Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 24
Filtrar
1.
Anim Cogn ; 27(1): 29, 2024 Apr 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38558203

RESUMEN

In the first two experiments an empty tube open at one end was placed in different locations. Male hamsters, tested one at a time, tended to stay close to the tube or in it. During the first minute of the first 4 sessions of Experiment 3, the hamster was unrestrained. If it entered the tube, it was locked within the tube. If it did not enter the tube during the first min, it was placed in it, and the tube was locked. Fifteen min later, the tube was opened, and the hamster was unrestrained for a further 20 min. The tube remained open during Session 5. Hamsters spent more time near the tube than predicted by chance and continued to enter the tube although tube-occupancy duration did not differ from chance levels. In Experiment 4, male rats were tested in two groups: rats in one group had been previously trapped in a tube and rats in the other group allowed to freely explore the test space. For the first two min of each of four 20-min sessions, trapped-group subjects were permitted to move about the chamber unless they entered the tube. In that case, they were locked in for the remainder of the session. If, after two min, they did not enter the tube, they were locked in it for the remaining 18 min. Free rats were unrestricted in all sessions. In Session 5, when both groups were permitted to move freely in the chamber, trapped and free rats spent more time in and near the tube than predicted by chance. These data show tube restraint does not seem to distress either hamsters or rats.


Asunto(s)
Empatía , Roedores , Humanos , Ratas , Masculino , Animales
2.
Chem Senses ; 43(4): 239-247, 2018 04 23.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29688449

RESUMEN

In the course of our ongoing studies of odor-cued taste avoidance (OCTA) to measure olfactory capabilities in animals, we observed that mice could rapidly learn to use the vapor of the classical bitterant quinine hydrochloride to avoid contact with the tastant. Here we expand on this observation to determine which among several compounds generally classed as bitter could be detected at a distance. Since mice were initially naïve we were able to assess whether the vapors of the bitter compounds tested were innately aversive as are their tastes. CD-1 mice could readily use vapor cues from quinine hydrochloride, denatonium benzoate (DB), and 6-propyl-2-thiouracil to avoid their taste. Although mice did not hesitate to make contact with these solutions on their first exposure, they did learn to do so typically after only 1 or 2 exposures. Bilaterally bulbectomized mice did not learn or retain the ability to avoid quinine and DB solutions by vapor alone, implicating olfaction as the mode of detection. Saturated aqueous solutions of sucrose octaacetate and caffeine which are bitter to humans and some strains of mice were not aversive in our studies. The very low vapor concentrations of the 3 bitterant solutions that mice detected at a distance, suggest that impurities in the reagent grade solutions, rather than the bitter molecules themselves were the basis of detection. Implications of these findings for taste testing and the role of odor in food acceptance/rejections decisions are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Agentes Aversivos/química , Reacción de Prevención/fisiología , Olfato , Gusto/fisiología , Animales , Cafeína/química , Señales (Psicología) , Femenino , Ratones , Bulbo Olfatorio/cirugía , Propiltiouracilo/química , Compuestos de Amonio Cuaternario/química , Quinina/química , Sacarosa/análogos & derivados , Sacarosa/química
3.
Anim Cogn ; 20(2): 299-308, 2017 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27822786

RESUMEN

In Experiment 1, rats choosing in an E maze preferred to release a rat standing in a pool of water to dry ground over a rat already standing on dry ground. Five additional experiments showed that the choosing rat's preference for releasing the wet rat was maintained by two separable outcomes: (1) the social contact offered by the released rat and (2) the reinforcing value of proximity to a pool of water. These results call into question Sato et al.'s (Anim Cogn 18:1039-1047, 2015) claim to have demonstrated that a rat's releasing of a wet rat to dry ground is empathically motivated.


Asunto(s)
Empatía , Conducta Social , Animales , Ratas , Ratas Sprague-Dawley
4.
Chem Senses ; 40(4): 269-78, 2015 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25787943

RESUMEN

In odor-cued taste avoidance (OCTA), thirsty mice, offered either an odorized nonaversive fluid (S+) or an odorized aversive fluid (S-), quickly learn to use odor to avoid drinking the S-. Acquisition of both odor detection and odor discrimination tasks is very rapid with learning evidenced in most cases by either long response times or total avoidance on the second presentation of the S- stimulus. OCTA is perhaps one of the simplest conditioning procedures for assessing olfaction in mice; it requires only a test box, drinkometer circuit, and thirsty mice accustomed to drinking in the apparatus. Its advantages over the most commonly used alternatives, habituation-dishabituation, and the mouse dig test, are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Reacción de Prevención/fisiología , Señales (Psicología) , Conducta de Ingestión de Líquido/fisiología , Odorantes , Olfato/fisiología , Gusto/fisiología , Animales , Femenino , Ratones , Bulbo Olfatorio/fisiología
5.
Anim Cogn ; 17(3): 609-18, 2014 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24126919

RESUMEN

Ben-Ami Bartal et al. (Science 334:1427-1430, 2011) showed that a rat in an open space (free rat) would touch the front door of a restraining tube to open its rear door, thereby enabling a rat trapped within (trapped rat) to enter a larger space that was farther away from the free rat. Since opening the rear door distanced the trapped rat from the free rat, Ben-Ami Bartal et al. argued free-rat behavior could not be motivated by the pursuit of social contact. Instead, this rat was empathically motivated, its goal being to reduce the presumed distress of the rat trapped in the restraining tube. In two experiments, we show that (a) a free rat will not learn to touch the front door to open the rear door when it is the first condition of the experiment; (b) over time, a trapped rat will often return to a restraining tube despite its presumed aversiveness; and (c) a free rat experienced in touching the front door will continue to touch it even if touching does not free the trapped rat. We explain these results and Ben-Ami Bartal et al.'s in terms of two processes, neophobia and the pursuit of social contact. When first placed in a restraining tube, neophobia causes the trapped rat to escape the tube when the rear door is opened. Across sessions, neophobia diminishes, permitting the rats' pursuit of social contact to emerge and dominate free- and trapped-rat behavior.


Asunto(s)
Empatía , Conducta de Ayuda , Ratas Sprague-Dawley/psicología , Conducta Social , Animales , Femenino , Ratas
6.
Eur J Neurosci ; 31(6): 1108-16, 2010 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20377623

RESUMEN

Although it is now established that sensory neurons in both the main olfactory epithelium and the vomeronasal organ may be activated by both general and pheromonal odorants, it remains unclear what initiates sampling by the vomeronasal organ. Anterograde transport of wheat germ agglutinin-horseradish peroxidase was used to determine that adequate intranasal syringing with zinc sulfate interrupted all inputs to the main olfactory bulb but left intact those to the accessory olfactory bulb. Adult male treated mice were frankly anosmic when tested with pheromonal and non-pheromonal odors and failed to engage in aggressive behavior. Treated juvenile females failed to show puberty acceleration subsequent to exposure to bedding from adult males. Activation of the immediate early gene c-Fos and electrovomeronasogram recording confirmed the integrity of the vomeronasal system in zinc sulfate-treated mice. These results support the hypothesis that odor detection by the main olfactory epithelium is required to initiate sampling by the vomeronasal system.


Asunto(s)
Bulbo Olfatorio/fisiología , Mucosa Olfatoria/fisiología , Factores de Edad , Agresión/fisiología , Análisis de Varianza , Animales , Animales Recién Nacidos , Conducta Animal , Discriminación en Psicología/efectos de los fármacos , Femenino , Masculino , Ratones , Ratones Endogámicos C57BL , Vías Nerviosas/efectos de los fármacos , Vías Nerviosas/fisiología , Odorantes , Bulbo Olfatorio/efectos de los fármacos , Mucosa Olfatoria/efectos de los fármacos , Proteínas Proto-Oncogénicas c-fos/metabolismo , Olfato/fisiología , Sulfato de Zinc/farmacología
7.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 113(3): 644-656, 2020 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32239520

RESUMEN

Two experiments evaluated whether rats' occupancy of a restraint tube is reinforcing. In Experiment 1, each rat in the 0-min group moved freely in a chamber where a wall blocked access to a restraint tube. After 10 min the wall was removed, permitting 15 min of chamber access and tube entry. The other 2 groups were locked in the tube for 10 and 20 min respectively before release into the chamber for 15 min. Across sessions, rats locked up for 10 and 20 min entered the tube more frequently than rats in the 0-min group, and during the first 2 sessions rats in the 20-min group stayed in the tube longer than the other groups. Over sessions this difference disappeared. However, for all groups and sessions the mean percentage of session time in the tube exceeded chance expectations. This result suggests tube occupation was reinforcing. In Experiment 2's Phase 1, rats could enter an open tube. On exiting, the tube door closed. A lever press opened the door for the rest of the 1-hr session. In Phase 2, these rats were locked in the tube for 10 min before the door opened. Upon exiting, the door closed. As in Phase 1, a lever press opened the door for the rest of the session. The latency between pressing and tube entry decreased over sessions, indicating that tube entry reinforced lever pressing. These results are difficult to reconcile with accounts of rat empathy based on the thesis that tube restraint distresses occupants.


Asunto(s)
Ratas Sprague-Dawley/psicología , Restricción Física/psicología , Recompensa , Animales , Condicionamiento Operante , Masculino , Ratas , Factores de Tiempo
8.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 110(2): 267-274, 2018 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30047125

RESUMEN

This report evaluates whether a rat releasing a trapped rat from a restraint tube is better explained as due to its empathic motivation or to the pursuit of social contact. In the first condition, each of six rats chose in an E maze between entering an empty goal box versus entering a goal box where its entrance caused a rat trapped in a restraint tube to be released. Rats preferred the goal box with the trapped rat over the empty goal box. In the second condition, these rats chose between releasing a restraint-tube-trapped rat in one goal box and another rat in the second goal box that was not locked into its restraint tube. Rats showed no preference between alternatives. In the third condition, rats chose between a goal box containing a rat with an open restraint tube and an empty goal box. Rats preferred the rat with the open restraint tube over the empty goal box. These results support attributing the response of releasing a rat from a restraint tube to the reinforcing power of social contact rather than interpreting this response as empathically motivated.


Asunto(s)
Empatía , Conducta Social , Animales , Conducta de Elección , Masculino , Aprendizaje por Laberinto , Ratas , Ratas Sprague-Dawley , Restricción Física/psicología
9.
J Neurosci ; 26(39): 9892-901, 2006 Sep 27.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17005853

RESUMEN

We assessed (1) whether the enantiomers of terpinen-4-ol, odorants that activate nearly identical areas of the olfactory bulb, are more difficult to discriminate than those of carvone, odorants that activate different areas of the olfactory bulb, and (2) whether olfactory bulb lesions that disrupt the pattern of bulbar activation produced by these enantiomers degraded the ability of rats to discriminate between them. In psychophysical tests, normal rats discriminated between the enantiomers of terpinen-4-ol and of carvone equally well. Surgical lesions that removed the majority of bulbar glomeruli activated by these odorants (as demonstrated in previous olfactory bulb studies using intrinsic optical imaging and 2-deoxyglucose) resulted in increased detection thresholds but few or no deficits in discriminating between suprathreshold concentrations of the enantiomers. These results fail to confirm predictions based on 2-deoxyglucose maps of bulbar activity that enantiomers of terpinen-4-ol should be more difficult to discriminate than those of carvone and that the ability to discriminate between enantiomers of an odorant are based on differences in patterns of bulbar activation revealed in such maps.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico , Discriminación en Psicología , Monoterpenos/química , Odorantes , Bulbo Olfatorio/fisiología , Trastornos de la Percepción/fisiopatología , Receptores Odorantes/fisiología , Terpenos/química , Acetatos , Animales , Condicionamiento Operante , Monoterpenos Ciclohexánicos , Masculino , Monoterpenos/farmacología , Bulbo Olfatorio/lesiones , Bulbo Olfatorio/patología , Bulbo Olfatorio/cirugía , Trastornos de la Percepción/etiología , Desempeño Psicomotor , Ratas , Ratas Long-Evans , Receptores Odorantes/efectos de los fármacos , Estereoisomerismo , Técnicas Estereotáxicas , Terpenos/farmacología
10.
Laryngoscope ; 117(4): 743-9, 2007 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17415147

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: We assessed whether a nasal spray containing zinc gluconate (ZG) compromises the integrity of olfactory epithelium and olfactory function. METHODS: Axonal transport of horseradish peroxidase from olfactory epithelium to the olfactory bulb was studied in 2- to 21-day survival mice given intranasal injections of 2, 8, or 50 microL of ZG (approximately 4, 15, and 94 times the equivalent recommended human dose). Other similarly treated mice were tested using precision olfactometry to detect and discriminate odors. RESULTS: Anatomic changes were graded as a function of dose and survival time. Two microliter injections had no discernable effect. while the 50 microL volume produced substantial deafferentation of input to the olfactory bulb in short-survival cases. Nearly complete restitution of input occurred within 3 weeks. At each volume and survival time, zinc sulfate (ZS) had a greater effect. Behaviorally, 2 microL and 8 microL ZG-treated mice and those given multiple injections of 2 microL ZG performed as well as controls, whereas those given 50 microL were hyposmic but not anosmic. ZS-treated mice performed more poorly, and those injected with 50 microL were anosmic for the first 8 to 10 test days. CONCLUSIONS: A massive dose of a ZG nasal spray did cause a transient disruption of the olfactory epithelium and compromised olfaction. More moderate volumes, even those far in excess of a recommended dose, were largely without effect on odor detection and discrimination tasks. These outcomes fail to support the claims from recent clinical case reports that use of a ZG-containing nasal spray can produce anosmia.


Asunto(s)
Gluconatos/efectos adversos , Trastornos del Olfato/inducido químicamente , Mucosa Olfatoria/efectos de los fármacos , Olfato/efectos de los fármacos , Administración Intranasal , Animales , Modelos Animales de Enfermedad , Esquema de Medicación , Gluconatos/administración & dosificación , Masculino , Ratones , Mucosa Olfatoria/patología , Soluciones
11.
J Neurosci ; 22(10): 4205-16, 2002 May 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12019338

RESUMEN

Rats were trained using olfactometry and operant conditioning to discriminate among homologous fatty acids, homologous aldehydes, and a series of unrelated odors. Their memory for the positive and negative assignment of each odor (tested under extinction) was assessed before and after they had received selective lesions of the olfactory bulbs or injection of the olfactory epithelial toxin 3-methyl indole (3-MI). Response accuracy on the memory test provided a measure of the extent to which treatments altered the remembered perceptual quality of the odors. The degree of deafferentation of the bulb by treatment with 3-MI was assessed using anterograde transport of horseradish peroxidase applied to the olfactory epithelium. Rats treated with 3-MI had a detectable reaction product only in varying numbers of glomeruli on the lateral and, in some cases, posterior medial walls of the olfactory bulb. Bulbar lesions destroyed the dorsal and dorsomedial bulbar areas that have been identified in optical and electrophysiological studies as showing responses to fatty acids. Rats with bulbar lesions had good to near perfect retention on their post-treatment memory test on all odor pairs, as did 3-MI-treated rats that still had substantial input to glomeruli on the lateral or posterior medial wall of the bulb. 3-MI-treated rats with substantially fewer afferent connections had severe retention deficits, particularly for the aldehyde and fatty acid odors, but this loss was secondary to deficits in the ability to discriminate among these odors. The results indicate that input to bulbar areas that are activated by a series of homologous odors may not be essential for odor discrimination and that deafferentation of the majority of bulbar glomeruli may be primarily without effect on odor quality perception as assessed by the memory test. These outcomes point to a much higher degree of redundancy within the olfactory bulb than that envisioned by current combinatorial or odotopic hypotheses of odor quality coding or, alternatively, to mechanisms of odor coding used in the awake behaving animal that have not yet been elucidated.


Asunto(s)
Odorantes , Bulbo Olfatorio/fisiología , Mucosa Olfatoria/fisiología , Olfato/fisiología , Vías Aferentes/efectos de los fármacos , Vías Aferentes/fisiología , Aldehídos/farmacología , Animales , Conducta Animal/efectos de los fármacos , Condicionamiento Operante/efectos de los fármacos , Condicionamiento Operante/fisiología , Discriminación en Psicología/efectos de los fármacos , Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Ácidos Grasos/farmacología , Femenino , Masculino , Memoria/fisiología , Trastornos del Olfato/inducido químicamente , Bulbo Olfatorio/patología , Mucosa Olfatoria/efectos de los fármacos , Ratas , Ratas Long-Evans , Escatol , Olfato/efectos de los fármacos
12.
J Neurosci ; 24(41): 9195-200, 2004 Oct 13.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15483138

RESUMEN

Experimental rats had their right olfactory bulb removed on postnatal day 2 (P2) and their left olfactory bulb removed on P90. Control rats had one or both olfactory bulbs removed on P90. Before and after their adult-stage surgery, rats were trained using olfactometry and operant conditioning to detect and discriminate odors. Anterograde transport of horseradish peroxidase applied to the olfactory epithelium revealed numerous axons of olfactory sensory neurons in the right hemisphere of 27 experimental rats. These axons terminated in glomerular-like clusters within the frontal neocortex (n = 5) or anterior olfactory nucleus with some axons extending into the subventricular epithelium (n = 22). Seventeen of the experimental rats were able to detect a variety of odors and to discriminate between odors. Performance accuracy was related to the location and density of these anomalous inputs; experimental rats with inputs confined to frontal neocortex and those lacking any inputs to the forebrain were anosmic, as were adult-operated bilaterally bulbectomized rats. Our results provide strong support for the contention that, in the absence of the olfactory bulbs, olfactory connections to novel forebrain sites can support both odor detection and odor discrimination.


Asunto(s)
Bulbo Olfatorio/fisiología , Olfato/fisiología , Animales , Conducta Animal/efectos de los fármacos , Conducta Animal/fisiología , Aprendizaje Discriminativo/efectos de los fármacos , Aprendizaje Discriminativo/fisiología , Ésteres/farmacología , Femenino , Masculino , Procedimientos Neuroquirúrgicos , Odorantes , Bulbo Olfatorio/cirugía , Ratas , Ratas Sprague-Dawley , Umbral Sensorial/fisiología , Estimulación Química
13.
J Neurosci ; 24(14): 3703-10, 2004 Apr 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15071119

RESUMEN

It is believed that odor transduction in the mammalian main olfactory system only involves the cAMP-signaling pathway. Here, we report on odor responsiveness in mice with a disrupted cyclic nucleotide-gated (CNG) channel subunit A2. Several odorants, including putative pheromones, can be detected and discriminated by these mice behaviorally. These odors elicit responses in the olfactory epithelium, main olfactory bulb, and olfactory (piriform) cortex of CNGA2 knock-out mice. In addition, responses to odors detected by CNGA2 knock-out mice are relatively insensitive to inhibitors of the cAMP pathway. These results provide strong evidence that cAMP-independent pathways in the main olfactory system of mammals participate in detecting a subset of odors.


Asunto(s)
Canales Iónicos/genética , Olfato/fisiología , Animales , Conducta Animal/efectos de los fármacos , Conducta Animal/fisiología , Condicionamiento Operante , AMP Cíclico/antagonistas & inhibidores , AMP Cíclico/metabolismo , Canales Catiónicos Regulados por Nucleótidos Cíclicos , Aprendizaje Discriminativo/efectos de los fármacos , Aprendizaje Discriminativo/fisiología , Relación Dosis-Respuesta a Droga , Canales Iónicos/deficiencia , Cetonas/farmacología , Ratones , Ratones Endogámicos , Ratones Noqueados , Datos de Secuencia Molecular , Odorantes , Bulbo Olfatorio/fisiología , Mucosa Olfatoria/efectos de los fármacos , Mucosa Olfatoria/fisiología , Vías Olfatorias/fisiología , Feromonas/farmacología , Subunidades de Proteína/deficiencia , Subunidades de Proteína/genética , Proteínas Proto-Oncogénicas c-fos/metabolismo , Pirazinas/farmacología , Umbral Sensorial/fisiología , Transducción de Señal/efectos de los fármacos , Transducción de Señal/genética , Transducción de Señal/fisiología , Olfato/efectos de los fármacos , Olfato/genética , Estimulación Química , Nervio Trigémino/fisiología , Tirosina 3-Monooxigenasa/metabolismo
14.
Pain ; 106(1-2): 101-8, 2003 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14581116

RESUMEN

Emotions have been shown to alter pain perception, but the underlying mechanism is unclear since emotions also affect attention, which itself changes nociceptive transmission. We manipulated independently direction of attention and emotional state, using tasks involving heat pain and pleasant and unpleasant odors. Shifts in attention between the thermal and olfactory modalities did not alter mood or anxiety. Yet, when subjects focused attention on the pain, they perceived it as clearly more intense and somewhat more unpleasant than when they attended to the odor. In contrast, odor valence altered mood, anxiety level, and pain unpleasantness, but did not change the perception of pain intensity. Pain unpleasantness ratings correlated with mood, but not with odor valence, suggesting that emotional changes underlie the selective modulation of pain affect. These results show that emotion and attention differentially alter pain perception and thus invoke at least partially separable neural modulatory circuits.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Emociones/fisiología , Dolor/fisiopatología , Percepción/fisiología , Olfato/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Afecto/fisiología , Femenino , Calor , Humanos , Masculino , Odorantes , Dolor/psicología
15.
Behav Neurosci ; 116(6): 1022-31, 2002 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12492301

RESUMEN

This study investigated the influence of brain docosahexaenoic acid (DHA) deficiency on simple and complex olfactory-based learning and memory in 2nd generation (F2) adult male rats. Rats raised and maintained on either an n-3-adequate or an n-3-deficient diet were tested for acquisition of an olfactory learning set and an olfactory memory task, and for motivation to obtain a water reward. Despite a 76% decrease in brain DHA, n-3-deficient rats were able to acquire most simple 2-odor discrimination tasks but were deficient in the acquisition of a 20-problem olfactory learning set. This deficit could not be attributed to changes in sensory capacity but, instead, appeared to represent a deficit in higher order learning.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos del Conocimiento/fisiopatología , Enfermedades Carenciales/complicaciones , Aprendizaje Discriminativo , Ácidos Docosahexaenoicos/farmacología , Trastornos de la Memoria/etiología , Animales , Química Encefálica , Enfermedades Carenciales/fisiopatología , Enfermedades Carenciales/veterinaria , Masculino , Ratas , Ratas Long-Evans , Olfato
16.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24879990

RESUMEN

The olfactory system is involved in sensory functions, emotional regulation and memory formation. Olfactory bulbectomy in rat has been employed as an animal model of depression for antidepressant discovery studies for many years. Olfaction is impaired in animals suffering from chronic stress, and patients with clinical depression were reported to have decreased olfactory function. It is believed that the neurobiological bases of depression might include dysfunction in the olfactory system. Further, brain stimulation, including nasal based drug delivery could provide novel therapies for management of depression.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno Depresivo/fisiopatología , Bulbo Olfatorio/fisiopatología , Olfato/fisiología , Animales , Trastorno Depresivo/terapia , Modelos Animales de Enfermedad , Humanos , Neurogénesis/fisiología , Bulbo Olfatorio/cirugía
17.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 91(2): 253-5, 2009 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19794837

RESUMEN

An easily constructed and inexpensive battery operated circuit is described for use as a lickometer or contact detector in behavioral studies with rodents.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Animal , Psicología Experimental/instrumentación , Transistores Electrónicos , Animales , Ratones , Ratas , Lengua , Tacto
18.
Chem Senses ; 32(7): 721-5, 2007 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17567630

RESUMEN

Response accuracy and odor-sampling times were used to compare the ability of mice to detect (+)-carvone and (+)-terpinen-4-ol and to discriminate between enantiomers of carvone and of terpinen-4-ol. Except for increased odor sampling when mice were first exposed to the (+)-carvone odor, there was no difference in odor-sampling time or response accuracy in tests of odor detection or in discriminating between enantiomers of these odorants. These results fail to support the suggestion that odorants that produce different patterns of olfactory bulb activation should be easier to discriminate than those that produce much more similar patterns of bulbar activation.


Asunto(s)
Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Monoterpenos/química , Terpenos/química , Acetatos/química , Animales , Monoterpenos Ciclohexánicos , Aprendizaje Discriminativo/fisiología , Masculino , Ratones , Ratones Endogámicos , Odorantes , Tiempo de Reacción , Olfato/fisiología , Estereoisomerismo
19.
Chem Senses ; 32(2): 173-81, 2007 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17151135

RESUMEN

Rats trained to detect propyl acetate and valeric acid and to discriminate between propyl acetate and amyl acetate and between valeric acid and butyric acid were injected with a low dose of 3-methyl indole, a treatment that produces well-defined and selective deafferentation of the olfactory bulbs. Treatment completely deafferented most but not all bulbar loci for aliphatic acids and at least disrupted those for propyl and amyl acetate. In posttreatment tests, experimental rats performed somewhat but not significantly more poorly than controls and about as well on the acid detection and discrimination tasks as on the corresponding acetate tests.


Asunto(s)
Bulbo Olfatorio/efectos de los fármacos , Escatol/farmacología , Olfato/efectos de los fármacos , Acetatos/farmacología , Animales , Ácidos Carboxílicos/farmacología , Masculino , Ratas , Ratas Wistar , Células Receptoras Sensoriales/efectos de los fármacos , Umbral Sensorial
20.
Chem Senses ; 32(5): 445-54, 2007 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17426048

RESUMEN

Response accuracy and odor sample times on positive (S+) and negative (S-) trials were recorded for mice trained on a variety of go, no-go odor detection and discrimination tasks. Odor sample time was relatively stable over extended training on the same task, increased during acquisition of difficult tasks, relatively insensitive to reinforcement magnitude, and, in some cases, provided more information regarding task difficulty and discrimination than did response accuracy. Mice generally sampled longer on S- trials in simple odor detection tasks but longer on S+ trials in odor discrimination tasks.


Asunto(s)
Odorantes , Olfato/fisiología , Animales , Masculino , Ratones , Umbral Sensorial
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA