Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 7 de 7
Filtrar
Mais filtros

Bases de dados
País/Região como assunto
Tipo de documento
País de afiliação
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Pediatrics ; 147(2)2021 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33431588

RESUMO

OBJECTIVES: To examine the contribution of early life factors and preschool- and school-aged language abilities to children's 11-year language and academic outcomes. METHODS: Participants (N = 839) were from a prospective community cohort study of 1910 infants recruited at 8 to 10 months of age. Early life factors included a combination of child (prematurity, birth weight), family (socioeconomic disadvantage, family history of language difficulties), and maternal factors (education, vocabulary, and age). Language (standardized assessment of receptive and expressive skills) and academic (national assessment) outcomes at 11 years were predicted by using a series of multivariable regression models. RESULTS: Early life factors explained 11% to 12% of variance in language scores at 11 years. The variance explained increased to 47% to 64% when language scores from 2 to 7 years were included. The largest increase in variance explained was with 4-year language scores. The same early life factors explained 13% to 14% of academic scores at 11 years, with increases to 43% to 54% when language scores from 2 to 11 years were included. Early life factors adequately discriminated between children with typical and low language scores but were much better discriminators of children with typical and low academic scores. When earlier language scores were added to models then the area under the curve increased to 0.9 and above. CONCLUSIONS: Children's language outcomes at 11 years are accurately predicted by their 4-year language ability and their academic outcomes at 11 years are predicted by early family and home environment factors. Children with low language abilities at 11 years consistently performed more poorly on national assessments of literacy and numeracy.


Assuntos
Sucesso Acadêmico , Linguagem Infantil , Escolaridade , Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Criança , Estudos de Coortes , Feminino , Previsões , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino , Relações Pais-Filho , Estudos Prospectivos , Fatores de Risco , Fatores Socioeconômicos , Vitória/epidemiologia
2.
J Anxiety Disord ; 70: 102191, 2020 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32014832

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: A translational trial evaluated the effectiveness of screening for inhibited childhood temperament, followed by a preventive parenting program -Cool Little Kids. This study determined the cost-effectiveness from societal and health sector perspectives using trial data. METHOD: Resources to deliver the screening and parenting sessions were determined from study records. Parents completed a questionnaire reporting resources used at one-year follow-up. Standard Australian unit costs were applied. Clinical outcomes for children and parental quality adjusted life-years (QALYs) were used to calculate incremental cost-effectiveness ratios (ICERs). RESULTS: Total societal costs were lower, but non-significant for the intervention compared to the control group (mean difference -$500 p = 0.937). Total health sector costs were significantly greater (mean difference $1,956; p = 0.015). The intervention led to significantly fewer internalising symptoms (Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ)-emotional difficulties adjusted mean difference -0.5; p = 0.006), fewer children with SDQ-emotional symptoms in the abnormal range (24.2 % vs. 33.0 % p = .014) and fewer with diagnosed anxiety (44.2 % vs. 50.2 % p = 0.427). From the societal perspective, the intervention would likely be cost-effective. Health sector ICERs were $1,171/SDQ-emotional symptom decrease, $51/abnormal SDQ avoided and $77/anxiety case avoided. CONCLUSIONS: This economic analysis alongside an implementation study provides an early indication that Cool Little Kids may be cost-effective.


Assuntos
Ansiedade/prevenção & controle , Análise Custo-Benefício , Depressão/prevenção & controle , Inibição Psicológica , Serviços de Saúde Mental Escolar/economia , Instituições Acadêmicas , Estudantes/psicologia , Pesquisa Translacional Biomédica , Ansiedade/diagnóstico , Ansiedade/psicologia , Austrália , Pré-Escolar , Mecanismos de Defesa , Depressão/diagnóstico , Depressão/psicologia , Feminino , Custos de Cuidados de Saúde , Humanos , Masculino , Poder Familiar/psicologia , Pais/psicologia , Anos de Vida Ajustados por Qualidade de Vida , Inquéritos e Questionários , Temperamento
4.
Int J Speech Lang Pathol ; 19(4): 360-369, 2017 08.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27467452

RESUMO

PURPOSE: To examine (1) the patterns of service use and costs associated with language impairment in a community cohort of children from ages 4-9 years and (2) the relationship between language impairment and health service utilisation. METHOD: Participants were children and caregivers of six local government areas in Melbourne participating in the community-based Early Language in Victoria Study (ELVS). Health service use was reported by parents. Costs were valued in Australian dollars in 2014, from the government and family perspectives. Depending on age, the Australian adapted Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals - Pre-school, 2nd Edition (CELF-P2) or the CELF, 4th Edition (CELF4) was used to assess expressive and receptive language. RESULT: At 5, 7 and 9 years respectively 21%, 11% and 8% of families reported using services for speech and/or language concerns. The annual costs associated with using services averaged A$612 (A$255 to government, A$357 to family) at 5 years and A$992 (A$317 to government, A$675 to family) at 7 years. Children with persistent language impairment had significantly higher service costs than those with typical language. CONCLUSION: Language impairment in 4-9-year-old children is associated with higher use of services and costs to both families and government compared to typical language.


Assuntos
Custos de Cuidados de Saúde/estatística & dados numéricos , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/economia , Aceitação pelo Paciente de Cuidados de Saúde/estatística & dados numéricos , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Vitória
5.
PLoS One ; 10(8): e0134251, 2015.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26241892

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Evidence is required as to when and where to focus resources to achieve the greatest gains for children's language development. Key to these decisions is the understanding of individual differences in children's language trajectories and the predictors of those differences. To determine optimal timing we must understand if and when children's relative language abilities become fixed. To determine where to focus effort we must identify mutable factors, that is those with the potential to be changed through interventions, which are associated with significant differences in children's language scores and rate of progress. METHODS: Uniquely this study examined individual differences in language growth trajectories in a population sample of children between 4 and 7 years using the multilevel model for change. The influence of predictors, grouped with respect to their mutability and their proximity to the child (least-mutable, mutable-distal, mutable-proximal), were estimated. RESULTS: A significant degree of variability in rate of progress between 4 and 7 years was evident, much of which was systematically associated with mutable-proximal factors, that is, those factors with evidence that they are modifiable through interventions with the child or family, such as shared book reading, TV viewing and number of books in the home. Mutable-distal factors, such as family income, family literacy and neighbourhood disadvantage, hypothesised to be modifiable through social policy, were important predictors of language abilities at 4 years. CONCLUSIONS: Potential levers for language interventions lie in the child's home learning environment from birth to age 4. However, the role of a family's material and cultural capital must not be ignored, nor should the potential for growth into the school years. Early Years services should acknowledge the effects of multiple, cascading and cumulative risks and seek to promote child language development through the aggregation of marginal gains in the pre-school years and beyond.


Assuntos
Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Modelos Psicológicos , Psicologia da Criança , Peso ao Nascer , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Humanos , Renda , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/epidemiologia , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/etiologia , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/prevenção & controle , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/psicologia , Alfabetização , Política Pública , Características de Residência , Fatores de Risco , Fatores Socioeconômicos , Populações Vulneráveis
6.
J Child Lang ; 37(5): 1109-21, 2010 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19874641

RESUMO

This study examines potential predictors of 'precocious talking' (expressive language ≥90th percentile) at one and two years of age, and of 'stability' in precocious talking across both time periods, drawing on data from a prospective community cohort comprising over 1,800 children. Logistic regression was used to examine the relationship between precocious talking and the following potential predictors: gender, birth order, birth weight, non-English speaking background, socioeconomic status, maternal age, maternal mental health scores, and vocabulary and educational attainment of parents. The strongest predictors of precocity (being female and having a younger mother) warrant further exploration. Overall, however, it appears that precocity in early vocabulary development is not strongly influenced by the variables examined, which together explained just 2.6% and 1% of the variation at 1;0 and 2;0 respectively.


Assuntos
Criança Superdotada , Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Adolescente , Adulto , Ordem de Nascimento/psicologia , Peso ao Nascer , Criança Superdotada/psicologia , Escolaridade , Feminino , Nível de Saúde , Humanos , Lactente , Testes de Linguagem , Modelos Logísticos , Masculino , Idade Materna , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Pais/psicologia , Estudos Prospectivos , Fatores Sexuais , Fatores Socioeconômicos , Vitória , Adulto Jovem
7.
J Paediatr Child Health ; 42(12): 764-70, 2006 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17096710

RESUMO

AIM: To describe changes in infant prelinguistic communication skills between 8 and 12 months, and identify factors associated with those skills. DESIGN: Parent questionnaire data for a prospective population-based cohort of infants in metropolitan Melbourne, Australia. PARTICIPANTS: 1911 infants born November 2002-August 2003. OUTCOME: Infant communication (Communication and Symbolic Behaviour Scales (CSBS)) at 8 and 12 months. Potential risk factors: sex; prematurity; multiple birth; sociodemographic indicators; maternal mental health, vocabulary and education; non-English speaking background; and family history of speech-language difficulties. Linear regression models were fitted to total standardised CSBS scores at 8 and 12 months. RESULTS: Social communication, especially the use of gesture, developed rapidly from 8 to 12 months. Female sex, twin birth, and family history were strongly associated with CSBS scores. The combined model accounted for 5% and 6% of the total variation at 8 and 12 months, respectively. CSBS score at 8 months strongly predicted CSBS score at 12 months (coefficient = 0.56, partial R(2) = 37.0). CONCLUSIONS: There is a dramatic increase in communication skills between 8 and 12 months, particularly the development of gesture, which (as in previous studies) predates and predicts future language development. Risk factors explained little variation in early communication trajectories and therefore, based on our findings, this developmental course is more likely to be biologically predetermined. Rather than focusing on risk factors, we suggest that language promotion activities in otherwise healthy young infants should either be universal or, if targeted, be based on the level of communication skills displayed.


Assuntos
Comunicação não Verbal/fisiologia , Austrália , Linguagem Infantil , Estudos de Coortes , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino , Saúde Mental , Mães/psicologia , Comunicação não Verbal/psicologia , Estudos Prospectivos , Análise de Regressão , Fatores de Risco , Fatores Socioeconômicos , Gêmeos
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA