Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 839
Filtrar
1.
Hippocampus ; 34(7): 342-356, 2024 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38780087

RESUMO

Although the phenomenon of memory formation and recall associated with the use of psychotropic drugs has been extensively studied, mechanisms underlying memories for natural reward have not been clarified. Herein, we test the hypothesis that glutamatergic receptors in the dentate gyrus play a role in memories associated with sucrose. We used pellet self-administration protocol to generate memories in two-port nose-poke discrimination task using male Wistar rats. During non-rewarded probe trial, the conditioned animals readily discriminated the active port versus inactive port and showed massive increase in mRNA expression of AMPA receptor subunit genes (gria2, gria3) as well as c-Fos protein in the DG. Access to sweet pellet further enhanced c-Fos expression in the DG. However, animals pre-treated with AMPA receptor antagonist CNQX (intra-DG), on exposure to operant chamber (no pellet), showed decreased discrimination as well as c-Fos expression. We suggest that AMPA receptors in DG mediate recall and consolidation of memories associated with sucrose consumption. CNQX pre-treated animals, if presented with sweet pellet on nose poke, exhibited high discrimination index coupled with increased c-Fos expression. In these CNQX treated rats, the DI was again decreased following administration of NMDA receptor antagonist AP5. We suggest that, although AMPA receptors are blocked, the access to sweet pellet may induce surge of glutamate in the DG, which in turn may reinstate memories via activation of erstwhile silent synapses in NMDA dependant manner.


Assuntos
Giro Denteado , Ratos Wistar , Receptores de AMPA , Receptores de N-Metil-D-Aspartato , Sacarose , Animais , Masculino , Receptores de AMPA/metabolismo , Receptores de AMPA/antagonistas & inibidores , Sacarose/administração & dosagem , Giro Denteado/efeitos dos fármacos , Giro Denteado/metabolismo , Receptores de N-Metil-D-Aspartato/metabolismo , Receptores de N-Metil-D-Aspartato/antagonistas & inibidores , Antagonistas de Aminoácidos Excitatórios/farmacologia , Proteínas Proto-Oncogênicas c-fos/metabolismo , Ratos , 6-Ciano-7-nitroquinoxalina-2,3-diona/farmacologia , Memória/fisiologia , Memória/efeitos dos fármacos , Condicionamento Operante/efeitos dos fármacos , Condicionamento Operante/fisiologia , Discriminação Psicológica/efeitos dos fármacos , Discriminação Psicológica/fisiologia , Autoadministração , RNA Mensageiro/metabolismo , Aprendizagem por Discriminação/efeitos dos fármacos , Aprendizagem por Discriminação/fisiologia
2.
Behav Pharmacol ; 35(1): 36-46, 2024 Feb 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38085665

RESUMO

Cannabidiol (CBD) is one of the major centrally active phytocannabinoid components of cannabis, and has been approved by the FDA only for the treatment of seizures associated with three rare disorders. It has also been touted as a potential treatment for anxiety in place of more traditional treatments like benzodiazepines. Although there is some evidence of anxiolytic effects of CBD, its suitability as a substitute for benzodiazepines is unknown. This experiment was designed to assess the extent to which CBD shares interoceptive discriminative-stimulus properties with the anxiolytic drug chlordiazepoxide (CDP), a benzodiazepine. In the present experiment, a range of doses (0-1569 mg/kg) of over-the-counter CBD oil was administered (i.g.) in male Sprague-Dawley rats trained to discriminate 5.6 mg/kg CDP from saline. Due to the long time-course effects of CBD, generalization tests were conducted at 90 and 120 min post-CBD administration. The two highest doses of CBD tested (1064 and 1569 mg/kg) were found to partially substitute for 5.6 mg/kg CDP, with mean percent responding on the CDP-associated lever reaching above 20% at time 2 (120 min post-CBD administration), suggesting that high doses of the over-the-counter CBD oils used in this experiment share interoceptive discriminative-stimulus properties to some degree with CDP. These results are novel in comparison to existing research into stimulus effects of CBD, in which substitution for benzodiazepines has not previously been observed.


Assuntos
Canabidiol , Ratos , Masculino , Animais , Ratos Sprague-Dawley , Canabidiol/farmacologia , Clordiazepóxido/farmacologia , Aprendizagem por Discriminação , Benzodiazepinas/farmacologia
3.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 48(8): 1165-1192, 2022 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35404645

RESUMO

The quest for determining the degree of learning difficulty associated with different types of categories has been instrumental in our understanding of human categorization behavior and, more broadly, human generalization. For instance, we now know that the topological nature of the dimensions (e.g., whether these are integral or separable) that define the family of categories generated with three binary dimensions yield two different learning difficulty orderings. In our study, for the first time, we replicated one such classic ordering involving integral dimensions and explored the impact that the choice of dimensional values, along with the nature of the stimulus presentation, had on learning difficulty during the learning phase of our four experiments. Two standard orderings were observed in our investigation which were consistent with the hypothesis that multilevel discrimination plays a key role in concept learning. We accounted for our empirical results by making this role explicit at three levels, the feature, object, and category structure levels, using a dual discrimination invariance model (DDIM) derived from the core invariance law of generalized invariance structure theory (GIST). The model involves a "discrimination switch" via what is referred to in GIST as the "tau discrimination threshold" where two distinct extreme degrees of discrimination yield two fundamental learning difficulty orderings able to account for results from our experiments without free parameters. We then show how the DDIM is not only a more accurate and general predictor than the best alternatives but also provides a plausible and tenable cognitive mechanism for understanding these behaviors. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).


Assuntos
Tumores do Estroma Gastrointestinal , Formação de Conceito , Aprendizagem por Discriminação , Generalização Psicológica , Humanos , Aprendizagem
4.
Psychopharmacology (Berl) ; 239(3): 819-830, 2022 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35137248

RESUMO

RATIONALE: Bupropion is a non-nicotine medication for smoking cessation that has overlapping stimulus effects with nicotine as demonstrated in drug discrimination studies. Whether these shared stimulus effects will alter acquisition or maintenance of a discrimination between nicotine and bupropion is unknown. OBJECTIVE: We sought to test this possibility using the drug discriminated goal-tracking (DGT) task and whether discrimination training history affected generalization and substitution tests. METHODS: Sixty adult Sprague-Dawley rats (30M/30F) were equally split into three discrimination training groups: SAL-0.4NIC, 10BUP-0.4NIC, and 20BUP-0.4NIC. On nicotine days, all rats were administered subcutaneously 0.4 mg/kg nicotine and had intermittent access to liquid sucrose. On intermixed non-reinforced days, rats were administered intraperitoneally saline, 10 or 20 mg/kg bupropion. Upon completion, a range of nicotine and bupropion doses were assessed before substitution tests with varenicline and sazetidine-A were conducted. RESULTS: The SAL-0.4NIC and 10BUP-0.4NIC groups readily discriminated by session 8, as evidenced by increased dipper entries (goal-tracking) on nicotine days. The 20BUP-0.4NIC group was slower to acquire the discrimination. Female rats, regardless of group, had higher conditioned responding evoked by the lowest dose of nicotine (0.025 mg/kg) in the dose-effect curve. The discrimination required rats to learn to withhold responding to the training dose of bupropion. This withholding of excitatory dipper entries generalized to other doses. Varenicline and sazetidine-A partially substituted for the nicotine stimulus, and this pattern did not differ with training history. CONCLUSIONS: We are the first to study a drug-drug discrimination using the DGT task. Females appeared to have a lower discrimination threshold for nicotine that was not impacted by the learning history. Further work on the importance of sex as a biological variable and how the complex interoceptive stimulus effects of nicotine can vary with training histories is needed.


Assuntos
Bupropiona , Nicotina , Animais , Bupropiona/farmacologia , Aprendizagem por Discriminação , Relação Dose-Resposta a Droga , Feminino , Objetivos , Nicotina/farmacologia , Agonistas Nicotínicos/farmacologia , Ratos , Ratos Sprague-Dawley
5.
Exp Clin Psychopharmacol ; 30(3): 253-268, 2022 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33119384

RESUMO

This review of research on behavioral discrimination of nicotine and how it informs public health policy for reducing risk of tobacco dependence is adapted from Kenneth A. Perkins's American Psychological Association Division 28 (Psychopharmacology and Substance Abuse) 2020 Med Associates Brady/Schuster Award Lecture. The author's initial programmatic clinical research on nicotine is introduced, especially efforts to develop and validate a novel method of acute nicotine dosing. After the public health rationale for characterizing the discriminative stimulus effects of nicotine in humans are described, details from two separate programs of research on nicotine discrimination in humans are presented. The first, conducted with nicotine dosing by nasal spray, documented that humans could discriminate nicotine administered rapidly, examined nicotine's neuropharmacological specificity, identified discrimination threshold dose in smokers and nonsmokers, and explored other conditions that might alter ability to discriminate its effects. The second, more recent program focused on threshold doses for discrimination of nicotine by cigarette smoking, a program that was very difficult to do until the past decade, and how nicotine's self-reported "reward" and preference via choice behavior relate to its discriminability. Differences due to menthol and degree of tobacco dependence were also examined. For each of these two programs, the main findings of selected studies are noted, followed by very recent work on nicotine discrimination and choice that informs Food and Drug Administration's efforts to formulate public policy to improve health and reduce the nearly half million American deaths per year due to persistent tobacco use. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).


Assuntos
Nicotina , Tabagismo , Aprendizagem por Discriminação , Relação Dose-Resposta a Droga , Humanos , Nicotina/farmacologia , Reforço Psicológico , Tabagismo/psicologia
6.
Neurobiol Learn Mem ; 185: 107507, 2021 11.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34474155

RESUMO

Our memory for time is a fundamental ability that we use to judge the duration of events, put our experiences into a temporal context, and decide when to initiate actions. The medial entorhinal cortex (MEC), with its direct projections to the hippocampus, has been proposed to be the key source of temporal information for hippocampal time cells. However, the behavioral relevance of such temporal firing patterns remains unclear, as most of the paradigms used for the study of temporal processing and time cells are either spatial tasks or tasks for which MEC function is not required. In this study, we asked whether the MEC is necessary for rats to perform a time duration discrimination task (TDD), in which rats were trained to discriminate between 10-s and 20-s delay intervals. After reaching a 90% performance criterion, the rats were assigned to receive an excitotoxic MEC-lesion or sham-lesion surgery. We found that after recovering from surgery, rats with MEC lesions were impaired on the TDD task in comparison to rats with sham lesions, failing to return to criterion performance. Their impairment, however, was specific to the longer, 20-s delay trials. These results indicate that time processing is dependent on MEC neural computations only for delays that exceed 10 s, perhaps because long-term memory resources are needed to keep track of longer time intervals.


Assuntos
Córtex Entorrinal/fisiologia , Memória Episódica , Percepção do Tempo/fisiologia , Animais , Condicionamento Operante/fisiologia , Aprendizagem por Discriminação , Córtex Entorrinal/lesões , Masculino , Transtornos da Memória/fisiopatologia , Ratos , Ratos Long-Evans
7.
Behav Pharmacol ; 32(7): 581-589, 2021 10 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34417356

RESUMO

Despite the evidence that the muscarinic agonist arecoline is a drug of abuse throughout Southeast Asia, its stimulus characteristics have not been well studied. The goal of this work was to understand more about the mediation of discriminative stimulus effects of arecoline. Arecoline (1.0 mg/kg s.c.) was trained as a discriminative stimulus in a group of eight rats. The ability of various cholinergic agonists and antagonists to mimic or antagonize the discriminative stimulus effects of arecoline and to modify its rate-suppressing effects was evaluated. A muscarinic antagonist, but neither of two nicotinic antagonists, was able to modify the discriminative stimulus effects of arecoline, suggesting a predominant muscarinic basis of arecoline's discriminative stimulus effects in this assay. However, both nicotine itself and two nicotine agonists with selective affinity for the α4ß2* receptor (ispronicline and metanicotine) produced full arecoline-like discriminative stimulus effects in these rats. The discriminative stimulus effects of the selective nicotine agonists were blocked by both the general nicotine antagonist mecamylamine and by the selective α4ß2* antagonist, dihydro-beta-erythroidine (DHßE). Surprisingly, only DHßE antagonized the rate-suppressing effects of the selective nicotine agonists. These data indicate a selective α4ß2* nicotine receptor component to the behavioral effects of arecoline. Although the nicotinic aspects of arecoline's behavior effects could suggest that abuse of arecoline-containing material (e.g. betel nut chewing) is mediated through nicotinic rather than muscarinic actions, further research, specifically on the reinforcing effects of arecoline, is necessary before this conclusion can be supported.


Assuntos
Arecolina/farmacologia , Comportamento Animal/efeitos dos fármacos , Di-Hidro-beta-Eritroidina/farmacologia , Mecamilamina/farmacologia , Nicotina/análogos & derivados , Piridinas/farmacologia , Transtornos Relacionados ao Uso de Substâncias , Animais , Aprendizagem por Discriminação/efeitos dos fármacos , Agonistas Muscarínicos/farmacologia , Nicotina/farmacologia , Agonistas Nicotínicos/farmacologia , Antagonistas Nicotínicos/farmacologia , Ratos , Receptores Nicotínicos/metabolismo , Transtornos Relacionados ao Uso de Substâncias/metabolismo , Transtornos Relacionados ao Uso de Substâncias/psicologia
8.
J Neurosci ; 41(21): 4631-4640, 2021 05 26.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33849950

RESUMO

Theoretical and modeling studies demonstrate that heterosynaptic plasticity-changes at synapses inactive during induction-facilitates fine-grained discriminative learning in Hebbian-type systems, and helps to achieve a robust ability for repetitive learning. A dearth of tools for selective manipulation has hindered experimental analysis of the proposed role of heterosynaptic plasticity in behavior. Here we circumvent this obstacle by testing specific predictions about the behavioral consequences of the impairment of heterosynaptic plasticity by experimental manipulations to adenosine A1 receptors (A1Rs). Our prior work demonstrated that the blockade of adenosine A1 receptors impairs heterosynaptic plasticity in brain slices and, when implemented in computer models, selectively impairs repetitive learning on sequential tasks. Based on this work, we predict that A1R knock-out (KO) mice will express (1) impairment of heterosynaptic plasticity and (2) behavioral deficits in learning on sequential tasks. Using electrophysiological experiments in slices and behavioral testing of animals of both sexes, we show that, compared with wild-type controls, A1R KO mice have impaired synaptic plasticity in visual cortex neurons, coupled with significant deficits in visual discrimination learning. Deficits in A1R knockouts were seen specifically during relearning, becoming progressively more apparent with learning on sequential visual discrimination tasks of increasing complexity. These behavioral results confirm our model predictions and provide the first experimental evidence for a proposed role of heterosynaptic plasticity in organism-level learning. Moreover, these results identify heterosynaptic plasticity as a new potential target for interventions that may help to enhance new learning on a background of existing memories.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Understanding how interacting forms of synaptic plasticity mediate learning is fundamental for neuroscience. Theory and modeling revealed that, in addition to Hebbian-type associative plasticity, heterosynaptic changes at synapses that were not active during induction are necessary for stable system operation and fine-grained discrimination learning. However, lacking tools for selective manipulation prevented behavioral analysis of heterosynaptic plasticity. Here we circumvent this barrier: from our prior experimental and computational work we predict differential behavioral consequences of the impairment of Hebbian-type versus heterosynaptic plasticity. We show that, in adenosine A1 receptor knock-out mice, impaired synaptic plasticity in visual cortex neurons is coupled with specific deficits in learning sequential, increasingly complex visual discrimination tasks. This provides the first evidence linking heterosynaptic plasticity to organism-level learning.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem por Discriminação/fisiologia , Plasticidade Neuronal/fisiologia , Receptor A1 de Adenosina/metabolismo , Córtex Visual/fisiologia , Animais , Feminino , Masculino , Camundongos , Camundongos Knockout
9.
Sci Rep ; 11(1): 8656, 2021 04 21.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33883618

RESUMO

Calcium-dependent activator protein for secretion 1 (CAPS1) is a key molecule in vesicular exocytosis, probably in the priming step. However, CAPS1's role in synaptic plasticity and brain function is elusive. Herein, we showed that synaptic plasticity and learning behavior were impaired in forebrain and/or hippocampus-specific Caps1 conditional knockout (cKO) mice by means of molecular, physiological, and behavioral analyses. Neonatal Caps1 cKO mice showed a decrease in the number of docked vesicles in the hippocampal CA3 region, with no detectable changes in the distribution of other major exocytosis-related molecules. Additionally, long-term potentiation (LTP) was partially and severely impaired in the CA1 and CA3 regions, respectively. CA1 LTP was reinforced by repeated high-frequency stimuli, whereas CA3 LTP was completely abolished. Accordingly, hippocampus-associated learning was severely impaired in adeno-associated virus (AAV) infection-mediated postnatal Caps1 cKO mice. Collectively, our findings suggest that CAPS1 is a key protein involved in the cellular mechanisms underlying hippocampal synaptic release and plasticity, which is crucial for hippocampus-associated learning.


Assuntos
Proteínas de Ligação ao Cálcio/fisiologia , Hipocampo/fisiologia , Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Proteínas do Tecido Nervoso/fisiologia , Plasticidade Neuronal/fisiologia , Animais , Western Blotting , Proteínas de Ligação ao Cálcio/metabolismo , Condicionamento Clássico , Aprendizagem por Discriminação , Feminino , Hipocampo/metabolismo , Hipocampo/ultraestrutura , Masculino , Camundongos , Camundongos Endogâmicos C57BL , Camundongos Knockout , Microscopia Eletrônica , Proteínas do Tecido Nervoso/metabolismo , Frações Subcelulares/metabolismo
10.
Gac. méd. espirit ; 23(1): 66-74, ene.-abr. 2021. tab
Artigo em Espanhol | LILACS | ID: biblio-1250007

RESUMO

RESUMEN Fundamento: La calidad de los instrumentos evaluativos resulta esencial en el proceso de evaluación del aprendizaje. Objetivo: Determinar los índices de dificultad y discriminación del examen final ordinario de la asignatura Ontogenia Humana y Soma. Metodología: Se realizó un estudio retrospectivo transversal en la asignatura Ontogenia y SOMA, del curso 2017-2018 en la Universidad de Ciencias Médicas de Sancti Spíritus, en el que se procesaron 163 exámenes ordinarios finales, el 30 % de los examinados seleccionados mediante un muestreo aleatorio estratificado por grupos, se calcularon el índice de dificultad e índice de discriminación por preguntas, temas y ciencias. Resultados: El número de incisos esperados y reales, de acuerdo con las horas clases para cada ciencia estuvo ajustado, no así en las temáticas Ontogenia de 17 incisos esperados se dedicaron 12 (70.5 %), en SOMA de 34 incisos esperados se dedicaron 29 (85.2 %); el índice de dificultad por temáticas, ciencias y temarios fue medianamente fácil (0.74-0.86) al igual que para el examen en su conjunto. El índice de discriminación del tema Ontogenia fue superior en el temario 1 (T1: 0.37 vs. T2: 0.24) y similar en SOMA (T1: 0.40 vs. T2: 0.39) y a nivel de las ciencias [Embriología (T1: 0.39 vs. T2: 0.31), Anatomía (T1: 0.39 vs. T2: 0.37)]. A nivel de preguntas, el índice de discriminación más bajo lo tuvo la pregunta 1 del primer temario (0.22), el resto tuvo índices de discriminación superiores a 0.30. Conclusiones: Ambos temarios tienen un índice de discriminación similar. El índice de dificultad fue medianamente fácil.


ABSTRACT Background: The quality of the assessment instruments is essential in the learning assessment process. Objective: To determine the difficulty and discernment indexes of the Human Ontogeny and SOMA ordinary final test subject. Methodology: A cross-sectional retrospective study was conducted on the Ontogeny and SOMA subject, from 2017 to 2018 academic year at the Sancti Spíritus Faculty of Medical Sciences, 163 final ordinary tests were processed, 30 % of the examinees selected by stratified random cluster sampling, the difficulty and discernment index were calculated by questions, topics and sciences. Results: The number of expected and real items, according to the class hours for each science, was adjusted, but not in the subjects (Ontogeny of 17 expected items were dedicated 12 (70.5 %), in SOMA of 34 expected items 29 were dedicated (85.2 %), the difficulty index by subjects, sciences and syllabus was moderately easy (0.74-0.86) as for the exam as a whole. The discernment index of the topic Ontogeny was higher in the syllabus 1 (T1: 0.37 vs. T2: 0.24) and similar in SOMA (T1: 0.40 vs. T2: 0.39) and [Embryology (T1: 0.39 vs. T2: 0.31), Anatomy (T1: 0.39 vs. T2: 0.37)]. At the science level questions, the lowest discernment index was found in question 1 from the first syllabus (0.22), the rest had discernment indexes higher than 0.30. Conclusions: Both topics have a similar discernment index. The difficulty index was moderately easy.


Assuntos
Faculdades de Medicina , Estudo de Validação , Aprendizagem por Discriminação
11.
Pharmacol Biochem Behav ; 205: 173173, 2021 06.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33753118

RESUMO

The current study investigated whether the stimulus effects of morphine can function as a positive and negative feature in a Pavlovian occasion setting drug discrimination preparation in male and female rats. Sprague-Dawley rats were assigned to a feature positive (FP) or feature negative (FN) training group and all received intermixed morphine (3.2 mg/kg, IP) or saline injections 15 min before 20-min daily training sessions. For FP rats, on morphine sessions, each of eight 15-s white noise (WN) presentations was followed by 4-s access to sucrose (0.01 ml, 26% w/v); on saline sessions, sucrose was withheld. FN rats learned the reverse contingency. FP discrimination was acquired somewhat sooner than FN discrimination, and females, but not males, became sensitized to the locomotor effects of morphine, which did not influence conditioned responding. Rats then entered dose generalization testing. There was no sex difference in dose generalization for FN groups (ED50 1.26 for males and 1.57 for females). Yet for FP rats, the dose response curve for females was shifted to the right compared to males (ED50 0.54 for males and 1.94 for females). FP females exhibited enhanced responding at a dose higher than that of their original training. These findings reveal the need to reassess our notions of drug stimuli that guide appropriate associative behaviours from the perspective of sex differences.


Assuntos
Condicionamento Clássico/efeitos dos fármacos , Aprendizagem por Discriminação/efeitos dos fármacos , Morfina/farmacologia , Analgésicos Opioides/farmacologia , Animais , Comportamento Animal/efeitos dos fármacos , Relação Dose-Resposta a Droga , Feminino , Generalização do Estímulo/efeitos dos fármacos , Locomoção/efeitos dos fármacos , Masculino , Ratos , Ratos Sprague-Dawley , Caracteres Sexuais
12.
Biol Open ; 10(3)2021 03 26.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33737293

RESUMO

Flying insects track turbulent odor plumes to find mates, food and egg-laying sites. To maintain contact with the plume, insects are thought to adapt their flight control according to the distribution of odor in the plume using the timing of odor onsets and intervals between odor encounters. Although timing cues are important, few studies have addressed whether insects are capable of deriving spatial information about odor distribution from bilateral comparisons between their antennae in flight. The proboscis extension reflex (PER) associative learning protocol, originally developed to study odor learning in honeybees, was used as a tool to ask if hawkmoths, Manduca sexta, can discriminate between odor stimuli arriving on either antenna. We show moths discriminated the odor arrival side with an accuracy of >70%. Information about spatial distribution of odor stimuli may be available to moths searching for odor sources, opening the possibility that they use both spatial and temporal odor information.This article has an associated First Person interview with the first author of the paper.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem por Discriminação , Comportamento Alimentar , Manduca/fisiologia , Odorantes , Comportamento Sexual Animal , Animais , Feminino
13.
Behav Brain Res ; 400: 113024, 2021 02 26.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33290755

RESUMO

Inaccurate discrimination between threat and safety cues is a common symptom of anxiety disorders such as Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD). Although females experience higher rates of these disorders than males, the body of literature examining sex differences in safety learning is still growing. Learning to discriminate safety cues from threat cues requires downregulating fear to the safety cue while continuing to express fear to the threat cue. However, successful discrimination between safety and threat cues does not necessarily guarantee that the safety cue can effectively reduce fear to the threat cue when they are presented together. The conditioned inhibitory ability of a safety cue to reduce fear in the presence of both safety and threat is most likely dependent on the ability to discriminate between the two. There are relatively few studies exploring conditioned inhibition as a method of safety learning. Adding to this knowledge gap is the general lack of inclusion of female subjects within these studies. In this review, we provide a qualitative review of our current knowledge of sex differences in safety discrimination versus conditioned inhibition in both humans and rodents. Overall, the literature suggests that while females and males perform similarly in discrimination learning, females show deficits in conditioned inhibition compared to males. Furthermore, while estrogen appears to have a protective effect on safety learning in humans, increased estrogen in female rodents appears to be correlated with impaired safety learning performance.


Assuntos
Condicionamento Clássico/fisiologia , Aprendizagem por Discriminação/fisiologia , Estrogênios/fisiologia , Medo/fisiologia , Inibição Psicológica , Segurança , Caracteres Sexuais , Animais , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
14.
Behav Brain Res ; 399: 112917, 2021 02 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32949641

RESUMO

Hippocampal circuitry is continuously modified by integration of adult-born dentate granule cells (DGCs). Prior work has shown that enhancing adult hippocampal neurogenesis decreases interference or overlap or conflict between ensembles of similar contexts and promotes discrimination of a shock-associated context from a similar, neutral context. However, the impact of enhanced integration of adult-born neurons on hippocampal network activity or downstream circuits such as the dorsolateral septum that mediate defensive behavioral responses is poorly understood. Here, we first replicated our finding that genetic expansion of the population of adult-born dentate granule cells (8 weeks and younger) promotes contextual fear discrimination. We found that enhanced contextual fear discrimination is associated with greater c-Fos expression in discrete hippocampal subfields along the proximo-distal and dorsoventral axis. Examination of the dorsolateral septum revealed an increase in activation of somatostatin expressing neurons consistent with recent characterization of these cells as calibrators of defensive behavior. Together, these findings begin to shed light on how genetically enhancing adult hippocampal neurogenesis affects activity of hippocampal-dorsolateral septal circuits.


Assuntos
Região CA3 Hipocampal/fisiologia , Giro Denteado/fisiologia , Aprendizagem por Discriminação/fisiologia , Medo/fisiologia , Neurogênese/fisiologia , Neurônios/fisiologia , Septo Pelúcido/fisiologia , Somatostatina/metabolismo , Animais , Região CA3 Hipocampal/citologia , Região CA3 Hipocampal/metabolismo , Giro Denteado/citologia , Giro Denteado/metabolismo , Masculino , Camundongos , Camundongos Endogâmicos C57BL , Camundongos Transgênicos , Neurônios/metabolismo , Proteínas Proto-Oncogênicas c-fos , Septo Pelúcido/citologia , Septo Pelúcido/metabolismo
15.
Behav Brain Res ; 396: 112884, 2021 01 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32871228

RESUMO

Accurate discrimination between safe and dangerous stimuli is essential for survival. Prior research has begun to uncover the neural structures that are necessary for learning this discrimination, but exploration of brain regions involved in this learning process has been mostly limited to males. Recent findings show sex differences in discrimination learning, with reduced fear expression to safe cues in females compared to males. Here, we used male and female Sprague Dawley rats to explore neural activation, as measured by Fos expression, in fear and safety learning related brain regions. Neural activation after fear discrimination (Discrimination) was compared between males and females, as well as with fear conditioned (Fear Only) and stimulus presented (Control) conditions. Correlations of discrimination ability and neural activation were also calculated. We uncovered a correlation between central amygdala (CeA) activation and discrimination abilities in males and females. Anterior medial bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BNST) was the only region where sex differences in Fos counts were observed in the Discrimination condition, and the only region where neural activation significantly differed between Fear Only and Discrimination conditions. Together, these findings indicate the importance of fear expression circuitry in mediating discrimination responses and generate important questions for future investigation.


Assuntos
Núcleo Central da Amígdala/fisiologia , Condicionamento Clássico/fisiologia , Aprendizagem por Discriminação/fisiologia , Medo/fisiologia , Núcleos Septais/fisiologia , Caracteres Sexuais , Animais , Comportamento Animal/fisiologia , Núcleo Central da Amígdala/metabolismo , Masculino , Proteínas Proto-Oncogênicas c-fos/metabolismo , Ratos , Ratos Sprague-Dawley , Núcleos Septais/metabolismo
16.
Cereb Cortex ; 31(4): 2169-2186, 2021 03 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33251536

RESUMO

In a changing environment, organisms need to decide when to select items that resemble previously rewarded stimuli and when it is best to switch to other stimulus types. Here, we used chemogenetic techniques to provide causal evidence that activity in the rodent anterior cingulate cortex and its efferents to the anterior thalamic nuclei modulate the ability to attend to reliable predictors of important outcomes. Rats completed an attentional set-shifting paradigm that first measures the ability to master serial discriminations involving a constant stimulus dimension that reliably predicts reinforcement (intradimensional-shift), followed by the ability to shift attention to a previously irrelevant class of stimuli when reinforcement contingencies change (extradimensional-shift). Chemogenetic disruption of the anterior cingulate cortex (Experiment 1) as well as selective disruption of anterior cingulate efferents to the anterior thalamic nuclei (Experiment 2) impaired intradimensional learning but facilitated 2 sets of extradimensional-shifts. This pattern of results signals the loss of a corticothalamic system for cognitive control that preferentially processes stimuli resembling those previously associated with reward. Previous studies highlight a separate medial prefrontal system that promotes the converse pattern, that is, switching to hitherto inconsistent predictors of reward when contingencies change. Competition between these 2 systems regulates cognitive flexibility and choice.


Assuntos
Núcleos Anteriores do Tálamo/metabolismo , Atenção/fisiologia , Giro do Cíngulo/metabolismo , Optogenética/métodos , Recompensa , Adenoviridae/metabolismo , Animais , Núcleos Anteriores do Tálamo/química , Núcleos Anteriores do Tálamo/efeitos dos fármacos , Atenção/efeitos dos fármacos , Aprendizagem por Discriminação/efeitos dos fármacos , Aprendizagem por Discriminação/fisiologia , Giro do Cíngulo/química , Giro do Cíngulo/efeitos dos fármacos , Injeções Intraventriculares , Masculino , Vias Neurais/química , Vias Neurais/efeitos dos fármacos , Vias Neurais/metabolismo , Piperazinas/administração & dosagem , Piperazinas/análise , Piperazinas/metabolismo , Ratos
17.
Neurobiol Learn Mem ; 177: 107344, 2021 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33242588

RESUMO

In different vertebrate species, hippocampus plays a crucial role for spatial orientation. However, even though cognitive lateralization is widespread in the animal kingdom, the lateralization of this hippocampal function has been poorly studied. The aim of the present study was to investigate the lateralization of hippocampal activation in domestic chicks, during spatial navigation in relation to free-standing objects. Two groups of chicks were trained to find food in one of the feeders located in a large circular arena. Chicks of one group solved the task using the relational spatial information provided by free-standing objects present in the arena, while the other group used the local appearance of the baited feeder as a beacon. The immediate early gene product c-Fos was employed to map neural activation of hippocampus and medial striatum of both hemispheres. Chicks that used spatial cues for navigation showed higher activation of the right hippocampus compared to chicks that oriented by local features and compared to the left hippocampus. Such differences between the two groups were not present in the left hippocampus or in the medial striatum. Relational spatial information seems thus to be selectively processed by the right hippocampus in domestic chicks. The results are discussed in light of existing evidence of hippocampal lateralization of spatial processing in chicks, with particular attention to the contrasting evidence found in pigeons.


Assuntos
Hipocampo/fisiologia , Navegação Espacial/fisiologia , Animais , Animais Recém-Nascidos/fisiologia , Encéfalo/anatomia & histologia , Encéfalo/metabolismo , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Galinhas , Sinais (Psicologia) , Aprendizagem por Discriminação/fisiologia , Lateralidade Funcional , Hipocampo/anatomia & histologia , Hipocampo/metabolismo , Masculino , Orientação Espacial/fisiologia , Proteínas Proto-Oncogênicas c-fos/metabolismo
18.
Mol Brain ; 13(1): 170, 2020 12 14.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33317605

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Immune molecules, such as cytokines, complement, and major histocompatibility complex (MHC) proteins, in the central nervous system are often associated with neuropsychiatric disorders. Neuronal MHC class I (MHCI), such as H-2D, regulate neurite outgrowth, the establishment and function of cortical connections, and activity-dependent refinement in mice. We previously established mice expressing MHCI specifically in astrocytes of the media prefrontal cortex (mPFC) using the adeno-associated virus (AAV) vector under the control of the GfaABC1D promoter. Mice expressing the soluble form of H-2D (sH-2D) in the mPFC (sH-2D-expressing mice) showed abnormal behaviors, including social interaction deficits and cognitive dysfunctions. However, the pathophysiological significance of astroglial MHCI on higher brain functions, such as learning, memory, and behavioral flexibility, remains unclear. Therefore, cognitive function in mice expressing sH-2D in astrocytes of the mPFC was tested using the visual discrimination (VD) task. METHODS: sH-2D-expressing mice were subjected to the VD and reversal learning tasks, and morphological analysis. RESULTS: In the pretraining, sH-2D-expressing mice required significantly more trials to reach the learning criterion than control mice. The total number of sessions, trials, normal trials, and correction trials to reach the VD criterion were also significantly higher in sH-2D-expressing mice than in control mice. A morphological study showed that dendritic complexity and spine density were significantly reduced in the dorsal striatum of sH-2D-expressing mice. CONCLUSION: Collectively, the present results suggest that the overexpression of astroglial MHCI in the mPFC results in impaired VD learning, which may be accompanied by decreased dendritic complexity in the dorsal striatum and mPFC.


Assuntos
Astrócitos/metabolismo , Aprendizagem por Discriminação , Discriminação Psicológica , Complexo Principal de Histocompatibilidade , Córtex Pré-Frontal/metabolismo , Percepção Visual , Animais , Corpo Estriado/citologia , Espinhas Dendríticas/metabolismo , Masculino , Camundongos Endogâmicos C57BL , Neurônios/metabolismo , Reversão de Aprendizagem , Solubilidade , Análise e Desempenho de Tarefas
19.
Pharmacol Biochem Behav ; 199: 173045, 2020 12.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33058788

RESUMO

Nicotine and varenicline (Chantix®; the leading non-nicotine cessation pharmacotherapy) can come to control appetitive behaviors such as goal-tracking. We tested rats (N = 48) in a drug-discriminated goal-tracking (DGT) task where each rat received daily subcutaneous injections of either nicotine (0.4 mg/kg) or saline (0.9% [w/v]) interspersed across the acquisition phase (Phase 1). On saline days, sucrose was intermittently available. On nicotine days, sucrose was withheld. All rats acquired the discrimination with increased goal-tracking rates on saline days relative to nicotine days. Following acquisition, rats were separated into four groups to assess drug-substitution and discrimination reversal in Phase 2. The first group maintained the stimulus-reinforcer relation from acquisition (NIC-). The reversal group was now given access to sucrose on nicotine days (NIC+). The substitution group replaced nicotine with varenicline (1 mg/kg) while maintaining the acquisition stimulus-reinforcer relation (VAR-). The substitution and reversal group had nicotine replaced by varenicline and the stimulus-reinforcer relation reversed (VAR+). Rats in all groups learned or maintained their Phase 1 discriminations. For Phase 2, the reversal groups (+ conditions) acquired their discriminations within 10 sessions. The VAR- group displayed a pattern of disrupted discrimination at the outset of Phase 2 but was reestablished after continued training. In substitution testing, VAR groups received nicotine and NIC groups received varenicline. The NIC- and VAR- groups displayed full substitution of the test stimulus whereas the NIC+ and VAR+ groups displayed partial substitution of the test stimulus. Rats underwent nicotine extinction in Phase 3. Initial responding for each group mimicked Phase 2 training (i.e., higher responding by the reversal groups). All rats maintained similarly low levels of responding after six sessions. In conclusion, initial learning history with nicotine (i.e., + or -) influenced drug-stimulus substitution and the rate at which new learning (e.g., reversal) occurs with the varenicline and nicotine interoceptive stimuli.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem por Discriminação , Substituição de Medicamentos , Objetivos , Nicotina/farmacologia , Agonistas Nicotínicos/farmacologia , Agentes de Cessação do Hábito de Fumar/farmacologia , Vareniclina/farmacologia , Animais , Masculino , Ratos , Ratos Sprague-Dawley
20.
Vet Rec ; 186(18): 586-587, 2020 06 13.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32527883

RESUMO

New research shows mice can detect differences between the urine of healthy mice and those with melanoma. Emma Culjat-Vukman reports.


Assuntos
Melanoma/veterinária , Odorantes , Doenças dos Roedores/diagnóstico , Olfato , Urina , Animais , Aprendizagem por Discriminação , Masculino , Melanoma/diagnóstico , Camundongos
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA