Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 18 de 18
Filtrar
Mais filtros

Bases de dados
País/Região como assunto
Tipo de documento
País de afiliação
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
J Speech Lang Hear Res ; 67(5): 1385-1399, 2024 May 07.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38625147

RESUMO

PURPOSE: Stuttering is a speech condition that can have a major impact on a person's quality of life. This descriptive study aimed to identify subgroups of people who stutter (PWS) based on stuttering burden and to investigate differences between these subgroups on psychosocial aspects of life. METHOD: The study included 618 adult participants who stutter. They completed a detailed survey examining stuttering symptomatology, impact of stuttering on anxiety, education and employment, experience of stuttering, and levels of depression, anxiety, and stress. A two-step cluster analytic procedure was performed to identify subgroups of PWS, based on self-report of stuttering frequency, severity, affect, and anxiety, four measures that together inform about stuttering burden. RESULTS: We identified a high- (n = 230) and a low-burden subgroup (n = 372). The high-burden subgroup reported a significantly higher impact of stuttering on education and employment, and higher levels of general depression, anxiety, stress, and overall impact of stuttering. These participants also reported that they trialed more different stuttering therapies than those with lower burden. CONCLUSIONS: Our results emphasize the need to be attentive to the diverse experiences and needs of PWS, rather than treating them as a homogeneous group. Our findings also stress the importance of personalized therapeutic strategies for individuals with stuttering, considering all aspects that could influence their stuttering burden. People with high-burden stuttering might, for example, have a higher need for psychological therapy to reduce stuttering-related anxiety. People with less emotional reactions but severe speech distortions may also have a moderate to high burden, but they may have a higher need for speech techniques to communicate with more ease. Future research should give more insights into the therapeutic needs of people highly burdened by their stuttering. SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL: https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.25582980.


Assuntos
Ansiedade , Efeitos Psicossociais da Doença , Depressão , Qualidade de Vida , Gagueira , Humanos , Gagueira/psicologia , Feminino , Masculino , Adulto , Qualidade de Vida/psicologia , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Ansiedade/psicologia , Depressão/psicologia , Depressão/etiologia , Adulto Jovem , Estresse Psicológico/psicologia , Adolescente , Idoso , Emprego/psicologia , Inquéritos e Questionários , Autorrelato
2.
BMJ Open ; 13(8): e071004, 2023 08 16.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37586864

RESUMO

INTRODUCTION: Australian practices for diagnosing fetal alcohol spectrum disorder (FASD) are lengthy and require specialist expertise. Specialist teams are based in urban locations; they are expensive and have prolonged waitlists. Innovative, flexible solutions are needed to ensure First Nations children living in rural/remote communities have culturally appropriate and equitable access to timely diagnosis and support. This study compares the accuracy of rapid assessments (index tests) that can be administered by a range of primary healthcare practitioners to specialist standardised FASD assessments (reference tests). The cost-efficiency of index tests will be compared with reference tests. METHODS AND ANALYSIS: At least 200 children aged 6-16 years at-risk of FASD will be recruited across at least seven study sites. Following standards for reporting diagnostic accuracy study (STARD) guidelines, all children will complete index and reference tests. Diagnostic accuracy statistics (including receiver operating curves, sensitivity, specificity, positive and negative predictive values and likelihood ratios) will identify whether rapid assessments can accurately identify: (1) the presence of an FASD diagnosis and (2) impairment in each neurodevelopmental domain, compared to comprehensive assessments. Direct and indirect healthcare costs for index tests compared to reference tests will be collected in primary healthcare and specialist settings. ETHICS AND DISSEMINATION OF RESULTS: Children's Health Queensland Hospital and Health Service Human Research Ethics Committee (HREC/20/QCHQ/63173); Griffith University Human Research Ethics Committee (2020/743). Results will assist in validating the use of index tests as part of a tiered neurodevelopmental assessment process that was co-designed with First Nations community and primary healthcare practitioners. Outcomes will be summarised and provided to participating practitioners and sites, and disseminated to community health services and consumers. Findings will be presented at national and international conferences and published in peer-reviewed journals. TRIAL REGISTRATION NUMBER: ACTRN12622000498796.


Assuntos
Transtornos do Espectro Alcoólico Fetal , Criança , Feminino , Gravidez , Humanos , Transtornos do Espectro Alcoólico Fetal/diagnóstico , Austrália , Custos de Cuidados de Saúde , Saúde da Criança , Hospitais Pediátricos
3.
Pediatrics ; 147(2)2021 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33431588

RESUMO

OBJECTIVES: To examine the contribution of early life factors and preschool- and school-aged language abilities to children's 11-year language and academic outcomes. METHODS: Participants (N = 839) were from a prospective community cohort study of 1910 infants recruited at 8 to 10 months of age. Early life factors included a combination of child (prematurity, birth weight), family (socioeconomic disadvantage, family history of language difficulties), and maternal factors (education, vocabulary, and age). Language (standardized assessment of receptive and expressive skills) and academic (national assessment) outcomes at 11 years were predicted by using a series of multivariable regression models. RESULTS: Early life factors explained 11% to 12% of variance in language scores at 11 years. The variance explained increased to 47% to 64% when language scores from 2 to 7 years were included. The largest increase in variance explained was with 4-year language scores. The same early life factors explained 13% to 14% of academic scores at 11 years, with increases to 43% to 54% when language scores from 2 to 11 years were included. Early life factors adequately discriminated between children with typical and low language scores but were much better discriminators of children with typical and low academic scores. When earlier language scores were added to models then the area under the curve increased to 0.9 and above. CONCLUSIONS: Children's language outcomes at 11 years are accurately predicted by their 4-year language ability and their academic outcomes at 11 years are predicted by early family and home environment factors. Children with low language abilities at 11 years consistently performed more poorly on national assessments of literacy and numeracy.


Assuntos
Sucesso Acadêmico , Linguagem Infantil , Escolaridade , Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Criança , Estudos de Coortes , Feminino , Previsões , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino , Relações Pais-Filho , Estudos Prospectivos , Fatores de Risco , Fatores Socioeconômicos , Vitória/epidemiologia
4.
Value Health ; 23(2): 164-170, 2020 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32113621

RESUMO

OBJECTIVES: To examine health-related quality of life (HRQoL) in young children with low language or congenital hearing loss and to explore the value of assessing HRQoL by concurrently administering 2 HRQoL instruments in populations of children. METHODS: Data were from 2 Australian community-based studies: Language for Learning (children with typical and low language at age 4 years, n = 1012) and the Statewide Comparison of Outcomes study (children with hearing loss, n = 108). HRQoL was measured using the parent-reported Health Utilities Index Mark 3 (HUI3) and the Pediatrics Quality of Life Inventory 4.0 (PedsQL) generic core scale. Agreement between the HRQoL instruments was assessed using intraclass correlation and Bland-Altman plots. RESULTS: Children with low language and with hearing loss had lower HRQoL than children with normal language; the worst HRQoL was experienced by children with both. The lower HRQoL was mainly due to impaired school functioning (PedsQL) and speech and cognition (HUI3). Children with hearing loss also had impaired physical and social functioning (PedsQL), vision, hearing, dexterity, and ambulation (HUI3). Correlations between instruments were poor to moderate, with low agreement. CONCLUSIONS: Children with low language and congenital hearing loss might benefit from interventions targeting overall health and well-being, not just their impairments. The HUI3 and PedsQL each seemed to provide unique information and thus may supplement each other in assessing HRQoL of young children, including those with low language or congenital hearing loss.


Assuntos
Percepção Auditiva , Comportamento Infantil , Linguagem Infantil , Crianças com Deficiência/psicologia , Perda Auditiva/diagnóstico , Audição , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/diagnóstico , Pessoas com Deficiência Auditiva/psicologia , Qualidade de Vida , Inquéritos e Questionários , Fatores Etários , Austrália , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Pré-Escolar , Pesquisa Comparativa da Efetividade , Efeitos Psicossociais da Doença , Crianças com Deficiência/reabilitação , Educação de Pessoas com Deficiência Auditiva , Feminino , Perda Auditiva/congênito , Perda Auditiva/psicologia , Perda Auditiva/reabilitação , Humanos , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/psicologia , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/reabilitação , Inclusão Escolar , Masculino , Pessoas com Deficiência Auditiva/reabilitação , Valor Preditivo dos Testes , Ensaios Clínicos Controlados Aleatórios como Assunto , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes
5.
BMC Pediatr ; 20(1): 52, 2020 02 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32013944

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: There is evidence that parents from more socioeconomically disadvantaged backgrounds engage in fewer verbal interactions with their child than more advantaged parents. This leads to the so-called, '30 million-word gap'. This study aims to investigate the number of words children hear and the number of vocalizations children produce in their first year of life and examines whether these aspects of the early language home environment differ by maternal education. METHODS: Mothers were recruited into a five-year prospective cohort study and categorized into either high or low maternal education groups. Data was derived from the first two waves of the study, when the children were six and twelve months old. At both waves, children were involved in day-long audio recordings using the Language Environment Analysis software that provided automatic counts of adult words spoken to the child, child vocalizations and conversational turns. Descriptive results are presented by maternal education groups. RESULTS: There was large variation within each maternal education group, with the number of adult words spoken to the child ranging from 2958 to 39,583 at six months and 4389 to 45,849 at twelve months. There were no meaningful differences between adult words, child vocalizations or conversational turns across maternal education groups at either wave of data collection. CONCLUSIONS: These results show that a word gap related to maternal education is not apparent up to twelve months of age. The large variability among both maternal education groups suggests that universal interventions that encourage all parents to talk more to their child may be more appropriate than interventions targeted towards disadvantaged families during the first year of life.


Assuntos
Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Idioma , Adulto , Austrália , Criança , Audição , Humanos , Lactente , Estudos Prospectivos
7.
Int J Speech Lang Pathol ; 19(4): 360-369, 2017 08.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27467452

RESUMO

PURPOSE: To examine (1) the patterns of service use and costs associated with language impairment in a community cohort of children from ages 4-9 years and (2) the relationship between language impairment and health service utilisation. METHOD: Participants were children and caregivers of six local government areas in Melbourne participating in the community-based Early Language in Victoria Study (ELVS). Health service use was reported by parents. Costs were valued in Australian dollars in 2014, from the government and family perspectives. Depending on age, the Australian adapted Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals - Pre-school, 2nd Edition (CELF-P2) or the CELF, 4th Edition (CELF4) was used to assess expressive and receptive language. RESULT: At 5, 7 and 9 years respectively 21%, 11% and 8% of families reported using services for speech and/or language concerns. The annual costs associated with using services averaged A$612 (A$255 to government, A$357 to family) at 5 years and A$992 (A$317 to government, A$675 to family) at 7 years. Children with persistent language impairment had significantly higher service costs than those with typical language. CONCLUSION: Language impairment in 4-9-year-old children is associated with higher use of services and costs to both families and government compared to typical language.


Assuntos
Custos de Cuidados de Saúde/estatística & dados numéricos , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/economia , Aceitação pelo Paciente de Cuidados de Saúde/estatística & dados numéricos , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Vitória
8.
J Paediatr Child Health ; 52(12): 1057-1061, 2016 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27662112

RESUMO

An inability or difficulty communicating can have a profound impact on a child's future ability to participate in society as a productive adult. Over the past few years the number of interventions for children with speech and language problems has almost doubled; the majority are targeted interventions delivered by speech pathologists. In this paper we examine the distribution of speech pathology services in metropolitan Melbourne and how these are aligned with need as defined by vulnerability in language and social disadvantage. We identified three times as many private sector services compared to public services for the 0-5 year age group. Overall there was poorer availability of services in some of the most vulnerable areas. The profound and long-term impact of impoverished childhood language, coupled with the considerable limitations on public spending, provide a strong impetus to deliver more equitably distributed speech pathology services.


Assuntos
Necessidades e Demandas de Serviços de Saúde , Patologia da Fala e Linguagem , Populações Vulneráveis , Pré-Escolar , Humanos , Lactente
9.
Pediatrics ; 137(1)2016 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26704085

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Universal newborn hearing screening was implemented worldwide largely on modeled, not measured, long-term benefits. Comparative quantification of population benefits would justify its high cost. METHODS: Natural experiment comparing 3 population approaches to detecting bilateral congenital hearing loss (>25 dB, better ear) in Australian states with similar demographics and services: (1) universal newborn hearing screening, New South Wales 2003-2005, n = 69; (2) Risk factor screening (neonatal intensive care screening + universal risk factor referral), Victoria 2003-2005, n = 65; and (3) largely opportunistic detection, Victoria 1991-1993, n = 86. Children in (1) and (2) were followed at age 5 to 6 years and in (3) at 7 to 8 years. Outcomes were compared between states using adjusted linear regression. RESULTS: Children were diagnosed younger with universal than risk factor screening (adjusted mean difference -8.0 months, 95% confidence interval -12.3 to -3.7). For children without intellectual disability, moving from opportunistic to risk factor to universal screening incrementally improved age of diagnosis (22.5 vs 16.2 vs 8.1 months, P < .001), receptive (81.8 vs 83.0 vs 88.9, P = .05) and expressive (74.9 vs 80.7 vs 89.3, P < .001) language and receptive vocabulary (79.4 vs 83.8 vs 91.5, P < .001); these nonetheless remained well short of cognition (mean 103.4, SD 15.2). Behavior and health-related quality of life were unaffected. CONCLUSIONS: With new randomized trials unlikely, this may represent the most definitive population-based evidence supporting universal newborn hearing screening. Although outperforming risk factor screening, school entry language still lagged cognitive abilities by nearly a SD. Prompt intervention and efficacy research are needed for children to reach their potential.


Assuntos
Transtornos da Audição/diagnóstico , Triagem Neonatal/métodos , Austrália , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Análise Custo-Benefício , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Recém-Nascido , Masculino , Triagem Neonatal/economia , Medição de Risco , Fatores de Risco
10.
Pediatrics ; 136(4): e838-47, 2015 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26347428

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: We have previously shown short-term benefits to phonology, letter knowledge, and possibly expressive language from systematically ascertaining language delay at age 4 years followed by the Language for Learning intervention. Here, we report the trial's definitive 6-year outcomes. METHODS: Randomized trial nested in a population-based ascertainment. Children with language scores >1.25 SD below the mean at age 4 were randomized, with intervention children receiving 18 1-hour home-based therapy sessions. Primary outcome was receptive/expressive language. Secondary outcomes were phonological, receptive vocabulary, literacy, and narrative skills; parent-reported pragmatic language, behavior, and health-related quality of life; costs of intervention; and health service use. For intention-to-treat analyses, trial arms were compared using linear regression models. RESULTS: Of 1464 children assessed at age 4, 266 were eligible and 200 randomized; 90% and 82% of intervention and control children were retained respectively. By age 6, mean language scores had normalized, but there was little evidence of a treatment effect for receptive (adjusted mean difference 2.3; 95% confidence interval [CI] -1.2 to 5.7; P = .20) or expressive (0.8; 95% CI -1.6 to 3.2; P = .49) language. Of the secondary outcomes, only phonological awareness skills (effect size 0.36; 95% CI 0.08-0.65; P = .01) showed benefit. Costs were higher for intervention families (mean difference AU$4276; 95% CI: $3424 to $5128). CONCLUSIONS: Population-based intervention targeting 4-year-old language delay was feasible but did not have lasting impacts on language, possibly reflecting resolution in both groups. Long-term literacy benefits remain possible but must be weighed against its cost.


Assuntos
Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/terapia , Terapia da Linguagem/métodos , Austrália , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Estudos Transversais , Feminino , Serviços de Assistência Domiciliar , Humanos , Idioma , Terapia da Linguagem/economia , Masculino , Aceitação pelo Paciente de Cuidados de Saúde , Qualidade de Vida , Resultado do Tratamento , Vocabulário
11.
PLoS One ; 10(8): e0134251, 2015.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26241892

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Evidence is required as to when and where to focus resources to achieve the greatest gains for children's language development. Key to these decisions is the understanding of individual differences in children's language trajectories and the predictors of those differences. To determine optimal timing we must understand if and when children's relative language abilities become fixed. To determine where to focus effort we must identify mutable factors, that is those with the potential to be changed through interventions, which are associated with significant differences in children's language scores and rate of progress. METHODS: Uniquely this study examined individual differences in language growth trajectories in a population sample of children between 4 and 7 years using the multilevel model for change. The influence of predictors, grouped with respect to their mutability and their proximity to the child (least-mutable, mutable-distal, mutable-proximal), were estimated. RESULTS: A significant degree of variability in rate of progress between 4 and 7 years was evident, much of which was systematically associated with mutable-proximal factors, that is, those factors with evidence that they are modifiable through interventions with the child or family, such as shared book reading, TV viewing and number of books in the home. Mutable-distal factors, such as family income, family literacy and neighbourhood disadvantage, hypothesised to be modifiable through social policy, were important predictors of language abilities at 4 years. CONCLUSIONS: Potential levers for language interventions lie in the child's home learning environment from birth to age 4. However, the role of a family's material and cultural capital must not be ignored, nor should the potential for growth into the school years. Early Years services should acknowledge the effects of multiple, cascading and cumulative risks and seek to promote child language development through the aggregation of marginal gains in the pre-school years and beyond.


Assuntos
Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Modelos Psicológicos , Psicologia da Criança , Peso ao Nascer , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Humanos , Renda , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/epidemiologia , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/etiologia , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/prevenção & controle , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/psicologia , Alfabetização , Política Pública , Características de Residência , Fatores de Risco , Fatores Socioeconômicos , Populações Vulneráveis
12.
Disabil Rehabil ; 37(23): 2164-73, 2015.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25586796

RESUMO

PURPOSE: To explore parents' views of the activities and participation of children with cerebral palsy (CP) with a range of communicative abilities and the factors (personal and environmental) that influenced these. METHODS: Thirteen parents of children with CP aged 4-9 years participated in semi-structured individual interviews. Interviews were recorded, transcribed and analysed thematically. Identified codes and themes were mapped to the domains of the International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health - Children and Youth Version (ICF-CY). RESULTS: Parents' responses reflected all ICF-CY domains comprising activity, participation and environmental factors. Codes were primarily mapped to the domains learning and applying knowledge, communication, mobility and interpersonal interactions and relationships. Key barriers identified included aspects of parents' own interactions with their child (e.g. not offering choices), unfamiliar people and settings, negative attitudes of others and children's frustration. Facilitators included support received from the child's family and school, being amongst children, having a familiar routine and the child's positive disposition. CONCLUSIONS: Despite the barriers experienced, children participated in a range of activities. Parents placed importance on communication and its influence on children's independence, behaviour and relationships. Barriers and facilitators identified highlight aspects of the environment that could be modified through intervention to enhance communication and participation. IMPLICATIONS FOR REHABILITATION: Children's activities and participation were largely related to early learning tasks (e.g. literacy), communication, mobility and interactions. Intervention aimed at improving activities and participation may address the various child, impairment, social and environment factors identified here as impacting on activities and participation (e.g. the child's personal characteristics, communication and physical impairments, the support and attitudes of others and the familiarity of the environment). Therapists will need to consider (and manage) the potential negative impact communication deficits may have on children's behaviour, independence and social skills which may in turn detrimentally impact on activity and participation.


Assuntos
Paralisia Cerebral/psicologia , Avaliação da Deficiência , Pais/psicologia , Qualidade de Vida , Participação Social , Atividades Cotidianas , Adulto , Paralisia Cerebral/diagnóstico , Paralisia Cerebral/reabilitação , Criança , Comportamento Infantil , Pré-Escolar , Comunicação , Crianças com Deficiência/reabilitação , Feminino , Humanos , Classificação Internacional de Funcionalidade, Incapacidade e Saúde , Relações Interpessoais , Entrevistas como Assunto , Masculino , Avaliação de Resultados em Cuidados de Saúde , Pesquisa Qualitativa , Inquéritos e Questionários
13.
Int J Speech Lang Pathol ; 17(1): 41-52, 2015 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24801409

RESUMO

PURPOSE: This study aimed to quantify the non-hospital healthcare costs associated with language difficulties within two nationally representative samples of children. METHOD: Data were from three biennial waves (2004-2008) of the Longitudinal Study of Australian Children (B cohort: 0-5 years; K cohort: 4-9 years). Language difficulties were defined as scores ≤ 1.25 SD below the mean on measures of parent-reported communication (0-3 years) and directly assessed vocabulary (4-9 years). Participant data were linked to administrative data on non-hospital healthcare attendances and prescription medications from the universal Australian Medicare subsidized healthcare scheme. RESULT: It was found that healthcare costs over each 2-year age band were higher for children with than without language difficulties at 0-1, 2-3, and 4-5 years, notably 36% higher (mean difference = $AU206, 95% CI = $90, $321) at 4-5 years (B cohort). The slightly higher 2-year healthcare costs for children with language difficulties at 6-7 and 8-9 years were not statistically different from those without language difficulties. Modelled to the corresponding Australian child population, 2-year government costs ranged from $AU1.2-$AU12.1 million (depending on age examined). Six-year healthcare costs increased with the persistence of language difficulties in the K cohort, with total Medicare costs increasing by $192 (95% CI = $74, $311; p = .002) for each additional wave of language difficulties. CONCLUSION: Language difficulties (whether transient or persistent) were associated with substantial excess population healthcare costs in childhood, which are in addition to the known broader costs incurred through the education system. It is unclear whether healthcare costs were specifically due to the assessment and/or treatment of language difficulties, as opposed to conditions that may be co-morbid with or may cause language difficulties.


Assuntos
Custos de Cuidados de Saúde , Transtornos da Linguagem/economia , Transtornos da Linguagem/terapia , Programas Nacionais de Saúde/economia , Patologia da Fala e Linguagem/economia , Fatores Etários , Austrália/epidemiologia , Criança , Comportamento Infantil , Desenvolvimento Infantil , Linguagem Infantil , Pré-Escolar , Custos de Medicamentos , Feminino , Pesquisas sobre Atenção à Saúde , Humanos , Lactente , Recém-Nascido , Transtornos da Linguagem/diagnóstico , Transtornos da Linguagem/epidemiologia , Transtornos da Linguagem/psicologia , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino , Prevalência , Patologia da Fala e Linguagem/métodos , Resultado do Tratamento , Vocabulário
14.
Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med ; 166(11): 1045-52, 2012 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22986757

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: To determine the emergent literacy and language effects of a low-intensity literacy promotion program (Let's Read) provided via universal well-child services to parents during the first 4 years of their child's life. DESIGN: Population-based, cluster randomized controlled trial performed between March 1, 2006, and December 10, 2010. SETTING: Maternal and child health centers (clusters) in 5 relatively disadvantaged local government areas in Melbourne, Australia. PARTICIPANTS: All parents attending their 4-week well-child appointments in participating centers were invited to take part in the study. INTERVENTION: The Let's Read program was delivered at 4, 12, 18, and 42 months during universal well-child care visits. MAIN OUTCOME MEASURE: Child emergent literacy skills (intrasyllabic, phonemic, and sound/letter knowledge) and language (core, receptive, and expressive), measured at 4 years of age. RESULTS: A total of 630 parents participated, with 365 children in 32 intervention clusters and 265 children in 33 control clusters; 563 children (89.4%) were retained in the study to 4 years of age. The adjusted mean differences (intervention minus control) for emergent literacy was 0.2 (95% CI, -0.2 to 0.6; P = .29) for intrasyllabic units, 0.05 (95% CI, -0.4 to 0.5; P = .85) for phonemic awareness, and 0.1 (95% CI, -1.5 to 1.6; P = .92) for letter knowledge. For language, the differences were 1.6 (95% CI, -1.1 to 4.3; P = .25) for core, 0.8 (95% CI, -2.0 to 3.7; P = .56) for receptive, and 1.4 (95% CI, -1.4 to 4.2; P = .32) for expressive scores. CONCLUSION: This population-wide primary care literacy promotion and book distribution program provided neither the anticipated benefits to literacy and language nor enhanced uptake of literacy activities at 4 years of age, even when targeted to relatively disadvantaged areas. TRIAL REGISTRATION: isrctn.org Identifier: ISRCTN04602902.


Assuntos
Serviços de Saúde da Criança , Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Serviços Preventivos de Saúde , Leitura , Livros , Pré-Escolar , Carência Cultural , Educação , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Análise de Intenção de Tratamento , Modelos Lineares , Masculino , Áreas de Pobreza , Fatores Socioeconômicos , Vitória
15.
BMC Pediatr ; 12: 96, 2012 Jul 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22776103

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Early language delay is a high-prevalence condition of concern to parents and professionals. It may result in lifelong deficits not only in language function, but also in social, emotional/behavioural, academic and economic well-being. Such delays can lead to considerable costs to the individual, the family and to society more widely. The Language for Learning trial tests a population-based intervention in 4 year olds with measured language delay, to determine (1) if it improves language and associated outcomes at ages 5 and 6 years and (2) its cost-effectiveness for families and the health care system. METHODS/DESIGN: A large-scale randomised trial of a year-long intervention targeting preschoolers with language delay, nested within a well-documented, prospective, population-based cohort of 1464 children in Melbourne, Australia. All children received a 1.25-1.5 hour formal language assessment at their 4th birthday. The 200 children with expressive and/or receptive language scores more than 1.25 standard deviations below the mean were randomised into intervention or 'usual care' control arms. The 20-session intervention program comprises 18 one-hour home-based therapeutic sessions in three 6-week blocks, an outcome assessment, and a final feed-back/forward planning session. The therapy utilises a 'step up-step down' therapeutic approach depending on the child's language profile, severity and progress, with standardised, manualised activities covering the four language development domains of: vocabulary and grammar; narrative skills; comprehension monitoring; and phonological awareness/pre-literacy skills. Blinded follow-up assessments at ages 5 and 6 years measure the primary outcome of receptive and expressive language, and secondary outcomes of vocabulary, narrative, and phonological skills. DISCUSSION: A key strength of this robust study is the implementation of a therapeutic framework that provides a standardised yet tailored approach for each child, with a focus on specific language domains known to be associated with later language and literacy. The trial responds to identified evidence gaps, has outcomes of direct relevance to families and the community, includes a well-developed economic analysis, and has the potential to improve long-term consequences of early language delay within a public health framework. TRIAL REGISTRATION: Current Controlled Trials ISRCTN03981121.


Assuntos
Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/terapia , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Humanos , Estudos Prospectivos , Características de Residência , Método Simples-Cego , Resultado do Tratamento
17.
J Child Lang ; 37(5): 1109-21, 2010 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19874641

RESUMO

This study examines potential predictors of 'precocious talking' (expressive language ≥90th percentile) at one and two years of age, and of 'stability' in precocious talking across both time periods, drawing on data from a prospective community cohort comprising over 1,800 children. Logistic regression was used to examine the relationship between precocious talking and the following potential predictors: gender, birth order, birth weight, non-English speaking background, socioeconomic status, maternal age, maternal mental health scores, and vocabulary and educational attainment of parents. The strongest predictors of precocity (being female and having a younger mother) warrant further exploration. Overall, however, it appears that precocity in early vocabulary development is not strongly influenced by the variables examined, which together explained just 2.6% and 1% of the variation at 1;0 and 2;0 respectively.


Assuntos
Criança Superdotada , Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Adolescente , Adulto , Ordem de Nascimento/psicologia , Peso ao Nascer , Criança Superdotada/psicologia , Escolaridade , Feminino , Nível de Saúde , Humanos , Lactente , Testes de Linguagem , Modelos Logísticos , Masculino , Idade Materna , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Pais/psicologia , Estudos Prospectivos , Fatores Sexuais , Fatores Socioeconômicos , Vitória , Adulto Jovem
18.
J Paediatr Child Health ; 42(12): 764-70, 2006 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17096710

RESUMO

AIM: To describe changes in infant prelinguistic communication skills between 8 and 12 months, and identify factors associated with those skills. DESIGN: Parent questionnaire data for a prospective population-based cohort of infants in metropolitan Melbourne, Australia. PARTICIPANTS: 1911 infants born November 2002-August 2003. OUTCOME: Infant communication (Communication and Symbolic Behaviour Scales (CSBS)) at 8 and 12 months. Potential risk factors: sex; prematurity; multiple birth; sociodemographic indicators; maternal mental health, vocabulary and education; non-English speaking background; and family history of speech-language difficulties. Linear regression models were fitted to total standardised CSBS scores at 8 and 12 months. RESULTS: Social communication, especially the use of gesture, developed rapidly from 8 to 12 months. Female sex, twin birth, and family history were strongly associated with CSBS scores. The combined model accounted for 5% and 6% of the total variation at 8 and 12 months, respectively. CSBS score at 8 months strongly predicted CSBS score at 12 months (coefficient = 0.56, partial R(2) = 37.0). CONCLUSIONS: There is a dramatic increase in communication skills between 8 and 12 months, particularly the development of gesture, which (as in previous studies) predates and predicts future language development. Risk factors explained little variation in early communication trajectories and therefore, based on our findings, this developmental course is more likely to be biologically predetermined. Rather than focusing on risk factors, we suggest that language promotion activities in otherwise healthy young infants should either be universal or, if targeted, be based on the level of communication skills displayed.


Assuntos
Comunicação não Verbal/fisiologia , Austrália , Linguagem Infantil , Estudos de Coortes , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino , Saúde Mental , Mães/psicologia , Comunicação não Verbal/psicologia , Estudos Prospectivos , Análise de Regressão , Fatores de Risco , Fatores Socioeconômicos , Gêmeos
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA