Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 35
Filtrar
Más filtros

Banco de datos
País/Región como asunto
Tipo del documento
País de afiliación
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
BMC Public Health ; 22(1): 1542, 2022 08 13.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35964046

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Physical activity has numerous health benefits, but participation is lower in disadvantaged communities. 'parkrun' overcomes one of the main barriers for disadvantaged communities, the cost of activities, by providing a free, regular community-based physical activity event for walkers, runners and volunteers. This study assesses equity of access (in terms of distance to the nearest parkrun) stratified by socioeconomic deprivation, and identifies the optimal location for 100 new events to increase equity of access. METHODS: We combined information about population location and socioeconomic deprivation, with information about the location of 403 existing parkrun events, to assess the current level of access by deprivation quintile. We then used a two-step location-allocation analysis (minimising the sum of deprivation-weighted distances) to identify optimal regions, then optimal towns within those regions, as the ideal locations for 100 new parkrun events. RESULTS: Currently, 63.1% of the Australian population lives within 5 km of an event, and the average distance to an event is 14.5 km. A socioeconomic gradient exists, with the most deprived communities having the largest average distance to an event (27.0 km), and the least deprived communities having the best access (living an average 6.6 km from an event). Access improves considerably after the introduction of new event locations with around 68% of the population residing within 5 km of an event, and the average distance to the nearest event approximately 8 km. Most importantly, the improvement in access will be greatest for the most deprived communities (now an average 11 km from an event). CONCLUSIONS: There is a socioeconomic gradient in access to parkrun events. Strategic selection of new parkrun locations will improve equity of access to community physical activity events, and could contribute to enabling greater participation in physical activity by disadvantaged communities.


Asunto(s)
Ejercicio Físico , Poblaciones Vulnerables , Australia , Humanos
2.
J Appl Physiol (1985) ; 106(3): 950-8, 2009 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19118155

RESUMEN

Joint position sense is believed to be mediated by muscle afferent signals. Because a "phantom" hand produced by a sensory and motor nerve block appears to move in the direction of voluntary effort, signals of "motor command" or "effort" can influence perceived joint position. To determine whether this occurs when sensory signals are available, three studies assessed position sense when motor command and afferent signals were available, but joint movement was prevented. First, the hand was positioned to stop movement at the proximal joint of the middle finger, and movement at the distal joint was impossible because the muscles had been "disengaged". Voluntary efforts produced illusory position changes in the direction of the effort (12.6 +/- 2.0 degrees distal joint; 12.3 +/- 2.3 degrees proximal joint for efforts at 30% maximum; means +/- SD). Second, when subjects attempted to move the index finger under isometric conditions, the index finger appeared to move 7.4 +/- 1.2 degrees in the direction of efforts. These illusions graded with the level of effort (10 or 30% maximum) and far exceeded any real joint movement. Finally, because changes in muscle afferent feedback might have accompanied the voluntary efforts, all forearm and hand muscles were completely paralyzed by locally infused rocuronium. During paralysis, passive wrist position was signaled accurately, but, during attempted efforts (30% maximum), perceived wrist position changed by 9.7 +/- 4.9 degrees . Before paralysis, isometric efforts changed it by 6.7 +/- 3.6 degrees . Thus all studies concur: when joint movement is prevented, signals of motor command contribute to joint position sense.


Asunto(s)
Vías Aferentes/fisiología , Retroalimentación Fisiológica/fisiología , Cinestesia/fisiología , Neuronas Motoras/fisiología , Transducción de Señal/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Vías Aferentes/efectos de los fármacos , Androstanoles/farmacología , Retroalimentación Fisiológica/efectos de los fármacos , Femenino , Humanos , Contracción Isométrica/efectos de los fármacos , Contracción Isométrica/fisiología , Cinestesia/efectos de los fármacos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Neuronas Motoras/efectos de los fármacos , Contracción Muscular/efectos de los fármacos , Contracción Muscular/fisiología , Fármacos Neuromusculares no Despolarizantes/farmacología , Miembro Fantasma , Rocuronio , Transducción de Señal/efectos de los fármacos , Adulto Joven
3.
Exp Brain Res ; 195(1): 167-72, 2009 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19350228

RESUMEN

We report an aftereffect in perception of the extent (or degree or range) of joint movement, showing for the first time that a prolonged exposure to a passive back-and-forth movement of a certain extent results in a change in judgment of the extent of a subsequently presented movement. The adapting stimulus, movement about the wrist, had an extent of either 30 degrees or 75 degrees , while the test stimulus was a 50 degrees movement. Following a 4-min adaptation period, the estimated magnitudes of the test stimuli were 61 degrees and 36 degrees in the 30 degrees and 75 degrees condition, respectively (t test(6) = 9.6; p < 0.001). The observed effect is an instance of repulsion or contrast commonly described in perception literature, with perceived value of the test stimulus pushed away from the adapting stimulus.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Fisiológica/fisiología , Efecto Tardío Figurativo/fisiología , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Movimiento/fisiología , Fenómenos Biomecánicos , Electromiografía , Humanos , Juicio/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Estimulación Física , Propiocepción/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Psicofísica , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Muñeca/inervación
4.
Exp Brain Res ; 195(4): 603-10, 2009 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19452145

RESUMEN

Along with afferent information, centrally generated motor command signals may play a role in joint position sense. Isometric muscle contractions can produce a perception of joint displacement in the same direction as the joint would move if unrestrained. Contradictory findings of perceived joint displacement in the opposite direction have been reported. As this only occurs if muscle spindle discharge in the contracting muscle is initially low, it may reflect increased muscle spindle firing from fusimotor activation, rather than central motor command signals. Methodological differences including the muscle contraction task and use of muscle conditioning could underlie the opposing findings. Hence, we tested perceived joint position during two contraction tasks ('hold force' and 'hold position') at the same joint (wrist) and controlled muscle spindle discharge with thixotropic muscle conditioning. We expected that prior conditioning of the contracting muscle would eliminate any effect of increased fusimotor activation, but not of central motor commands. Muscle conditioning altered perceived wrist position as expected. Further, during muscle contractions, subjects reported wrist positions displaced ~12 degrees in the direction of contraction, despite no change in wrist position. This was similar for 'hold force' and 'hold position' tasks and occurred despite prior conditioning of the agonist muscle. However, conditioning of the antagonist muscle did reduce the effect of voluntary contraction on position sense. The errors in position sense cannot be explained by fusimotor activation. We propose that central signals combine with afferent signals to determine limb position and that multiple sources of information are weighted according to their reliability.


Asunto(s)
Extremidades/fisiología , Contracción Muscular/fisiología , Husos Musculares/fisiología , Músculo Esquelético/fisiología , Orientación/fisiología , Propiocepción/fisiología , Adulto , Fenómenos Biomecánicos , Extremidades/inervación , Retroalimentación/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Neuronas Motoras gamma/fisiología , Movimiento/fisiología , Husos Musculares/inervación , Músculo Esquelético/inervación , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Rango del Movimiento Articular/fisiología , Volición/fisiología , Articulación de la Muñeca/inervación , Articulación de la Muñeca/fisiología , Adulto Joven
5.
J Stud Alcohol Drugs ; 80(1): 96-101, 2019 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30807280

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: Training in an inhibitory control task has produced reductions in alcohol use among heavy drinkers. However, the longevity of effects remains unknown, and much research has used suboptimal control conditions. Here, we assess the effectiveness of "Beer-NoGo" inhibitory training to reduce consumption up to 4 weeks after training compared with a "Beer-Go" control task, an online version of the Brief Alcohol Intervention (BAI), and an Oddball control condition. METHOD: Eighty-one regular drinkers were randomized into one of four training conditions. In the Beer-NoGo condition, participants responded to a letter superimposed on water-related images and refrained from responding to another letter superimposed on beer-related images. The mapping was reversed for the Beer-Go condition, whereas the Oddball control condition was presented with letters only and inhibition was not required. The last condition was an online BAI. Alcohol use was assessed using a bogus taste test and weekly alcohol consumption. RESULTS: Taste-test consumption was greater in the Beer-Go condition than in the Beer-NoGo, which did not differ from the Oddball and BAI conditions. All groups reduced alcohol intake during the study; however, in the first week the Beer-Go group reduced their drinking while the Beer-NoGo group increased. No group differences were apparent at the fourth week. CONCLUSIONS: The Beer-NoGo task did not produce effects beyond simple assessment on reducing alcohol use among regular drinkers. Previously reported training effects may be artifacts of the Beer-Go task as a suboptimal control. More robust forms of inhibitory training are necessary if a useful clinical adjunct for managing alcohol abuse is to be developed.


Asunto(s)
Consumo de Bebidas Alcohólicas/prevención & control , Intoxicación Alcohólica/prevención & control , Alcoholismo/prevención & control , Inhibición Psicológica , Adulto , Cerveza , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
6.
J Appl Physiol (1985) ; 104(6): 1674-82, 2008 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18403446

RESUMEN

Exercise performance is impaired by increased respiratory work, yet the mechanism for this is unclear. This experiment assessed whether neural drive to an exercising muscle was affected by cortically driven increases in ventilation. On each of 5 days, eight subjects completed a 2-min maximal voluntary contraction (MVC) of the elbow flexor muscles, followed by 4 min of recovery, while transcranial magnetic stimulation tested for suboptimal neural drive to the muscle. On 1 day, subjects breathed without instructions under normocapnia. During the 2-min MVC, ventilation was approximately 3.5 times that at rest. On another day, subjects breathed without instruction under hypercapnia. During the 2-min MVC, ventilation was approximately 1.5 times that on the normocapnic day. On another 2 days under normocapnia, subjects voluntarily matched their breathing to the uninstructed breathing under normocapnia and hypercapnia using target feedback of the rate and inspiratory volume. On a fifth day under normocapnia, the volume feedback was set to each subject's vital capacity. On this day, ventilation during the 2-min MVC was approximately twice that on the uninstructed normocapnic day (or approximately 7 times rest). The experimental manipulations succeeded in producing voluntary and involuntary hyperpnea. However, maximal voluntary force, fatigue and voluntary activation of the elbow flexor muscles were unaffected by cortically or chemically driven increases in ventilation. Results suggest that any effects of increased respiratory work on limb exercise performance are not due to a failure to drive both muscle groups optimally.


Asunto(s)
Contracción Muscular , Músculo Esquelético/inervación , Ventilación Pulmonar , Mecánica Respiratoria , Músculos Respiratorios/inervación , Administración por Inhalación , Adulto , Dióxido de Carbono/administración & dosificación , Codo , Electromiografía , Potenciales Evocados Motores , Femenino , Humanos , Hipercapnia/fisiopatología , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Fatiga Muscular , Fuerza Muscular , Estimulación Magnética Transcraneal , Volición
7.
Clin Neurophysiol ; 119(3): 704-714, 2008 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18164657

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: The contribution of movement-related potentials (MRPs) to the Go/NoGo N2 and P3 'inhibitory' effects is controversial. This study examined these components in overt and covert response inhibition tasks. METHODS: Twenty adult participants counted or button-pressed in response to frequent (60%) and rare (20%) Go stimuli in a Go/NoGo task with equiprobable rare (20%) NoGo stimuli. RESULTS: The N2 NoGo effect did not differ between Count and Press responses, but the P3 NoGo effect was amplified during the Press task. Additionally, subtraction of the ERP waveform for Count NoGo from Press NoGo trials revealed a positivity between 200 and 400ms, occurring maximally over the central region, contralateral to the responding hand. This difference wave became significant at 210-260ms, close to the estimated time taken to stop an overt response. CONCLUSIONS: The N2 NoGo effect may reflect a non-motoric stage of inhibition, or recognition of the need for inhibition, while the NoGo P3 may overlap with a positive MRP occurring specifically on trials where overt motor responses must be inhibited. SIGNIFICANCE: The study confirms that the N2 and P3 NoGo effects are not solely due to movement-related potentials, and posits the NoGo P3 as a marker of motor inhibition.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Cognición/fisiología , Potenciales Relacionados con Evento P300/fisiología , Inhibición Psicológica , Movimiento/fisiología , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Estimulación Acústica/métodos , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Mapeo Encefálico , Variación Contingente Negativa/fisiología , Relación Dosis-Respuesta en la Radiación , Electroencefalografía/métodos , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología
8.
J Neurosci ; 26(18): 4796-802, 2006 May 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16672652

RESUMEN

The role of group III and IV muscle afferents in controlling the output from human muscles is poorly understood. We investigated the effects of these afferents from homonymous or antagonist muscles on motoneuron pools innervating extensor and flexor muscles of the elbow. In study 1, subjects (n = 8) performed brief maximal voluntary contractions (MVCs) of elbow extensors before and after a 2 min MVC of the extensors. During MVCs, electromyographic responses from triceps were evoked by stimulation of the corticospinal tracts [cervicomedullary motor evoked potentials (CMEPs)]. The same subjects repeated the protocol, but input from fatigue-sensitive afferents was prolonged after the fatiguing contraction by maintained muscle ischemia. In study 2, CMEPs were evoked in triceps during brief extensor MVCs before and after a 2 min sustained flexor MVC (n = 7) or in biceps during brief flexor MVCs before and after a sustained extensor MVC (n = 7). Again, ischemia was maintained after the sustained contractions. During sustained MVCs of the extensors, CMEPs in triceps decreased by approximately 35%. Without muscle ischemia, CMEPs recovered within 15 s, but with maintained ischemia, they remained depressed (by approximately 28%; p < 0.001). CMEPs in triceps were also depressed (by approximately 20%; p < 0.001) after fatiguing flexor contractions, whereas CMEPs in biceps were facilitated (by approximately 25%; p < 0.001) after fatiguing extensor contractions. During fatigue, inputs from group III and IV muscle afferents from homonymous or antagonist muscles depress extensor motoneurons but facilitate flexor motoneurons. The more pronounced inhibitory influence of these afferents on extensors suggests that these muscles may require greater cortical drive to generate force during fatigue.


Asunto(s)
Potenciales Evocados Motores/fisiología , Neuronas Motoras/fisiología , Contracción Muscular/fisiología , Fatiga Muscular/fisiología , Músculo Esquelético/citología , Músculo Esquelético/inervación , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Plexo Braquial/fisiología , Plexo Braquial/efectos de la radiación , Relación Dosis-Respuesta en la Radiación , Codo/inervación , Codo/fisiología , Estimulación Eléctrica/métodos , Electromiografía/métodos , Potenciales Evocados Motores/efectos de la radiación , Femenino , Humanos , Isquemia/fisiopatología , Masculino , Neuronas Motoras/efectos de la radiación , Fatiga Muscular/efectos de la radiación , Músculo Esquelético/fisiología , Inhibición Neural/fisiología , Inhibición Neural/efectos de la radiación , Tractos Piramidales/fisiología , Tractos Piramidales/efectos de la radiación , Factores de Tiempo
9.
J Appl Physiol (1985) ; 103(2): 560-8, 2007 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17463302

RESUMEN

During sustained maximal voluntary contractions (MVCs), most fatigue occurs within the muscle, but some occurs because voluntary activation of the muscle declines (central fatigue), and some of this reflects suboptimal output from the motor cortex (supraspinal fatigue). This study examines whether supraspinal fatigue occurs during a sustained submaximal contraction of 5% MVC. Eight subjects sustained an isometric elbow flexion of 5% MVC for 70 min. Brief MVCs were performed every 3 min, with stimulation of the motor point, motor cortex, and brachial plexus. Perceived effort and pain, elbow flexion torque, and surface EMGs from biceps and brachioradialis were recorded. During the sustained 5% contraction, perceived effort increased from 0.5 to 3.9 (out of 10), and elbow flexor EMG increased steadily by approximately 60-80%. Torque during brief MVCs fell to 72% of control values, while both the resting twitch and EMG declined progressively. Thus the sustained weak contraction caused fatigue, some of which was due to peripheral mechanisms. Voluntary activation measured by motor point and motor cortex stimulation methods fell to 90% and 80%, respectively. Thus some of the fatigue was central. Calculations based on the fall in voluntary activation measured with cortical stimulation indicate that about two-thirds of the fatigue was due to supraspinal mechanisms. Therefore, sustained performance of a very low-force contraction produces a progressive inability to drive the motor cortex optimally during brief MVCs. The effect of central fatigue on performance of the weak contraction is less clear, but it may contribute to the increase in perceived effort.


Asunto(s)
Corteza Motora/fisiología , Contracción Muscular/fisiología , Fatiga Muscular/fisiología , Músculo Esquelético/fisiología , Adulto , Codo , Estimulación Eléctrica , Electromiografía , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Columna Vertebral
10.
Clin Neurophysiol ; 118(2): 343-55, 2007 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17140848

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVES: In the Go/NoGo task, the N2 and P3 components are often thought to index response inhibition, or conflict between competing responses. If so, they should be affected by response preparation when the prediction of an informative cue is incorrect. METHODS: Twenty-six adult participants completed a cued-Go/NoGo task. Targets required a left or right button press, or no response, while cues predicted the probable identity of the target. Analyses examined (a) effects of cues on response preparation, and "inhibitory" components to NoGo targets, (b) typical Go/NoGo differences, and (c) the impact of cue (in)validity. RESULTS: A reaction time benefit was associated with valid cueing, and a cost with invalid cueing. Late CNV results indicated that participants used cue information to prepare responses, and the P3, but not the N2, showed an increase with prior preparation. Typical frontal N2 and P3 NoGo>Go effects were observed, and the P3 but not the N2 showed an Invalid>Valid effect. CONCLUSIONS: The P3, rather than the N2, reflects the inhibition of a planned response and/or the conflict between competing responses. SIGNIFICANCE: The findings suggest the need for a major review of current interpretations of the N2 and P3 in inhibitory tasks.


Asunto(s)
Corteza Cerebral/fisiología , Señales (Psicología) , Toma de Decisiones/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Inhibición Neural/fisiología , Pensamiento/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Potenciales Relacionados con Evento P300/fisiología , Femenino , Lóbulo Frontal/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Valores de Referencia
11.
Clin Neurophysiol ; 118(10): 2234-47, 2007 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17709297

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: We aimed to examine relationships between the phase of narrow-band electroencephalographic (EEG) activity at stimulus onset and the resultant event-related potentials (ERPs) in active vs. passive auditory oddball tasks, using a novel conceptualisation of orthogonal phase effects. METHODS: This study focused on the operation of three recently-reported phase-influenced mechanisms, and ERP responses to the standard stimuli were analysed. Prestimulus narrow-band EEG activity (in 1 Hz bands from 1 to 13 Hz) at Cz was assessed for each trial using digital filtering. For each frequency, the cycle at stimulus onset was used to sort trials into four phases, for which ERPs were derived from both the filtered and unfiltered EEG activity at Fz, Cz and Pz. RESULTS: Preferred brain states at various frequencies were indicated by approximately 20% differential occurrence within the orthogonal phase dimensions explored. CONCLUSIONS: The preferred states were associated with more efficient processing of the stimulus, as reflected in differences in latency and/or amplitude of various ERP components, and provided evidence for the operation of the three separate phase-influenced mechanisms. SIGNIFICANCE: Both the occurrence of preferred brain states, and the mechanisms linking them to ERP outcomes focused on here, appeared relatively invariant across tasks, suggesting that they largely reflect reflexive brain processes.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Electroencefalografía/estadística & datos numéricos , Potenciales Evocados Auditivos/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica , Adolescente , Adulto , Variación Contingente Negativa , Interpretación Estadística de Datos , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
12.
Int J Psychophysiol ; 63(1): 25-38, 2007 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16919346

RESUMEN

The present study examined the development of response inhibition during the Stop-signal and Go/Nogo tasks in children using performance and ERP measures. Twenty-four children aged 7 to 12 years completed both tasks, each with an auditory Nogo/Stop-signal presented on 30% of trials. On average, response inhibition was more difficult in the Stop-signal than Go/Nogo task. Response inhibition performance did not develop significantly across the age range, while response execution varied significantly in a task dependent manner (Go/Nogo: increasing accuracy and reducing response variability with age; Stop-signal: reducing Go mean reaction time and response variability with age). The N1, P2, N2 and P3 components showed different scalp distributions, with N1 and P2 peaking earlier, and P3 later, in Nogo compared to Stop stimuli. N2 and P3 amplitude were positively correlated with successful inhibition probability in the Go/Nogo task only. N2 amplitude and latency to both Nogo and successful Stop stimuli decreased linearly with age, but not in the frontal regions. N1 and P3 amplitude in the parietal region increased with age for Stop-signals. An age-related reduction in P3 latency to Nogo stimuli correlated significantly with reduced RT and variability in Go responding, indicating a relationship between efficient Nogo and Go processing. Together the behavioural and ERP results suggest little development of the response inhibition process as measured via the Stop-signal and Go/Nogo tasks across the 7 to 12 year age range, while response execution processes develop substantially.


Asunto(s)
Desarrollo Infantil/fisiología , Conducta de Elección/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados Auditivos/fisiología , Inhibición Psicológica , Estimulación Acústica/métodos , Factores de Edad , Análisis de Varianza , Mapeo Encefálico , Niño , Electroencefalografía/métodos , Femenino , Humanos , Modelos Lineales , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología
13.
Drug Alcohol Depend ; 171: 20-30, 2017 Feb 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28012428

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Increases in inhibitory errors in heavy drinkers are established; less well-studied is whether heavy drinkers are aware of these errors in performance. Reductions in error detection and awareness limit the possibility for remedial action to be taken, and have implications for substance abusers seeking to control use: failure to monitor and/or adjust ongoing behaviour may be linked to using more or more often than intended, and failing to adjust behaviour after a slip. Here we report the first study of both inhibitory control and error awareness in young heavy drinkers, using behavioural and psychophysiological measures. METHODS: Heavy drinkers (n=25) and light- or non-drinking controls (n=35) completed a difficult inhibitory task which required signaling the awareness of inhibitory errors on the subsequent trial, while brain electrical activity was recorded. RESULTS: Heavy drinkers made more inhibitory errors than controls, but we observed no difference in error awareness, both via overt signaling and with equivalent amplitude of the error positivity (Pe), indexing conscious error detection. Similarly, controls and heavy drinkers showed no difference in amplitude or latency of the error-related negativity (ERN), indexing early pre-conscious error detection. CONCLUSIONS: This research suggests no significant difference in detection of errors in heavy drinkers, even as they are more prone to make these errors, a result seen in dependent drinkers reported elsewhere. Future research with larger sample sizes, and a more difficult task producing sufficient errors, should determine whether heavy drinkers employ sufficient post-error remedial action.


Asunto(s)
Consumo de Bebidas Alcohólicas/psicología , Intoxicación Alcohólica/psicología , Concienciación/fisiología , Inhibición Psicológica , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Adulto , Consumo de Bebidas Alcohólicas/epidemiología , Intoxicación Alcohólica/diagnóstico , Intoxicación Alcohólica/epidemiología , Electroencefalografía/métodos , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Monitoreo Fisiológico/métodos , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Adulto Joven
14.
Drug Alcohol Depend ; 173: 47-58, 2017 Apr 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28196787

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Disinhibition is apparent in users of many substances, including heavy drinkers. Previous research has shown that brief training to improve inhibitory control is associated with reduced alcohol consumption. We investigated whether a new form of inhibitory training would produce greater reductions, relative to a carefully designed control condition and a proven method of reducing consumption, the Brief Alcohol Intervention (BAI). METHODS: One hundred and fourteen regular drinkers were assigned randomly to one of five training conditions: Control (no inhibitory training); Beer-NoGo (inhibit responses linked to task-irrelevant pictures of beer); Restrained-Stop (requiring more urgent inhibition but without pictures of beer); Combined (a previously untested form of training requiring urgent inhibition to pictures of beer); or BAI. The outcome measures were alcohol consumption in the week before and after training, and in a bogus taste test administered immediately post-training. RESULTS: Participation in the study, regardless of condition, was associated with reductions in weekly consumption. However, only the BAI produced a greater reduction relative to the Control condition. The training tasks were not associated with reductions in taste test consumption. CONCLUSIONS: Although concerns about low power limit confidence, the current study suggests that three forms of inhibitory training do not have a substantial effect on drinking beyond the effect of simple assessment, in comparison to a control task which does not promote impulsive responding. Future research needs to establish a training protocol that produces greater reductions in consumption not only relative to the effect of assessment but also relative to a BAI.


Asunto(s)
Consumo de Bebidas Alcohólicas/prevención & control , Alcoholismo/terapia , Terapia Conductista/métodos , Inhibición Psicológica , Consumo de Bebidas Alcohólicas/psicología , Alcoholismo/psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Resultado del Tratamiento , Adulto Joven
15.
Front Psychol ; 8: 2129, 2017.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29276495

RESUMEN

Aims: Long-term heavy use of cannabis and alcohol are known to be associated with memory impairments. In this study, we used event-related potentials to examine verbal learning and memory processing in a commonly used behavioral task. Method: We conducted two studies: first, a small pilot study of adolescent males, comprising 13 Drug-Naive Controls (DNC), 12 heavy drinkers (HD) and 8 cannabis users (CU). Second, a larger study of young adults, comprising 45 DNC (20 female), 39 HD (16 female), and 20 CU (9 female). In both studies, participants completed a modified verbal learning task (the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, RAVLT) while brain electrical activity was recorded. ERPs were calculated for words which were subsequently remembered vs. those which were not remembered, and for presentations of learnt words, previously seen words, and new words in a subsequent recognition test. Pre-planned principal components analyses (PCA) were used to quantify the ERP components in these recall and recognition phases separately for each study. Results: Memory performance overall was slightly lower than published norms using the standardized RAVLT delivery, but was generally similar and showed the expected changes over trials. Few differences in performance were observed between groups; a notable exception was markedly poorer delayed recall in HD relative to DNC (Study 2). PCA identified components expected from prior research using other memory tasks. At encoding, there were no between-group differences in the usual P2 recall effect (larger for recalled than not-recalled words). However, alcohol-related differences were observed in a larger P540 (indexing recollection) in HD than DNC, and cannabis-related differences were observed in a smaller N340 (indexing familiarity) and a lack of previously seen > new words effect for P540 in Study 2. Conclusions: This study is the first examination of ERPs in the RAVLT in healthy control participants, as well as substance-using individuals, and represents an important advance in methodology. The results indicate alterations in recognition memory processing, which even if not manifesting in overt behavioral impairment, underline the potential for brain dysfunction with early exposure to alcohol and cannabis.

16.
Int J Psychophysiol ; 61(2): 121-33, 2006 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16214250

RESUMEN

The cued Go/NoGo task elicits response preparation during the foreperiod, and, depending on the S2 signal, either response execution or inhibition. This study aimed to determine how processes in the foreperiod might affect or predict post-S2 processing. Thirty-two adults participated in a cued Go/NoGo task (50% Go), with a median split of mean RT producing "Fast" and "Slow" groups. ERP measures were subjected to both ANOVA and regression techniques. There were no differences in the NoGo N2 effect related to response speed, nor was the effect related to pre-S2 processes. The anterior shift of the NoGo P3 was larger in the Fast group, and while the late CNV was associated with the absolute amplitude of both Go and NoGo P3, it was not related to the anterior-posterior Go/NoGo differences. Together, these data suggest that the inhibitory process may be reflected in the NoGo P3 effect, rather than the NoGo N2 effect.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Corteza Cerebral/fisiología , Variación Contingente Negativa/fisiología , Señales (Psicología) , Inhibición Psicológica , Percepción de la Altura Tonal/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Nivel de Alerta/fisiología , Mapeo Encefálico , Dominancia Cerebral/fisiología , Electroencefalografía , Potenciales Evocados Auditivos/fisiología , Femenino , Lóbulo Frontal/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Lóbulo Parietal/fisiología , Psicoacústica , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Estadística como Asunto
17.
Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging ; 254: 103-11, 2016 Aug 30.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27399307

RESUMEN

Previous research has reported mixed evidence of sex differences in the relationship between heavy alcohol use and deficits in behavioural control. Here, we examine sex differences in behavioural and event-related potential (ERP) markers of deficient inhibition. Participants were 71 young adults aged 18-21, who either drank heavily regularly (i.e., four standard drinks on one occasion, at least once a month, n=33, 20 male) or drank heavily less often than this (including never, n=38, 21 male). They completed a stop-signal task while ERPs were recorded. Increases in stop-signal reaction time, the time required to stop a response, were related to heavy drinking only in female participants. P3 amplitude, ERN amplitude and ERN latency did not display a significant interaction between group and sex. Heavy drinkers, regardless of sex, displayed a marginally larger successful>failed effect for P3 amplitude, and a marginally smaller error-related negativity. An apparent disconnect exists in behavioural and psychophysiological measures of sex differences in the relationship between heavy alcohol consumption and inhibitory processing; male heavy drinkers display only psychophysiological but not behavioural deficits, while female heavy drinkers display both. Future research may determine whether sex differences are apparent for other substances besides alcohol.


Asunto(s)
Consumo de Bebidas Alcohólicas/fisiopatología , Trastornos Relacionados con Alcohol/fisiopatología , Encéfalo/fisiopatología , Potenciales Relacionados con Evento P300/fisiología , Inhibición Psicológica , Inhibición Neural/fisiología , Adolescente , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Electroencefalografía , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Factores Sexuales , Adulto Joven
18.
Physiol Rep ; 4(15)2016 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27482074

RESUMEN

The onset of voluntary muscle contractions causes rapid increases in ventilation and is accompanied by a sensation of effort. Both the ventilatory response and perception of effort are proportional to contraction intensity, but these behaviors have been generalized from contractions of a single muscle group. Our aim was to determine how these relationships are affected by simultaneous contractions of multiple muscle groups. We examined the ventilatory response and perceived effort of contraction during separate and simultaneous isometric contractions of the contralateral elbow flexors and of an ipsilateral elbow flexor and knee extensor. Subjects made 10-sec contractions at 25, 50, and 100% of maximum during normocapnia and hypercapnia. For simultaneous contractions, both muscle groups were activated at the same intensities. Ventilation was measured continuously and subjects rated the effort required to produce each contraction. As expected, ventilation and perceived effort increased proportionally with contraction intensity during individual contractions. However, during simultaneous contractions, neither ventilation nor effort reflected the combined muscle output. Rather, the ventilatory response was similar to when contractions were performed separately, and effort ratings showed a small but significant increase for simultaneous contractions. Hypercapnia at rest doubled baseline ventilation, but did not affect the difference in perceived effort between separate and simultaneous contractions. The ventilatory response and the sense of effort at the onset of muscle activity are not related to the total output of the motor pathways, or the working muscles, but arise from cortical regions upstream from the motor cortex.


Asunto(s)
Contracción Isométrica , Percepción , Esfuerzo Físico , Ventilación Pulmonar , Adulto , Brazo/fisiología , Electromiografía , Femenino , Humanos , Hipercapnia/fisiopatología , Hipercapnia/psicología , Pierna/fisiología , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Músculo Esquelético/fisiología , Adulto Joven
19.
Psychiatry Res ; 233(3): 424-35, 2015 Sep 30.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26208747

RESUMEN

Difficulties in monitoring ongoing behaviour may be linked to real-life problematic drinking behaviours. Prior research suggests female heavy drinkers in particular display greater cognitive control deficits. Here, we examine trial-to-trial behavioural adaptations in a conflict monitoring task, relative to drinking behaviour and sex. Heavy drinkers (n=31, 16 male) and controls (n=35, 18 male) completed an Eriksen flanker task while brain electrical activity was recorded. For reaction time, error rates, and N2 and P3 amplitude of the event-related potential, trial-to-trial conflict adaptation was evidenced by a differential response to the current (congruent vs. incongruent) trials dependent on the identity of the previous trial. For the proportion of errors, heavy drinkers showed increased conflict adaptation compared to controls. Conflict adaptation for N2 (indexing monitoring) was larger for female heavy drinkers than controls, and the opposite was observed for males. There were no interactions involving group or sex for the P3 (indexing inhibition). The results suggest a compensatory response, such that heavy drinkers are required to increase performance monitoring in order to achieve the same behavioural outcome as controls. We also confirm the importance of sex as a factor in the relationship between behavioural control and heavy alcohol use.


Asunto(s)
Consumo de Bebidas Alcohólicas/psicología , Conflicto Psicológico , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Caracteres Sexuales , Adaptación Psicológica/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Consumo de Bebidas Alcohólicas/efectos adversos , Electroencefalografía/métodos , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Inhibición Psicológica , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Adulto Joven
20.
Clin Neurophysiol ; 115(6): 1320-31, 2004 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15134699

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: Previous event-related potential (ERP) research on inhibitory functioning in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (AD/HD) has often failed to use tasks which optimally assess inhibition. We report on an improved measure of inhibitory function, involving inhibition of a prepotent response, in children with AD/HD. METHODS: Twelve males with AD/HD and 12 control males, aged 7-12 years, completed a cued Go/NoGo task where Go stimuli were presented on 70% of trials. ERP and behavioural measures were collected, together with reading, spelling and full-scale IQ scores. RESULTS: The behavioural performance of children with AD/HD was not significantly different from normal controls, although children with AD/HD made faster responses and more errors. Group differences were apparent in the early processing components (P1, N1, P2) of responses to Warning, Go and NoGo stimuli. For the frontally maximal N2, a NoGo>Go effect was found, consistent with previous work linking this component with inhibitory processing. In control children this effect was particularly strong in the right frontal region, while children with AD/HD showed a much larger NoGo>Go effect, and an earlier N2 peak, than controls, with a focal shift to the left frontal region. CONCLUSIONS: Compared with normal controls, children with AD/HD demonstrate early stimulus processing atypicalities, suggesting problems with sensory registration and identification of stimuli. Further, N2 results suggest that children with AD/HD must trigger the inhibition process earlier and more strongly than controls to perform at a comparable behavioural level. SIGNIFICANCE: The results support the theory that behavioural inhibition is deficient in AD/HD, as children with AD/HD show abnormalities in inhibitory ERP components relating to the effort involved in inhibiting a prepotent response.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/fisiopatología , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Inhibición Neural/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Niño , Electroencefalografía , Humanos , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA