Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 153
Filtrar
Más filtros

Banco de datos
País/Región como asunto
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Toxicol Appl Pharmacol ; 491: 117079, 2024 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39218163

RESUMEN

Fatty acid-binding proteins (FABPs) are small intracellular proteins that regulate fatty acid metabolism, transport, and signalling. There are ten known human isoforms, many of which are upregulated and involved in clinical pathologies. As such, FABP inhibition may be beneficial in disease states such as cancer, and those involving the cardiovascular system, metabolism, immunity, and cognition. Recently, a potent, selective FABP5 inhibitor (ART26.12), with 90-fold selectivity to FABP3 and 20-fold selectivity to FABP7, was found to be remarkably benign, with a no-observed-adverse-effect level of 1000 mg/kg in rats and dogs, showing no genotoxicity, cardiovascular, central, or respiratory toxicity. To understand the potential implication of FABP inhibition more fully, this review systematically assessed literature investigating genetic knockout, knockdown, and pharmacological inhibition of FABP3, FABP4, FABP5, or FABP7. Analysis of the literature revealed that animals bred not to express FABPs showed the most biological effects, suggesting key roles of these proteins during development. FABP ablation sometimes exacerbated symptoms of disease models, particularly those linked to metabolism, inflammatory and immune responses, cardiac contractility, neurogenesis, and cognition. However, FABP inhibition (genetic silencing or pharmacological) had a positive effect in many more disease conditions. Several polymorphisms of each FABP gene have also been linked to pathological conditions, but it was unclear how several polymorphisms affected protein function. Overall, analysis of the literature to date suggests that pharmacological inhibition of FABPs in adults is of low risk.


Asunto(s)
Proteínas de Unión a Ácidos Grasos , Proteínas de Unión a Ácidos Grasos/genética , Proteínas de Unión a Ácidos Grasos/metabolismo , Animales , Humanos , Perros
2.
PLoS Comput Biol ; 18(6): e1010210, 2022 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35679329

RESUMEN

When two streams of pedestrians cross at an angle, striped patterns spontaneously emerge as a result of local pedestrian interactions. This clear case of self-organized pattern formation remains to be elucidated. In counterflows, with a crossing angle of 180°, alternating lanes of traffic are commonly observed moving in opposite directions, whereas in crossing flows at an angle of 90°, diagonal stripes have been reported. Naka (1977) hypothesized that stripe orientation is perpendicular to the bisector of the crossing angle. However, studies of crossing flows at acute and obtuse angles remain underdeveloped. We tested the bisector hypothesis in experiments on small groups (18-19 participants each) crossing at seven angles (30° intervals), and analyzed the geometric properties of stripes. We present two novel computational methods for analyzing striped patterns in pedestrian data: (i) an edge-cutting algorithm, which detects the dynamic formation of stripes and allows us to measure local properties of individual stripes; and (ii) a pattern-matching technique, based on the Gabor function, which allows us to estimate global properties (orientation and wavelength) of the striped pattern at a time T. We find an invariant property: stripes in the two groups are parallel and perpendicular to the bisector at all crossing angles. In contrast, other properties depend on the crossing angle: stripe spacing (wavelength), stripe size (number of pedestrians per stripe), and crossing time all decrease as the crossing angle increases from 30° to 180°, whereas the number of stripes increases with crossing angle. We also observe that the width of individual stripes is dynamically squeezed as the two groups cross each other. The findings thus support the bisector hypothesis at a wide range of crossing angles, although the theoretical reasons for this invariant remain unclear. The present results provide empirical constraints on theoretical studies and computational models of crossing flows.


Asunto(s)
Peatones , Algoritmos , Humanos , Modelos Teóricos
3.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 117(49): 31494-31499, 2020 12 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33229535

RESUMEN

Animals that move through complex habitats must frequently contend with obstacles in their path. Humans and other highly cognitive vertebrates avoid collisions by perceiving the relationship between the layout of their surroundings and the properties of their own body profile and action capacity. It is unknown whether insects, which have much smaller brains, possess such abilities. We used bumblebees, which vary widely in body size and regularly forage in dense vegetation, to investigate whether flying insects consider their own size when interacting with their surroundings. Bumblebees trained to fly in a tunnel were sporadically presented with an obstructing wall containing a gap that varied in width. Bees successfully flew through narrow gaps, even those that were much smaller than their wingspans, by first performing lateral scanning (side-to-side flights) to visually assess the aperture. Bees then reoriented their in-flight posture (i.e., yaw or heading angle) while passing through, minimizing their projected frontal width and mitigating collisions; in extreme cases, bees flew entirely sideways through the gap. Both the time that bees spent scanning during their approach and the extent to which they reoriented themselves to pass through the gap were determined not by the absolute size of the gap, but by the size of the gap relative to each bee's own wingspan. Our findings suggest that, similar to humans and other vertebrates, flying bumblebees perceive the affordance of their surroundings relative their body size and form to navigate safely through complex environments.


Asunto(s)
Abejas/anatomía & histología , Abejas/fisiología , Tamaño Corporal , Vuelo Animal/fisiología , Animales , Factores de Tiempo , Grabación en Video , Alas de Animales/anatomía & histología , Alas de Animales/fisiología
4.
J Vis ; 23(10): 3, 2023 09 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37676673

RESUMEN

Patterns of crowd behavior are believed to result from local interactions between pedestrians. Many studies have investigated the local rules of interaction, such as steering, avoiding, and alignment, but how pedestrians control their walking speed when following another remains unsettled. Most pedestrian models assume the physical speed and distance of others as input. The present study compares such "omniscient" models with "visual" models based on optical variables. We experimentally tested eight speed control models from the pedestrian- and car-following literature. Walking participants were asked to follow a leader (a moving pole) in a virtual environment, while the leader's speed was perturbed during the trial. In Experiment 1, the leader's initial distance was varied. Each model was fit to the data and compared. The results showed that visual models based on optical expansion (\(\dot{\theta }\)) had the smallest root mean square error in speed across conditions, whereas other models exhibited increased error at longer distances. In Experiment 2, the leader's size (pole diameter) was varied. A model based on the relative rate of expansion (\(\dot{\theta }/\theta \)) performed better than the expansion rate model (\(\dot{\theta }\)), because it is less sensitive to leader size. Together, the results imply that pedestrians directly control their walking speed in one-dimensional following using relative rate of expansion, rather than the distal speed and distance of the leader.


Asunto(s)
Peatones , Humanos , Caminata
5.
Eur J Neurosci ; 55(4): 952-970, 2022 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33759226

RESUMEN

Anxiety and trauma-related disorders, such as post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), are debilitating mental illnesses with great personal and socioeconomic costs. Examining memory formation and relevant behavioural responding associated with aversive stimuli may improve our understanding of the neurobiology underlying fear memory processing and PTSD treatment. The neurocircuitry underpinning learned fear and its inhibition through extinction is complex, involving synergistic interactions between different neurotransmitter systems in inter-connected brain areas. Endocannabinoid and noradrenergic transmission have both been implicated separately in fear memory processing and PTSD, but potential interactions between these systems in relation to fear extinction have received little attention to date. Their receptors are expressed together in brain areas crucial for fear extinction, which is enhanced by both cannabinoid and noradrenergic receptor activation in these areas. Moreover, cannabinoid signalling modulates the activity of locus coeruleus noradrenaline (NA) neurons and the release of NA in the medial prefrontal cortex, a brain area that is crucial for fear extinction. Interestingly, endocannabinoid-noradrenergic system interactions have been shown to regulate the encoding and retrieval of fear memory. Thus, noradrenergic regulation of fear extinction may also be driven indirectly in part via cannabinoid receptor signalling. In this perspective paper, we collate the available relevant literature and propose a synergistic role for the endocannabinoid and noradrenergic systems in regulating fear extinction, the study of which may further our understanding of the neurobiological substrates of PTSD and its treatment.


Asunto(s)
Cannabinoides , Trastornos por Estrés Postraumático , Endocannabinoides , Extinción Psicológica , Miedo/fisiología , Humanos , Norepinefrina
6.
Proc Biol Sci ; 289(1970): 20212089, 2022 03 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35232235

RESUMEN

Patterns of collective motion in bird flocks, fish schools and human crowds are believed to emerge from local interactions between individuals. Most 'flocking' models attribute these local interactions to hypothetical rules or metaphorical forces and assume an omniscient third-person view of the positions and velocities of all individuals in space. We develop a visual model of collective motion in human crowds based on the visual coupling that governs pedestrian interactions from a first-person embedded viewpoint. Specifically, humans control their walking speed and direction by cancelling the average angular velocity and optical expansion/contraction of their neighbours, weighted by visibility (1 - occlusion). We test the model by simulating data from experiments with virtual crowds and real human 'swarms'. The visual model outperforms our previous omniscient model and explains basic properties of interaction: 'repulsion' forces reduce to cancelling optical expansion, 'attraction' forces to cancelling optical contraction and 'alignment' to cancelling the combination of expansion/contraction and angular velocity. Moreover, the neighbourhood of interaction follows from Euclid's Law of perspective and the geometry of occlusion. We conclude that the local interactions underlying human flocking are a natural consequence of the laws of optics. Similar perceptual principles may apply to collective motion in other species.


Asunto(s)
Aves , Aglomeración , Animales , Humanos , Movimiento (Física)
7.
J Exp Biol ; 225(4)2022 02 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35067721

RESUMEN

Insects are remarkable flyers and capable of navigating through highly cluttered environments. We tracked the head and thorax of bumblebees freely flying in a tunnel containing vertically oriented obstacles to uncover the sensorimotor strategies used for obstacle detection and collision avoidance. Bumblebees presented all the characteristics of active vision during flight by stabilizing their head relative to the external environment and maintained close alignment between their gaze and flightpath. Head stabilization increased motion contrast of nearby features against the background to enable obstacle detection. As bees approached obstacles, they appeared to modulate avoidance responses based on the relative retinal expansion velocity (RREV) of obstacles and their maximum evasion acceleration was linearly related to RREVmax. Finally, bees prevented collisions through rapid roll manoeuvres implemented by their thorax. Overall, the combination of visuo-motor strategies of bumblebees highlights elegant solutions developed by insects for visually guided flight through cluttered environments.


Asunto(s)
Vuelo Animal , Visión Ocular , Aceleración , Animales , Abejas , Vuelo Animal/fisiología , Insectos , Movimiento (Física)
8.
Int J Mol Sci ; 23(16)2022 Aug 19.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36012600

RESUMEN

Cannabidiol, the main non-psychotropic constituent of cannabis, has potential as a treatment for anxiety-related disorders since it reduces learned fear expression and enhances fear extinction. The return of fear over time after successful extinction and stress-induced extinction resistance are potential barriers to the treatment of these disorders with extinction-based psychological therapy. In two experiments using rats subjected to auditory fear conditioning, we determined the effects of systemic cannabidiol treatment on (1) delayed extinction and later spontaneous fear recovery, and (2) extinction resistance caused by immediate extinction (the immediate extinction deficit (IED)). In Experiment 1, cannabidiol was given before delayed extinction occurring 24 h after conditioning, with extinction recall and spontaneous fear recovery tested drug-free 1 and 21 days after extinction, respectively. We found that cannabidiol had no effect on extinction recall but it prevented spontaneous fear recovery. In Experiment 2, the IED procedure was first validated, with immediate extinction occurring 30 min after conditioning. We confirmed that immediate extinction impaired extinction recall, compared to delayed extinction. Next, cannabidiol was given before immediate or no extinction, with extinction recall tested drug-free the next day. We found that cannabidiol rescued the IED, which did not involve effects on fear memory consolidation. In summary, cannabidiol prevented spontaneous fear recovery after delayed extinction and ameliorated extinction resistance caused by immediate extinction. Although the pharmacological mechanisms underlying these effects remain to be determined, our results add to evidence indicating that cannabidiol might prove useful as an adjunct for potentiating the psychological treatment of anxiety-related disorders.


Asunto(s)
Cannabidiol , Miedo , Animales , Cannabidiol/farmacología , Condicionamiento Clásico , Condicionamiento Psicológico , Extinción Psicológica , Ratas
9.
J Vis ; 21(12): 13, 2021 11 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34812836

RESUMEN

It takes less effort to walk from here to the Tiki Hut on the brick walkway than on the sandy beach. Does that influence how far away the Tiki Hut looks? The energetic cost of walking on dry sand is twice that of walking on firm ground (Lejeune et al., 1998). If perceived distance depends on the energetic cost or anticipated effort of walking (Proffitt, 2006), then the distance of a target viewed over sand should appear much greater than one viewed over brick. If perceived distance is specified by optical information (e.g., declination angle from the horizon; Ooi et al., 2001), then the distances should appear similar. Participants (N = 13) viewed a target at a distance of 5, 7, 9, or 11 m over sand or brick and then blind-walked an equivalent distance on the same or different terrain. First, we observed no main effect of walked terrain; walked distances on sand and brick were the same (p = 0.46), indicating that locomotion was calibrated to each substrate. Second, responses were actually greater after viewing over brick than over sand (p < 0.001), opposite to the prediction of the energetic hypothesis. This unexpected overshooting can be explained by the slight incline of the brick walkway, which partially raises the visually perceived eye level (VPEL) and increases the target distance specified by the declination angle. The result is thus consistent with the information hypothesis. We conclude that visually perceived egocentric distance depends on optical information and not on the anticipated energetic cost of walking.


Asunto(s)
Locomoción , Caminata , Percepción de Distancia , Humanos
10.
Physica A ; 5692021 May 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34334928

RESUMEN

It is unclear how building occupants take information from the social and built environment into account when choosing an egress route during emergency evacuation. Conflicting tendencies have been previously reported: to follow the crowd, to avoid congestion, and to avoid unknown egress routes alone. We hypothesize that these tendencies depend on an interaction between social influence and the affordances (opportunities for egress) of the built environment. In three virtual reality (VR) experiments (each N = 15), we investigated how social influence interacts with the affordances of available exits to determine exit choice. Participants were immersed in a crowd of virtual humans walking to the left or right exit, and were asked to walk to one of the exits. Experiment 1 tested the role of social influence by manipulating both the proportion of the crowd walking toward one exit (Crowd Proportion of 0 to 100%, in 10% increments) and the absolute number of virtual humans going to the exit (Crowd Size of 10 or 20). Experiment 2 tested the role of affordances by introducing two visible exit doors (1m width) in a closed room, and following the same protocol. Experiment 3 tested larger exit doors (3m width) that afford rapid egress for more people. In the small crowd, participants were increasingly likely to follow the majority as its proportion increased. In the large crowd, however, participants tended to avoid the more crowded exit if the doors were narrow (Experiment 2), but not if the doors were wide (Experiment 3). Participants tended to follow a 100% majority in all experiments, thereby avoiding going to an exit alone. We propose that the dynamics of exit choice can be understood in terms of competition between alternative egress routes: the attraction of an exit increases with the proportion of the crowd moving toward it, becoming dominant at 100%, but decreases with the absolute number in the crowd moving toward it, relative to the exit's affordance for egress.

11.
Am J Physiol Renal Physiol ; 319(4): F697-F711, 2020 10 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32865013

RESUMEN

Praliciguat, a clinical-stage soluble guanylate cyclase (sGC) stimulator, increases cGMP via the nitric oxide-sGC pathway. Praliciguat has been shown to be renoprotective in rodent models of hypertensive nephropathy and renal fibrosis. In the present study, praliciguat alone and in combination with enalapril attenuated proteinuria in the obese ZSF1 rat model of diabetic nephropathy. Praliciguat monotherapy did not affect hemodynamics. In contrast, enalapril monotherapy lowered blood pressure but did not attenuate proteinuria. Renal expression of genes in pathways involved in inflammation, fibrosis, oxidative stress, and kidney injury was lower in praliciguat-treated obese ZSF1 rats than in obese control rats; fasting glucose and cholesterol were also lower with praliciguat treatment. To gain insight into how tubular mechanisms might contribute to its pharmacological effects on the kidneys, we studied the effects of praliciguat on pathological processes and signaling pathways in cultured human primary renal proximal tubular epithelial cells (RPTCs). Praliciguat inhibited the expression of proinflammatory cytokines and secretion of monocyte chemoattractant protein-1 in tumor necrosis factor-α-challenged RPTCs. Praliciguat treatment also attenuated transforming growth factor-ß-mediated apoptosis, changes to a mesenchyme-like cellular phenotype, and phosphorylation of SMAD3 in RPTCs. In conclusion, praliciguat improved proteinuria in the ZSF1 rat model of diabetic nephropathy, and its actions in human RPTCs suggest that tubular effects may contribute to its renal benefits, building upon strong evidence for the role of cGMP signaling in renal health.


Asunto(s)
Apoptosis/efectos de los fármacos , Nefropatías Diabéticas/tratamiento farmacológico , Agonistas de la Guanilato Ciclasa C/farmacología , Túbulos Renales Proximales/efectos de los fármacos , Nefritis/tratamiento farmacológico , Pirazoles/farmacología , Pirimidinas/farmacología , Inhibidores de la Enzima Convertidora de Angiotensina/farmacología , Animales , Línea Celular , Citocinas/metabolismo , Nefropatías Diabéticas/metabolismo , Nefropatías Diabéticas/patología , Modelos Animales de Enfermedad , Progresión de la Enfermedad , Enalapril/farmacología , Humanos , Mediadores de Inflamación/metabolismo , Túbulos Renales Proximales/metabolismo , Túbulos Renales Proximales/patología , Masculino , Nefritis/metabolismo , Nefritis/patología , Fosforilación , Ratas Zucker , Transducción de Señal , Proteína smad3/metabolismo
12.
Anal Chem ; 92(7): 4731-4735, 2020 04 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32159946

RESUMEN

Obtaining kinetic and thermodynamic information for protein amyloid formation can yield new insight into the mechanistic details of this biomedically important process. The kinetics of the structural change that initiates the amyloid pathway, however, has been challenging to access for any amyloid protein system. Here, using the protein ß-2-microglobulin (ß2m) as a model, we measure the kinetics and energy barrier associated with an initial amyloidogenic structural change. Using covalent labeling and mass spectrometry, we measure the decrease in solvent accessibility of one of ß2m's Trp residues, which is buried during the initial structural change, as a way to probe the kinetics of this structural change at different temperatures and under different amyloid forming conditions. Our results provide the first-ever measure of the activation barrier for a structural change that initiates the amyloid formation pathway. The results also yield new mechanistic insight into ß2m's amyloidogenic structural change, especially the role of Pro32 isomerization in this reaction.


Asunto(s)
Termodinámica , Microglobulina beta-2/química , Humanos , Cinética , Modelos Moleculares , Conformación Proteica
13.
J Exp Biol ; 222(Pt Suppl 1)2019 02 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30728233

RESUMEN

A basic set of navigation strategies supports navigational tasks ranging from homing to novel detours and shortcuts. To perform these last two tasks, it is generally thought that humans, mammals and perhaps some insects possess Euclidean cognitive maps, constructed on the basis of input from the path integration system. In this article, I review the rationale and behavioral evidence for this metric cognitive map hypothesis, and find it unpersuasive: in practice, there is little evidence for truly novel shortcuts in animals, and human performance is highly unreliable and biased by environmental features. I develop the alternative hypothesis that spatial knowledge is better characterized as a labeled graph: a network of paths between places augmented with local metric information. What distinguishes such a cognitive graph from a metric cognitive map is that this local information is not embedded in a global coordinate system, so spatial knowledge is often geometrically inconsistent. Human path integration appears to be better suited to piecewise measurements of path lengths and turn angles than to building a consistent map. In a series of experiments in immersive virtual reality, we tested human navigation in non-Euclidean environments and found that shortcuts manifest large violations of the metric postulates. The results are contrary to the Euclidean map hypothesis and support the cognitive graph hypothesis. Apparently Euclidean behavior, such as taking novel detours and approximate shortcuts, can be explained by the adaptive use of non-Euclidean strategies.


Asunto(s)
Cognición , Percepción Espacial , Navegación Espacial , Humanos
14.
Proc Biol Sci ; 285(1878)2018 05 16.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29769363

RESUMEN

It is commonly believed that global patterns of motion in flocks, schools and crowds emerge from local interactions between individuals, through a process of self-organization. The key to explaining such collective behaviour thus lies in deciphering these local interactions. We take an experiment-driven approach to modelling collective motion in human crowds. Previously, we observed that a pedestrian aligns their velocity vector (speed and heading direction) with that of a neighbour. Here we investigate the neighbourhood of interaction in a crowd: which neighbours influence a pedestrian's behaviour, how this depends on neighbour position, and how the influences of multiple neighbours are combined. In three experiments, a participant walked in a virtual crowd whose speed and heading were manipulated. We find that neighbour influence is linearly combined and decreases with distance, but not with lateral position (eccentricity). We model the neighbourhood as (i) a circularly symmetric region with (ii) a weighted average of neighbours, (iii) a uni-directional influence, and (iv) weights that decay exponentially to zero by 5 m. The model reproduces the experimental data and predicts individual trajectories in observational data on a human 'swarm'. The results yield the first bottom-up model of collective crowd motion.


Asunto(s)
Aglomeración , Caminata , Humanos , Modelos Teóricos
15.
Behav Brain Sci ; 41: e250, 2018 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30767832

RESUMEN

We highlight that optimal cue combination does not represent a general principle of cue interaction during navigation, extending Rahnev & Denison's (R&D) summary of nonoptimal perceptual decisions to the navigation domain. However, we argue that the term "suboptimality" does not capture the way visual and nonvisual cues interact in navigational decisions.


Asunto(s)
Señales (Psicología) , Toma de Decisiones , Humanos
16.
Exp Brain Res ; 235(6): 1885-1897, 2017 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28303327

RESUMEN

Path integration is fundamental to human navigation. When a navigator leaves home on a complex outbound path, they are able to keep track of their approximate position and orientation and return to their starting location on a direct homebound path. However, there are several sources of error during path integration. Previous research has focused almost exclusively on encoding error-the error in registering the outbound path in memory. Here, we also consider execution error-the error in the response, such as turning and walking a homebound trajectory. In two experiments conducted in ambulatory virtual environments, we examined the contribution of execution error to the rotational component of path integration using angle reproduction tasks. In the reproduction tasks, participants rotated once and then rotated again to face the original direction, either reproducing the initial turn or turning through the supplementary angle. One outstanding difficulty in disentangling encoding and execution error during a typical angle reproduction task is that as the encoding angle increases, so does the required response angle. In Experiment 1, we dissociated these two variables by asking participants to report each encoding angle using two different responses: by turning to walk on a path parallel to the initial facing direction in the same (reproduction) or opposite (supplementary angle) direction. In Experiment 2, participants reported the encoding angle by turning both rightward and leftward onto a path parallel to the initial facing direction, over a larger range of angles. The results suggest that execution error, not encoding error, is the predominant source of error in angular path integration. These findings also imply that the path integrator uses an intrinsic (action-scaled) rather than an extrinsic (objective) metric.


Asunto(s)
Actividad Motora/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Navegación Espacial/fisiología , Realidad Virtual , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
17.
Lung ; 195(5): 601-606, 2017 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28623537

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: The literature is devoid of a comprehensive analysis of silicone airway stenting for benign central airway obstruction (BCAO). With the largest series in the literature to date, we aim to demonstrate the safety profile, pattern of re-intervention, and duration of silicone airway stents. METHODS: An institutional database was used to identify patients with BCAO who underwent rigid bronchoscopy with dilation and silicone stent placement between 2002 and 2015 at Rush University Medical Center. RESULTS: During the study period, 243 stents were utilized in 63 patients with BCAO. Pure tracheal stenosis was encountered in 71% (45/63), pure tracheomalacia in 11% (7/63), and a hybrid of both in 17% (11/63). Median freedom from re-intervention was 104 (IQR 167) days. Most common indications for re-intervention include mucus accumulation (60%; 131/220), migration (28%; 62/220), and intubation (8%; 18/220). The most common diameters of stent placed were 12 mm (94/220) and 14 mm (96/220). The most common lengths utilized were 30 mm (60/220) and 40 mm (77/220). Duration was not effected by stent size when placed for discrete stenosis. However, 14 mm stents outperformed 12 mm when tracheomalacia was present (157 vs. 37 days; p = 0.005). Patients with a hybrid stenosis fared better when longer stents were used (60 mm stents outlasted 40 mm stents 173 vs. 56 days; p = 0.05). CONCLUSION: Rigid bronchoscopy with silicone airway stenting is a safe and effective option for the management of benign central airway obstruction. Our results highlight several strategies to improve stent duration.


Asunto(s)
Obstrucción de las Vías Aéreas/cirugía , Broncoscopía , Falla de Prótesis , Siliconas , Stents , Estenosis Traqueal/cirugía , Traqueomalacia/cirugía , Adulto , Anciano , Obstrucción de las Vías Aéreas/etiología , Bases de Datos Factuales , Dilatación , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Estudios Retrospectivos , Estenosis Traqueal/complicaciones , Traqueomalacia/complicaciones
18.
J Vis ; 17(5): 12, 2017 05 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28538992

RESUMEN

When walking to intercept a moving target, people take an interception path that appears to anticipate the target's trajectory. According to the constant bearing strategy, the observer holds the bearing direction of the target constant based on current visual information, consistent with on-line control. Alternatively, the interception path might be based on an internal model of the target's motion, known as model-based control. To investigate these two accounts, participants walked to intercept a moving target in a virtual environment. We degraded the target's visibility by blurring the target to varying degrees in the midst of a trial, in order to influence its perceived speed and position. Reduced levels of visibility progressively impaired interception accuracy and precision; total occlusion impaired performance most and yielded nonadaptive heading adjustments. Thus, performance strongly depended on current visual information and deteriorated qualitatively when it was withdrawn. The results imply that locomotor interception is normally guided by current information rather than an internal model of target motion, consistent with on-line control.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Caminata/fisiología , Adulto Joven
19.
Anesth Analg ; 123(2): 396-401, 2016 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27028773

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Hormonal action has been implicated as a possible mechanism for pregnancy-induced analgesia. Previous investigators have reported an increase in heat pain tolerance during labor compared with nonpregnant controls and postulated it was because of the hormonal changes during pregnancy. However, these previous reports did not include measurement of hormonal values. The purpose of our study was to quantitatively test if changes in pregnancy hormone concentrations correlated with changes in temperature ratings. METHODS: This was a prospective cohort study consisting of 32 women scheduled for elective cesarean delivery at term between July 2010 and January 2013. Heat pain threshold and tolerance, estrogen, progesterone, and oxytocin levels were measured twice in each patient at term and again 4 to 8 weeks postpartum. RESULTS: All hormone levels decreased significantly between term pregnancy and the postpartum visit (all P values < 0.029). However, there were no statistically significant differences between term and postpartum heat pain measurements. The mean baseline heat pain threshold was 40.9°C at term compared with 40.3°C °postpartum (P = 0.47; mean change, -0.6°C; 95% confidence interval of change, -1.8°C to +0.7°C). The mean baseline heat pain tolerance was 46.1°C at term and 46.0°C postpartum (P = 0.59; mean change, -0.1°C; 95% confidence interval of change, -0.8°C° to +0.6°C). CONCLUSIONS: Our findings show that amounts of estradiol and progesterone changed significantly between the term and the postpartum visit; however, the thermal pain tolerance did not significantly change. In summary, we did not observe an association between hormonal changes and changes in pain threshold measures. This finding argues against the concept of simple progesterone- or estrogen-induced analgesia in humans.


Asunto(s)
Estradiol/sangre , Calor/efectos adversos , Oxitocina/sangre , Umbral del Dolor , Dolor/etiología , Progesterona/sangre , Adulto , Biomarcadores/sangre , Femenino , Humanos , Dolor/diagnóstico , Dolor/fisiopatología , Dimensión del Dolor , Periodo Posparto , Embarazo , Estudios Prospectivos , Nacimiento a Término
20.
Lung ; 194(4): 619-24, 2016 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27107874

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: Pulmonary lobectomy with en bloc chest wall resection is a common strategy for treating lung cancers invading the chest wall. We hypothesized a direct relationship exists between number of ribs resected and postoperative respiratory complications. METHODS: An institutional database was queried for patients with non-small cell lung cancer that underwent lobectomy with en bloc chest wall resection between 2003 and 2014. Propensity matching was used to identify a cohort of patients who underwent lobectomy via thoracotomy without chest wall resection. Patients were propensity matched on age, gender, smoking history, FEV1, and DLCO. The relationship between number of ribs resected and postoperative respiratory complications (bronchoscopy, re-intubation, pneumonia, or tracheostomy) was examined. RESULTS: Sixty-eight patients (34 chest wall resections; 34 without chest wall resection) were divided into 3 cohorts: cohort A = 0 ribs resected (n = 34), cohort B = 1-3 ribs resected (n = 24), and cohort C = 4-6 ribs resected (n = 10). Patient demographics were similar between cohorts. The 90-day mortality rate was 2.9 % (2/68) and did not vary between cohorts. On multivariate analysis, having 1-3 ribs resected (OR 19.29, 95 % CI (1.33, 280.72); p = 0.03), 4-6 ribs resected [OR 26.66, (1.48, 481.86); p = 0.03), and a lower DLCO (OR 0.91, (0.84, 0.99); p = 0.02) were associated with postoperative respiratory complications. CONCLUSIONS: In patients undergoing lobectomy with en bloc chest wall resection for non-small cell lung cancer, the number of ribs resected is directly associated with incidence of postoperative respiratory complications.


Asunto(s)
Carcinoma de Pulmón de Células no Pequeñas/cirugía , Neoplasias Pulmonares/cirugía , Neumonectomía/efectos adversos , Neumonectomía/métodos , Costillas/cirugía , Pared Torácica/cirugía , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Broncoscopía , Carcinoma de Pulmón de Células no Pequeñas/patología , Femenino , Humanos , Intubación Intratraqueal , Tiempo de Internación , Neoplasias Pulmonares/patología , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Invasividad Neoplásica , Neumonectomía/mortalidad , Neumonía Bacteriana/etiología , Complicaciones Posoperatorias/etiología , Capacidad de Difusión Pulmonar , Pared Torácica/patología , Toracotomía , Traqueostomía
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA