Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 16 de 16
Filtrar
Mais filtros

Bases de dados
País/Região como assunto
Tipo de documento
País de afiliação
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Health Psychol ; 43(10): 718-729, 2024 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38884977

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: We aim to identify vaccination invitations that foster trust and improve vaccination uptake overall, especially among ethnic minority groups who are more at risk from coronavirus disease (COVID-19) and less likely to be vaccinated. METHOD: In a preregistered 4 × 4 mixed-design experiment, we manipulated how much risk-benefit information the message included within-subjects and the message source between-subjects (N = 4,038 U.K. and U.S. participants, 50% ethnic minority). Participants read four vaccine invitations that varied in vaccination risk-benefit information (randomized order): control (no information), benefits only, risk and benefit, and risk and benefit that mentions vulnerable groups. The messages were sent by one of four sources (random allocation): control (health institution), medical professional (unnamed), warm and competent medical professional (unnamed), and named warm and competent medical professional (Sanjay/Lamar). Participants assessed how much they trusted the message and how likely they would be to book their vaccination appointment. RESULTS: Information about vaccination benefits and risks increased trust, especially among ethnic minority groups-for whom the effect replicated within each group. Trust also increased when the message was sent by a warm and competent medical professional relative to a health institution, but the importance of the source mattered less when more information was shared. CONCLUSIONS: Our research demonstrates the positive impact of outlining the benefits and disclosing the risks of COVID vaccines in vaccination invitation messages. Having a warm and competent medical professional source can also increase trust, especially where the message is limited in scope. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).


Assuntos
COVID-19 , Confiança , Vacinação , Humanos , Feminino , Masculino , Adulto , COVID-19/prevenção & controle , COVID-19/etnologia , Vacinação/psicologia , Intenção , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Adulto Jovem , Estados Unidos , Medição de Risco , Vacinas contra COVID-19/administração & dosagem , Reino Unido , Minorias Étnicas e Raciais , Etnicidade , Adolescente , SARS-CoV-2
2.
Appetite ; 198: 107323, 2024 07 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38556057

RESUMO

The COVID-19 pandemic brought increases in food insecurity in Los Angeles (L.A.) County, defined as lacking household access to adequate food because of limited money or other resources. Here, we aimed to understand the lived experiences of food insecurity during the COVID-19 pandemic and its aftermath. In August-December 2022, we interviewed 30 residents of L.A. County who were participants in an ongoing internet panel and had reported experiencing food insecurity between April 2020 and July 2021. A stratified-sampling approach was used to recruit a diverse sample with and without government food assistance. We report five key findings, which underscore the stress and worry associated with the experience of food insecurity, and the coping strategies people implemented: (1) The pandemic prompted food insecurity as well as stressful shifts in eating behaviors compared to before the pandemic, with some eating much less food, some eating less nutritious food, and some eating much more due to being stuck at home; (2) Buying food became more effortful and financially challenging; (3) Government food assistance from the Supplemental Nutrition Assistance Program (SNAP) was important for reducing food insecurity, but was sometimes insufficient, inconsistent, and didn't cover all retailers or food items; (4) Interviewees had to rely on their social networks, food banks or pantries, churches, and schools to meet their food needs and cope with food insecurity, but some faced barriers in doing so; (5) For some, food insecurity was worse in late 2022, almost two years after the pandemic started. We conclude with implications for policymakers and practitioners, emphasizing the importance of meeting the needs of diverse residents and addressing food insecurity in the aftermath of the COVID-19 pandemic.


Assuntos
COVID-19 , Assistência Alimentar , Insegurança Alimentar , Pesquisa Qualitativa , Humanos , COVID-19/epidemiologia , COVID-19/psicologia , Feminino , Masculino , Adulto , Los Angeles/epidemiologia , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Pandemias , SARS-CoV-2 , Abastecimento de Alimentos , Adaptação Psicológica , Comportamento Alimentar/psicologia , Idoso , Adulto Jovem , Estresse Psicológico/psicologia
3.
Public Health Nutr ; 26(10): 1944-1955, 2023 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37403467

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: The COVID-19 pandemic increased food insufficiency: a severe form of food insecurity. Drawing on an ecological framework, we aimed to understand factors that contributed to changes in food insufficiency from April to December 2020, in a large urban population hard hit by the pandemic. DESIGN: We conducted internet surveys every 2 weeks in April-December 2020, including a subset of items from the Food Insecurity Experience Scale. Longitudinal analysis identified predictors of food insufficiency, using fixed effects models. SETTING: Los Angeles County, which has a diverse population of 10 million residents. PARTICIPANTS: A representative sample of 1535 adults in Los Angeles County who are participants in the Understanding Coronavirus in America tracking survey. RESULTS: Rates of food insufficiency spiked in the first year of the pandemic, especially among participants living in poverty, in middle adulthood and with larger households. Government food assistance from the Supplemental Nutrition Assistance Program was significantly associated with reduced food insufficiency over time, while other forms of assistance such as help from family and friends or stimulus funds were not. CONCLUSIONS: The findings highlight that during a crisis, there is value in rapidly monitoring food insufficiency and investing in government food benefits.


Assuntos
COVID-19 , Assistência Alimentar , Adulto , Humanos , Pandemias , Los Angeles/epidemiologia , Fatores de Proteção , COVID-19/epidemiologia
4.
PLoS One ; 16(11): e0260378, 2021.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34807946

RESUMO

Numeracy refers to the ability to use numbers, including converting percentages (e.g., 10%) into absolute frequencies (e.g., 1 in 10). Studies have suggested that numeracy is correlated to financial outcomes, suggesting its relevance to financial decisions. However, almost all research on numeracy has been conducted in high-income countries in Europe and North America. Our analyses suggest that low numeracy is much more common in low-income countries, thus potentially threatening the financial well-being of the world's poorest. We analyzed data from the Lloyd's Register Foundation World Risk Poll, which assessed basic numeracy in 141 countries, including 21 low-income, 34 lower middle income, 43 upper middle income, and 43 high-income countries. Numeracy was associated with being among the poorest 20% of one's country, and with difficulty living on one's income, even after accounting for income, education, and demographics. These findings underscore the importance of worldwide numeracy education.


Assuntos
Escolaridade , Pobreza , Países Desenvolvidos , Países em Desenvolvimento , Humanos , Renda
5.
PLoS One ; 15(11): e0241895, 2020.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33175894

RESUMO

Despite the profound health and economic implications of Covid-19, there is only limited knowledge to date about the role of economic concerns, health worries and social distancing for mental health outcomes during the pandemic. We analyze online survey data from the nationally representative "Understanding America Study" (UAS) covering the period of March 10-31st 2020 (sample size: 6,585). Mental health is assessed by the validated PHQ-4 instrument for measuring symptoms of depression and anxiety. About 29% (CI:27.4-.30.4%) of the US adult population reported some depression/anxiety symptoms over the study period, with symptoms deteriorating over the month of March. Worsening mental health was most strongly associated with concerns about the economic consequences of the pandemic, while concerns about the potential implications of the virus for respondents' own health and social distancing also predicted increases in symptoms of depression and anxiety during the early stages of the pandemic in the US, albeit less strongly. Our findings point towards the possibility of a major mental health crisis unfolding simultaneously with the pandemic, with economic concerns being a key driving force of this crisis. These results highlight the likely importance of economic countermeasures and social policy for mitigating the impact of Covid-19 on adult mental health in the US over and above an effective public health response.


Assuntos
Infecções por Coronavirus/patologia , Status Econômico , Saúde Mental , Pneumonia Viral/patologia , Adulto , Ansiedade/diagnóstico , Ansiedade/epidemiologia , Betacoronavirus/isolamento & purificação , COVID-19 , Infecções por Coronavirus/epidemiologia , Infecções por Coronavirus/virologia , Depressão/diagnóstico , Depressão/epidemiologia , Feminino , Comportamentos Relacionados com a Saúde , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Pandemias , Pneumonia Viral/epidemiologia , Pneumonia Viral/virologia , Risco , SARS-CoV-2 , Inquéritos e Questionários , Estados Unidos/epidemiologia
6.
J Exp Psychol Appl ; 25(1): 77-87, 2019 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30475023

RESUMO

To help consumers make informed decisions, regulators often impose disclosure requirements on financial institutions. However, disclosures may not be informative for consumers if they contain difficult-to-evaluate attributes, such as annual percentage rates (APRs). To improve a consumer's ability to evaluate the relative attractiveness of products with difficult-to-evaluate attributes, evaluability theory suggests providing consumers with distributional information. Here, we tested whether credit card disclosures containing graphs of the distribution of APRs in the credit card market help consumers estimate the relative costs of credit and evaluate credit cards. In two studies, we found that consumers using standard credit card disclosures (without distributional information) underestimated the costs of credit card APRs relative to the market. We then built on the graph design literature to design different graphs for presenting distributional APR information. A comparison of the graphs we designed showed that a histogram was most successful at improving consumers' estimates of APR costs relative to the market and modifying consumers' evaluations of an expensive credit card. We discuss the implications of our findings for evaluability theory, graph design, and communication efforts that aim to provide consumers with meaningful financial disclosures. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).


Assuntos
Comportamento do Consumidor/economia , Tomada de Decisões , Revelação/legislação & jurisprudência , Adulto , Feminino , Administração Financeira , Humanos , Masculino
7.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 114(13): 3297-3304, 2017 03 28.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28270610

RESUMO

We describe two collaborations in which psychologists and economists provided essential support on foundational projects in major research programs. One project involved eliciting adolescents' expectations regarding significant future life events affecting their psychological and economic development. The second project involved eliciting consumers' expectations regarding inflation, a potentially vital input to their investment, saving, and purchasing decisions. In each project, we sought questions with the precision needed for economic modeling and the simplicity needed for lay respondents. We identify four conditions that, we believe, promoted our ability to sustain these transdisciplinary collaborations and coproduce the research: (i) having a shared research goal, which neither discipline could achieve on its own; (ii) finding common ground in shared methodology, which met each discipline's essential evidentiary conditions, but without insisting on its culturally acquired tastes; (iii) sharing the effort throughout, with common language and sense of ownership; and (iv) gaining mutual benefit from both the research process and its products.


Assuntos
Economia , Pesquisa Interdisciplinar , Psicologia , Adolescente , Comportamento Cooperativo , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Estatísticos , Recursos Humanos
8.
Sci Eng Ethics ; 23(5): 1369-1386, 2017 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27752964

RESUMO

As the specter of climate change looms on the horizon, people will face complex decisions about whether to support climate change policies and how to cope with climate change impacts on their lives. Without some grasp of the relevant science, they may find it hard to make informed decisions. Climate experts therefore face the ethical need to effectively communicate to non-expert audiences. Unfortunately, climate experts may inadvertently violate the maxims of effective communication, which require sharing communications that are truthful, brief, relevant, clear, and tested for effectiveness. Here, we discuss the 'mental models' approach towards developing communications, which aims to help experts to meet the maxims of effective communications, and to better inform the judgments and decisions of non-expert audiences.


Assuntos
Acesso à Informação , Mudança Climática , Clima , Comunicação , Conhecimentos, Atitudes e Prática em Saúde , Opinião Pública , Pesquisadores , Tomada de Decisões , Humanos , Julgamento , Modelos Psicológicos , Política Pública , Pesquisa
9.
Psychol Aging ; 31(7): 724-736, 2016 11.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27831712

RESUMO

We tested interventions to reduce "sunk-cost bias," the tendency to continue investing in failing plans even when those plans have soured and are no longer rewarding. We showed members of a national U.S. life-span panel a hypothetical scenario about a failing plan that was halfway complete. Participants were randomly assigned to an intervention to focus on how to improve the situation, an intervention to focus on thoughts and feelings, or a no-intervention control group. First, we found that the thoughts and feelings intervention reduced sunk-cost bias in decisions about project completion, as compared to the improvement intervention and the no-intervention control. Second, older age was associated with greater willingness to cancel the failing plan across all 3 groups. Third, we found that introspection processes helped to explain the effectiveness of the interventions. Specifically, the larger reduction in sunk-cost bias as observed in the thoughts and feelings intervention (vs. the improvement intervention) was associated with suppression of future-oriented thoughts of eventual success, and with suppression of augmentations of the scenario that could make it seem reasonable to continue the plan. Fourth, we found that introspection processes were related to age differences in decisions. Older people were less likely to mention future-oriented thoughts of eventual success associated with greater willingness to continue the failing plan. We discuss factors to consider when designing interventions for reducing sunk-cost bias. (PsycINFO Database Record


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/psicologia , Tomada de Decisões , Adolescente , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Adulto Jovem
10.
J Exp Psychol Appl ; 21(2): 158-66, 2015 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25581089

RESUMO

Many consumers have monetary or environmental motivations for saving energy. Indeed, saving energy produces both monetary benefits, by reducing energy bills, and environmental benefits, by reducing carbon footprints. We examined how consumers' willingness and reasons to enroll in energy-savings programs are affected by whether advertisements emphasize monetary benefits, environmental benefits, or both. From a normative perspective, having 2 noteworthy kinds of benefit should not decrease a program's attractiveness. In contrast, psychological research suggests that adding external incentives to an intrinsically motivating task may backfire. To date, however, it remains unclear whether this is the case when both extrinsic and intrinsic motivations are inherent to the task, as with energy savings, and whether removing explicit mention of extrinsic motivation will reduce its importance. We found that emphasizing a program's monetary benefits reduced participants' willingness to enroll. In addition, participants' explanations about enrollment revealed less attention to environmental concerns when programs emphasized monetary savings, even when environmental savings were also emphasized. We found equal attention to monetary motivations in all conditions, revealing an asymmetric attention to monetary and environmental motives. These results also provide practical guidance regarding the positioning of energy-saving programs: emphasize intrinsic benefits; the extrinsic ones may speak for themselves.


Assuntos
Publicidade/métodos , Conservação de Recursos Energéticos/economia , Análise Custo-Benefício , Tomada de Decisões , Meio Ambiente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Motivação , Adulto Jovem
11.
Med Decis Making ; 35(1): 22-6, 2015 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25035261

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Numeracy refers to people's ability to use numbers. Low numeracy has been associated with difficulties in understanding risk-benefit information and making health decisions. Older adults tend to perform worse than younger adults on measures of numeracy, but some theories of aging suggest that older adults may lack motivation for such tasks. We therefore test whether age differences in numeracy performance are mediated by a reduced motivation to think hard about complex problems-as measured by need for cognition. METHOD: We recruited an age-diverse convenience sample of 306 UK adults. They completed measures of numeracy and need for cognition. They self-reported their educational attainment and other demographics. RESULTS: Older age was related to lower numeracy and lower need for cognition. The negative relationship between age and numeracy was mediated by need for cognition. These findings held after we controlled for educational attainment and other demographic factors. DISCUSSION: Older adults may show lower numeracy performance due to lack of motivation. We discuss strategies for improving people's motivation to complete numeracy measures and other numerical tasks.


Assuntos
Cognição , Matemática , Motivação , Adolescente , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Tomada de Decisões , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Fatores Socioeconômicos , Reino Unido , Adulto Jovem
12.
Psychol Aging ; 29(3): 642-7, 2014 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25244483

RESUMO

Because people of all ages face decisions that affect their quality of life, decision-making competence is important across the life span. According to theories of rational decision making, one crucial decision skill involves the ability to discontinue failing commitments despite irrecoverable investments also referred to as "sunk costs." We find that older adults are better than younger adults at making decisions to discontinue such failing commitments especially when irrecoverable losses are large, as well as at coping with the associated irrecoverable losses. Our results are relevant to interventions that aim to promote better decision-making competence across the life span.


Assuntos
Adaptação Psicológica , Envelhecimento/psicologia , Tomada de Decisões , Investimentos em Saúde , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Adulto Jovem
13.
J Exp Psychol Appl ; 20(2): 126-35, 2014 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24708355

RESUMO

Emerging technologies promise potential benefits at a potential cost. Developers of educational communications aim to improve people's understanding and to facilitate public debate. However, even relatively uninformed recipients may have initial feelings that are difficult to change. We report that people's initial affective impressions about carbon capture and sequestration (CCS), a low-carbon coal-based electricity-generation technology with which most people are unfamiliar, influences how they interpret previously validated education materials. As a result, even individuals who had originally self-identified as uninformed persisted in their initial feelings after reading the educational communication-though perseverance of feelings about CCS was stronger among recipients who had originally self-identified as relatively informed (Study 1). Moreover, uninformed recipients whose initial feelings were experimentally manipulated by relatively uninformative pro-CCS or anti-CCS arguments persisted in their manipulated feelings after reading the educational communication, due to evaluating the educational communication in line with their manipulated impressions (Study 2). Hence, our results suggest that educational communications will have more impact if they are disseminated before people form strong feelings about the topic under consideration, especially if these are based on little to no factual understanding.


Assuntos
Atitude , Sequestro de Carbono , Comunicação , Educação , Disseminação de Informação/métodos , Opinião Pública , Adulto , Afeto , Análise Custo-Benefício , Eletricidade , Emoções , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
14.
Med Decis Making ; 32(2): 232-6, 2012.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21521797

RESUMO

OBJECTIVES: Risk perceptions are central to good health decisions. People can judge valid probabilities but use 50% disproportionately. The authors hypothesized that 50% is more likely than other responses to reflect not knowing the probability, especially among individuals with low education and numeracy, and evaluated the usefulness of eliciting "don't know" explanations. METHODS: Respondents (n = 1020) judged probabilities for living or dying in the next 10 years, indicating whether they gave a good estimate or did not know the chances. They completed demographics, medical history, and numeracy questions. RESULTS: Overall, 50% was more likely than other probabilities to be explained as "don't know" (v. "a good estimate"). Correlations of using 50% with low education and numeracy were mediated by expressing "don't know." Judged probabilities for survival and mortality explained as "don't know" had lower correlations with age, diseases, and specialist visits. CONCLUSIONS: When judging risks, 50% may reflect not knowing the probability, especially among individuals with low numeracy and education. Probabilities expressed as "don't know" are less valid. Eliciting uncertainty could benefit theoretical models and educational efforts.


Assuntos
Indicadores Básicos de Saúde , Probabilidade , Medição de Risco/estatística & dados numéricos , Estatística como Assunto , Análise de Sobrevida , Adolescente , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Escolaridade , Feminino , Humanos , Julgamento , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Incerteza , Adulto Jovem
15.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 107(37): 16054-9, 2010 Sep 14.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20713724

RESUMO

In a national online survey, 505 participants reported their perceptions of energy consumption and savings for a variety of household, transportation, and recycling activities. When asked for the most effective strategy they could implement to conserve energy, most participants mentioned curtailment (e.g., turning off lights, driving less) rather than efficiency improvements (e.g., installing more efficient light bulbs and appliances), in contrast to experts' recommendations. For a sample of 15 activities, participants underestimated energy use and savings by a factor of 2.8 on average, with small overestimates for low-energy activities and large underestimates for high-energy activities. Additional estimation and ranking tasks also yielded relatively flat functions for perceived energy use and savings. Across several tasks, participants with higher numeracy scores and stronger proenvironmental attitudes had more accurate perceptions. The serious deficiencies highlighted by these results suggest that well-designed efforts to improve the public's understanding of energy use and savings could pay large dividends.


Assuntos
Conservação de Recursos Energéticos , Conservação de Recursos Energéticos/economia , Coleta de Dados , Hábitos , Estados Unidos
16.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 92(5): 938-956, 2007 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17484614

RESUMO

The authors evaluated the reliability and validity of a set of 7 behavioral decision-making tasks, measuring different aspects of the decision-making process. The tasks were administered to individuals from diverse populations. Participants showed relatively consistent performance within and across the 7 tasks, which were then aggregated into an Adult Decision-Making Competence (A-DMC) index that showed good reliability. The validity of the 7 tasks and of overall A-DMC emerges in significant relationships with measures of socioeconomic status, cognitive ability, and decision-making styles. Participants who performed better on the A-DMC were less likely to report negative life events indicative of poor decision making, as measured by the Decision Outcomes Inventory. Significant predictive validity remains when controlling for demographic measures, measures of cognitive ability, and constructive decision-making styles. Thus, A-DMC appears to be a distinct construct relevant to adults' real-world decisions.


Assuntos
Aptidão , Tomada de Decisões , Individualidade , Adaptação Psicológica , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Cultura , Feminino , Humanos , Inteligência , Controle Interno-Externo , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Inventário de Personalidade/estatística & dados numéricos , Resolução de Problemas , Psicometria/estatística & dados numéricos , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Autoimagem , Valores Sociais , Fatores Socioeconômicos
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA