports of an individual N-of-1 trial or a series of prospectively planned, multiple, crossover N-of-1 trials. CENT 2015 elaborates on 14 items of the CONSORT 2010 checklist, totalling 25 checklist items (44 sub-items), and recommends diagrams to help authors document the progress of one participant through a trial or more than one participant through a trial or series of trials, as applicable. Examples of good reporting and evidence based rationale for CENT 2015 checklist items are provided.


Asunto(s)
Investigación Biomédica/normas , Ensayos Clínicos como Asunto/normas , Guías como Asunto , Edición/normas , Proyectos de Investigación/normas , Informe de Investigación/normas , Terminología como Asunto , Humanos
13.
J Consult Clin Psychol ; 73(1): 6-14, 2005 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15709827

RESUMEN

This meta-analysis summarizes results from 30 randomized experiments that compare behavioral marital therapy with no-treatment control with distressed couples. Results showed that behavioral marital therapy is significantly more effective than no treatment (d=.585). Although behavioral marital therapy research studies tend to be conducted under conditions that are less clinically representative than other samples of studies, representativeness was not significantly related to outcome. However, evidence also suggested that publication bias may exist in this literature whereby small sample studies with small effects are systematically missing compared with other studies. This bias may inflate the effects of behavioral marital therapies reported in previous meta-analyses, though we also explore a number of alternative explanations for this small sample bias.


Asunto(s)
Terapia Conductista/métodos , Terapia Conyugal/métodos , Humanos , Sesgo de Publicación , Ensayos Clínicos Controlados Aleatorios como Asunto
14.
J Consult Clin Psychol ; 73(5): 924-35, 2005 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16287392

RESUMEN

When treatments are administered in groups, clients interact in ways that lead to violations of a key assumption of most statistical analyses-the assumption of independence of observations. The resulting dependencies, when not properly accounted for, can increase Type I errors dramatically. Of the 33 studies of group-administered treatment on the empirically supported treatments list, none appropriately analyzed their data. The current authors provide corrections that can be applied to improper analyses. After the corrections, only 12.4% to 68.2% of tests that were originally reported as significant remained significant, depending on what assumptions were made about how large the dependencies among observations really are. Of the 33 studies, 6-19 studies no longer had any significant results after correction. The authors end by providing recommendations for researchers planning group-administered treatment research.


Asunto(s)
Terapia Conductista/normas , Investigación Empírica , Medicina Basada en la Evidencia , Psicoterapia de Grupo/normas , Resultado del Tratamiento , Análisis de Varianza , Terapia Conductista/estadística & datos numéricos , Sesgo , Interpretación Estadística de Datos , Humanos , Psicoterapia de Grupo/estadística & datos numéricos , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Proyectos de Investigación
15.
Eval Rev ; 29(6): 530-58, 2005 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16244051

RESUMEN

Propensity score analysis is a relatively recent statistical innovation that is useful in the analysis of data from quasi-experiments. The goal of propensity score analysis is to balance two non-equivalent groups on observed covariates to get more accurate estimates of the effects of a treatment on which the two groups differ. This article presents a general introduction to propensity score analysis, provides an example using data from a quasi-experiment compared to a benchmark randomized experiment, offers practical advice about how to do such analyses, and discusses some limitations of the approach. It also presents the first detailed instructions to appear in the literature on how to use classification tree analysis and bagging for classification trees in the construction of propensity scores. The latter two examples serve as an introduction for researchers interested in computing propensity scores using more complex classification algorithms known as ensemble methods.


Asunto(s)
Modelos Estadísticos , Evaluación de Resultado en la Atención de Salud/estadística & datos numéricos , Ensayos Clínicos Controlados Aleatorios como Asunto/métodos , Proyectos de Investigación , Algoritmos , Sesgo , Humanos , Ensayos Clínicos Controlados Aleatorios como Asunto/estadística & datos numéricos
16.
Eval Rev ; 29(3): 241-67, 2005 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15860765

RESUMEN

This study describes a method for incorporating external estimates of intraclass correlation to improve the precision for the analysis of an existing group-randomized trial. The authors use a random-effects meta-analytic approach to pool the information across studies, which takes into account any interstudy heterogeneity that may exist. This approach can be used in several different situations to estimate the degrees of freedom available for an adjusted test of the intervention effect in a study where the challenges of group-randomized trials were not fully considered when the study was planned. The authors discuss the limitations of this approach and the circumstances in which it is likely to be helpful.


Asunto(s)
Proyectos de Investigación/estadística & datos numéricos , Humanos , Metaanálisis como Asunto , Modelos Estadísticos , Ensayos Clínicos Controlados Aleatorios como Asunto
17.
Eval Rev ; 29(3): 268-86, 2005 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15860766

RESUMEN

This article builds on the previous article by Blitstein et al. (2005), which showed how external estimates of intraclass correlation can be used to improve the precision for the analysis of an existing group randomized trial. The authors extend that work to sample size estimation and power analysis for future group-randomized trials. Often this approach will allow a smaller study than would otherwise be possible without sacrificing statistical power. Such studies are needed, for example, as pilot studies to help plan for a full-scale efficacy trial, as replication studies, or in situations in which resource constraints prohibit a larger trial. The authors discuss the circumstances under which this strategy will be most helpful and the risks associated with conducting smaller studies.


Asunto(s)
Proyectos de Investigación/estadística & datos numéricos , Humanos , Metaanálisis como Asunto , Modelos Estadísticos , Ensayos Clínicos Controlados Aleatorios como Asunto
18.
Res Synth Methods ; 6(3): 219-20, 2015 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26097018

RESUMEN

This issue of Research Synthesis Methods is devoted to discussion of the origins of modern meta-analysis. Three articles by pioneers in development meta-analysis are by Gene Glass, Frank Schmidt, and Robert Rosenthal, respectively. They reflect on their own experiences about how they made these developments. The fourth article is by William Shadish, and seeks to analyze the impact of meta-analysis and the reasons why meta-analysis developed at the time it did, and by the people who did so. The articles are followed by commentaries by Douglas Altman, Iain Chalmers, Harris Cooper, Kay Dickersin, Larry Hedges, David Hoaglin, and Hannah Rothstein, who each comment on both the four target articles and on their own perspectives about how and why meta-analysis developed when and how it did.


Asunto(s)
Investigación Biomédica/historia , Ensayos Clínicos como Asunto/historia , Interpretación Estadística de Datos , Metaanálisis como Asunto , Proyectos de Investigación , Literatura de Revisión como Asunto , Historia del Siglo XX , Historia del Siglo XXI
19.
Res Synth Methods ; 6(3): 246-64, 2015 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26212600

RESUMEN

This article looks at the impact of meta-analysis and then explores why meta-analysis was developed at the time and by the scholars it did in the social sciences in the 1970s. For the first problem, impact, it examines the impact of meta-analysis using citation network analysis. The impact is seen in the sciences, arts and humanities, and on such contemporaneous developments as multilevel modeling, medical statistics, qualitative methods, program evaluation, and single-case design. Using a constrained snowball sample of citations, we highlight key articles that are either most highly cited or most central to the systematic review network. Then, the article examines why meta-analysis came to be in the 1970s in the social sciences through the work of Gene Glass, Robert Rosenthal, and Frank Schmidt, each of whom developed similar theories of meta-analysis at about the same time. The article ends by explaining how Simonton's chance configuration theory and Campbell's evolutionary epistemology can illuminate why meta-analysis occurred with these scholars when it did and not in medical sciences.


Asunto(s)
Investigación Biomédica/historia , Ensayos Clínicos como Asunto/historia , Interpretación Estadística de Datos , Metaanálisis como Asunto , Proyectos de Investigación , Literatura de Revisión como Asunto , Historia del Siglo XX , Historia del Siglo XXI
20.
Psychol Methods ; 20(1): 26-42, 2015 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24885341

RESUMEN

Single-case designs (SCDs) are short time series that assess intervention effects by measuring units repeatedly over time in both the presence and absence of treatment. This article introduces a statistical technique for analyzing SCD data that has not been much used in psychological and educational research: generalized additive models (GAMs). In parametric regression, the researcher must choose a functional form to impose on the data, for example, that trend over time is linear. GAMs reverse this process by letting the data inform the choice of functional form. In this article we review the problem that trend poses in SCDs, discuss how current SCD analytic methods approach trend, describe GAMs as a possible solution, suggest a GAM model testing procedure for examining the presence of trend in SCDs, present a small simulation to show the statistical properties of GAMs, and illustrate the procedure on 3 examples of different lengths. Results suggest that GAMs may be very useful both as a form of sensitivity analysis for checking the plausibility of assumptions about trend and as a primary data analysis strategy for testing treatment effects. We conclude with a discussion of some problems with GAMs and some future directions for research on the application of GAMs to SCDs.


Asunto(s)
Investigación Biomédica/estadística & datos numéricos , Modelos Estadísticos , Proyectos de Investigación/estadística & datos numéricos , Humanos
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA