Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 62
Filter
Add more filters

Country/Region as subject
Publication year range
1.
BMC Med ; 22(1): 265, 2024 Jun 24.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38915081

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: People with different types of dementia may have distinct symptoms and experiences that affect their quality of life. This study investigated whether quality of life varied across types of dementia and over time. METHODS: The participants were 1555 people with mild-to-moderate dementia and 1327 carers from the IDEAL longitudinal cohort study, recruited from clinical services. As many as possible were followed for up to 6 years. Diagnoses included were Alzheimer's disease, vascular dementia, mixed Alzheimer's and vascular dementia, Parkinson's disease dementia, dementia with Lewy bodies, and frontotemporal dementia. Self- and informant-rated versions of the Quality of Life in Alzheimer's Disease scale were used. A joint model, incorporating a mixed effects model with random effects and a survival model to account for dropout, was used to examine whether quality of life varied by dementia type at the time of diagnosis and how trajectories changed over time. RESULTS: The strongest associations between dementia type and quality of life were seen around the time of diagnosis. For both self-ratings and informant ratings, people with Parkinson's disease dementia or dementia with Lewy bodies had lower quality of life scores. Over time there was little change in self-rated scores across all dementia types (- 0.15 points per year). Informant-rated scores declined over time (- 1.63 points per year), with the greatest decline seen in ratings by informants for people with dementia with Lewy bodies (- 2.18 points per year). CONCLUSIONS: Self-rated quality of life scores were relatively stable over time whilst informant ratings showed a steeper decline. People with Parkinson's disease dementia or dementia with Lewy bodies report particularly low levels of quality of life, indicating the importance of greater attention to the needs of these groups.


Subject(s)
Dementia , Quality of Life , Humans , Quality of Life/psychology , Male , Female , Longitudinal Studies , Aged , Dementia/psychology , Aged, 80 and over , Middle Aged
2.
Cogn Neuropsychiatry ; 27(4): 296-313, 2022 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35477346

ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION: Alwyn Lishman was interested in how memory research could be applied to clinical psychiatry. After a brief review of his major contributions, this paper will focus on his research on the alcoholic Korsakoff syndrome. It will consider how his findings relate to contemporary debates, particularly on how the syndrome should be defined, and its relationship to broader alcohol-induced cognitive impairments. METHODS: A review of the contribution of Alwyn Lishman, Robin Jacobson and colleagues to our knowledge of Korsakoff's syndrome, together with a review of the pertinent recent literature. RESULTS: Lishman and colleagues followed earlier authors in defining the Korsakoff syndrome in terms of disproportionate memory impairment, but they also noted a variable degree of IQ, frontal-executive, and timed visuo-spatial impairment in their cases. More recent authors have included such features in their definitions of the syndrome. Lishman also argued for a specific "alcoholic dementia". The present paper argues that recent definitions of the Korsakoff syndrome confound its core and associated features, and also fail to recognise the multifactorial basis of alcohol-related brain damage. CONCLUSIONS: Korsakoff's syndrome is best defined in terms of disproportionate memory impairment, and more widespread cognitive impairment is best encompassed within "alcohol-related brain damage".


Subject(s)
Alcohol Amnestic Disorder , Alcoholism , Cognitive Dysfunction , Korsakoff Syndrome , Wernicke Encephalopathy , Alcohol Amnestic Disorder/complications , Alcohol Amnestic Disorder/psychology , Alcoholism/psychology , Humans , Korsakoff Syndrome/complications , Memory Disorders , Wernicke Encephalopathy/complications
3.
Age Ageing ; 49(3): 446-452, 2020 04 27.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32037460

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: a large number of studies have explored factors related to self- and informant ratings of quality of life in people with dementia, but many studies have had relatively small sample sizes and mainly focused on health conditions and dementia symptoms. The aim of this study is to compare self- and informant-rated quality of life, life satisfaction and well-being, and investigate the relationships of the two different rating methods with various social, psychological and health factors, using a large cohort study of community-dwelling people with dementia and carers in Great Britain. METHODS: this study included 1,283 dyads of people with mild-to-moderate dementia and their primary carers in the Improving the experience of Dementia and Enhancing Active Life study. Multivariate modelling was used to investigate associations of self- and informant-rated quality of life, life satisfaction and well-being with factors in five domains: psychological characteristics and health; social location; capitals, assets and resources; physical fitness and health; and managing everyday life with dementia. RESULTS: people with dementia rated their quality of life, life satisfaction and well-being more highly than did the informants. Despite these differences, the two approaches had similar relationships with social, psychological and physical health factors in the five domains. CONCLUSION: although self- and informant ratings differ, they display similar results when focusing on factors associated with quality of life, life satisfaction and well-being. Either self- or informant ratings may offer a reasonable source of information about people with dementia in terms of understanding associated factors.


Subject(s)
Dementia , Quality of Life , Cohort Studies , Dementia/diagnosis , Dementia/therapy , Humans , Personal Satisfaction , United Kingdom
4.
Aging Ment Health ; 24(6): 956-964, 2020 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30836765

ABSTRACT

Objectives: Increasingly, research has explored how psychological resources enable adaptation to illness. However, it is unclear whether psychological resources protect against the potential negative effects on living well with a progressive and life-limiting condition such as dementia. This paper examines the association between psychological resources and the ability to 'live well' with dementia.Method: Data from 1547 people with mild-to-moderate dementia in the Improving the experience of Dementia and Enhancing Active Life (IDEAL) cohort were used. Multivariate linear regression was employed to examine the association between self-reported measures of psychological resources (self-efficacy, optimism and self-esteem) and indices of capability to 'live well' (quality of life, well-being and life satisfaction).Results: All three measures of psychological resources had positive and independent associations with indices of living well and the effect sizes were similar. Effect sizes reduced when accounting for shared variance between psychological resources, showing some overlap in these constructs.Conclusion: Self-efficacy, optimism and self-esteem were each associated with capability to 'live well'. Overlap between these three resources is evident and when combined they may provide greater resilience when dealing with the challenges of living with dementia. Interventions for people with dementia could seek to improve levels of these potentially-modifiable psychological resources.


Subject(s)
Dementia , Quality of Life , Adaptation, Psychological , Cohort Studies , Humans , Self Concept , Self Efficacy
5.
J Neurovirol ; 25(6): 754-764, 2019 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31214916

ABSTRACT

This study investigated whether HIV-positive participants, stable on combined antiretroviral therapy (cART), showed cognitive impairments relative to HIV-negative controls; and whether clinical and neuroimaging factors correlated with cognitive function in the HIV-positive participants. One hundred and twenty-six white men who have sex with men, of whom 78 were HIV-positive and stable on cART and 48 were HIV negative, were recruited to this cross-sectional study. The median age of HIV-positive participants in this study was 47. They underwent clinical and neuropsychological evaluation and magnetic resonance imaging of the brain, including diffusion tensor imaging (DTI). Cognitive scores for both groups were compared, and regression models were run to explore the influence of clinical, psychiatric, lifestyle, and neuroimaging variables on cognition. The prevalence of neurocognitive impairment, using the multivariate normative comparison criteria, was 28% in HIV-positive participants and 5% in HIV-negative participants. After covarying for age, years of education, and non-English speaking background, there were significant differences between the HIV group and the controls across four cognitive domains. The HIV group showed significantly higher mean diffusivity (MD) and lower fractional anisotropy (FA) than the control group on DTI. Although anxiety levels were clinically low, anxiety and DTI measures were the only variables to show significant correlations with cognitive function. In the HIV group, poorer cognitive performance was associated with higher MD and lower FA on DTI and higher (albeit clinically mild) levels of anxiety. Our findings suggest that white matter changes and subtle anxiety levels contribute independently to cognitive impairment in HIV.


Subject(s)
AIDS Dementia Complex/diagnostic imaging , AIDS Dementia Complex/epidemiology , AIDS Dementia Complex/pathology , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Brain/pathology , Adult , Aged , Anxiety/epidemiology , Cognitive Dysfunction/epidemiology , Cognitive Dysfunction/etiology , Cross-Sectional Studies , Diffusion Tensor Imaging , HIV Infections/complications , HIV Infections/diagnostic imaging , HIV Infections/pathology , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Neuroimaging , Prevalence
6.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc ; 25(10): 1061-1075, 2019 11.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31474234

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVES: In this paper, I review three 'anomalies' or disorders in autobiographical memory: neurological retrograde amnesia (RA), spontaneous confabulation, and psychogenic amnesia. METHODS: Existing theories are reviewed, their limitations considered, some of my own empirical findings briefly described, and possible interpretations proposed and interspersed with illustrative case-reports. RESULTS: In RA, there may be an important retrieval component to the deficit, and factors at encoding may give rise to the relative preservation of early memories (and the reminiscence bump) which manifests as a temporal gradient. Spontaneous confabulation appears to be associated with a damaged 'filter' in orbitofrontal and ventromedial frontal regions. Consistent with this, an empirical study has shown that both the initial severity of confabulation and its subsequent decline are associated with changes in the executive function (especially in cognitive estimate errors) and inversely with the quantity of accurate autobiographical memories retrieved. Psychogenic amnesia can be 'global' or 'situation-specific'. The former is associated with a precipitating stress, depressed mood, and (often) a past history of a transient neurological amnesia. In these circumstances, frontal control mechanisms can inhibit retrieval of autobiographical memories, and even the sense of 'self' (identity), while compromised medial temporal function prevents subsequent retrieval of what occurred during a 'fugue'. An empirical investigation of psychogenic amnesia and some recent imaging studies have provided findings consistent with this view. CONCLUSIONS: Taken together, these various observations point to the importance of frontal 'control' systems (in interaction with medial temporal/hippocampal systems) in the retrieval and, more particularly, the disrupted retrieval of 'old' memories.


Subject(s)
Amnesia/physiopathology , Executive Function/physiology , Memory, Episodic , Mental Recall/physiology , Prefrontal Cortex/physiopathology , Humans
7.
Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord ; 33(1): 29-35, 2019.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30802226

ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION: Understanding key influences on outcomes for caregivers of people with dementia is hampered by inconsistent conceptualization and measurement of outcomes and limited evidence about the relative impact of different variables. We aimed to address these issues. METHODS: We analyzed data from 1283 caregivers of community-dwelling individuals with mild-to-moderate dementia in the Improving the experience of Dementia and Enhancing Active Life cohort study. We generated a "living well" latent factor from measures of quality of life, satisfaction with life, and well-being. We used structural equation modelling to derive latent variables for 7 domains reflecting caregivers' perceptions of their personal resources and experiences, and to examine the associations with caregivers' perceptions of their capability to "live well." RESULTS: The domain of psychological characteristics and psychological health was most strongly related to living well [2.53; 95% confidence interval (CI), 2.08-2.97], followed by physical fitness and physical health (1.48; 95% CI, 1.04-1.91) and experiencing caregiving (1.34; 95% CI, 0.99-1.70). Social capitals, assets and resources (0.68; 95% CI, 0.35-1.00) and relationship with the person with dementia (-0.22; 95% CI, -0.41 to -0.03) had smaller, significant associations. Social location (0.28; 95% CI, -0.33 to 0.89) and managing everyday life with dementia (0.06; 95% CI, -0.15 to 0.28) were not significantly associated with living well. DISCUSSION: These findings demonstrate the importance of supporting caregivers' psychological and physical health and their ability to develop and maintain positive coping strategies, as well as enabling them to maintain vital social capitals, assets and resources.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Psychological , Caregivers/psychology , Dementia/nursing , Quality of Life/psychology , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Caregivers/statistics & numerical data , Cohort Studies , Female , Humans , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Middle Aged , Physical Fitness/psychology , Social Support , Surveys and Questionnaires , United Kingdom
8.
Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord ; 33(1): 36-41, 2019.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30802227

ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION: We aimed to better understand what predicts the capability to "live well" with dementia by identifying the relative contribution of life domains associated with the subjective experience of living well. METHODS: We analyzed data from 1547 individuals with mild-to-moderate dementia in the IDEAL cohort. We generated a "living well" latent factor from measures of quality of life, satisfaction with life, and well-being. We used multivariate modeling to identify variables related to living well measures and structural equation modeling to derive latent variables for 5 life domains and to examine the associations of these domains with living well. RESULTS: All 5 domains were individually associated with living well. When modeled together, the psychological characteristics and psychological health domain was the only independent predictor of living well [effect size, 3.55; 95% confidence interval (CI): 2.93-4.17], and effect sizes were smaller for physical fitness and physical health (1.23, 95% CI: -0.10 to 2.58), social capitals, assets and resources (0.67; 95% CI: -0.04 to 1.38), managing everyday life with dementia (0.33; 95% CI: -0.06 to 0.71), and social location (0.08; 95% CI: -2.10 to 2.26). DISCUSSION: Psychological resources, and the social, environmental, and physical factors that underpin positive psychological states, are potentially important targets for interventions and initiatives that aim to improve the experience of living with dementia.


Subject(s)
Activities of Daily Living/psychology , Dementia/psychology , Quality of Life/psychology , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Cohort Studies , Cross-Sectional Studies , Female , Humans , Independent Living , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Middle Aged , Personal Satisfaction , Physical Fitness , United Kingdom
9.
Age Ageing ; 48(3): 361-367, 2019 05 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30403771

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: The aim was to investigate the co-morbidity profile of people with dementia and examine the associations between severity of co-morbidity, health-related quality of life (HRQoL) and quality of life (QoL). METHODS: The improving the experience of Dementia and Enhancing Active Life (IDEAL) cohort consisted of 1,547 people diagnosed with dementia who provided information on the number and type of co-morbid conditions. Participants also provided ratings of their health-related and dementia-specific QoL. RESULTS: The majority of the sample were living with more than one chronic condition. Hypertension was commonly reported and frequently combined with connective tissue disease, diabetes and depression. The number of co-morbid conditions was associated with low QoL scores, and those with severe co-morbidity (≥5 conditions) showed the greatest impact on their well-being. CONCLUSIONS: Co-morbidity is an important risk factor for poor QoL and health status in people with dementia. Greater recognition of the nature and impact of co-morbidity is needed to inform support and interventions for people with dementia and a multidisciplinary approach to care provision is recommended.


Subject(s)
Dementia/epidemiology , Quality of Life , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Comorbidity , Dementia/psychology , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Prevalence , Quality of Life/psychology , Severity of Illness Index
10.
Brain ; 140(9): 2498-2510, 2017 Sep 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29050391

ABSTRACT

There are very few case series of patients with acute psychogenic memory loss (also known as dissociative/functional amnesia), and still fewer studies of outcome, or comparisons with neurological memory-disordered patients. Consequently, the literature on psychogenic amnesia is somewhat fragmented and offers little prognostic value for individual patients. In the present study, we reviewed the case records and neuropsychological findings in 53 psychogenic amnesia cases (ratio of 3:1, males:females), in comparison with 21 consecutively recruited neurological memory-disordered patients and 14 healthy control subjects. In particular, we examined the pattern of retrograde amnesia on an assessment of autobiographical memory (the Autobiographical Memory Interview). We found that our patients with psychogenic memory loss fell into four distinct groups, which we categorized as: (i) fugue state; (ii) fugue-to-focal retrograde amnesia; (iii) psychogenic focal retrograde amnesia following a minor neurological episode; and (iv) patients with gaps in their memories. While neurological cases were characterized by relevant neurological symptoms, a history of a past head injury was actually more common in our psychogenic cases (P = 0.012), perhaps reflecting a 'learning episode' predisposing to later psychological amnesia. As anticipated, loss of the sense of personal identity was confined to the psychogenic group. However, clinical depression, family/relationship problems, financial/employment problems, and failure to recognize the family were also statistically more common in that group. The pattern of autobiographical memory loss differed between the psychogenic groups: fugue cases showed a severe and uniform loss of memories for both facts and events across all time periods, whereas the two focal retrograde amnesia groups showed a 'reversed' temporal gradient with relative sparing of recent memories. After 3-6 months, the fugue patients had improved to normal scores for facts and near-normal scores for events. By contrast, the two focal retrograde amnesia groups showed less improvement and continued to show a reversed temporal gradient. In conclusion, the outcome in psychogenic amnesia, particularly those characterized by fugue, is better than generally supposed. Findings are interpreted in terms of Markowitsch's and Kopelman's models of psychogenic amnesia, and with respect to Anderson's neuroimaging findings in memory inhibition.


Subject(s)
Amnesia, Retrograde/classification , Amnesia/classification , Adult , Aged , Amnesia/complications , Amnesia, Retrograde/complications , Craniocerebral Trauma/complications , Craniocerebral Trauma/epidemiology , Depression/complications , Depression/epidemiology , Family Conflict , Female , Humans , London/epidemiology , Male , Memory, Episodic , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Self Concept , Young Adult
11.
Hippocampus ; 27(2): 194-209, 2017 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27859925

ABSTRACT

The specific role of the perirhinal (PRC), entorhinal (ERC) and parahippocampal cortices (PHC) in supporting familiarity-based recognition remains unknown. An fMRI study explored whether these medial temporal lobe (MTL) structures responded in the same way or differentially to familiarity as a function of stimulus type at recognition. A secondary aim was to explore whether the hippocampus responds in the same way to equally strong familiarity and recollection and whether this is influenced by the kind of stimulus involved. Univariate and multivariate analyses revealed that familiarity responses in the PRC, ERC, PHC and the amygdala are material-specific. Specifically, the PRC and ERC selectively responded to object familiarity, while the PHC responded to both object and scene familiarity. The amygdala only responded to familiarity memory for faces. The hippocampus did not respond to stimulus familiarity for any of the three types of stimuli, but it did respond to recollection for all three types of stimuli. This was true even when recollection was contrasted to equally accurate familiarity. Overall, the findings suggest that the role of the MTL neocortices and the amygdala in familiarity-based recognition depends on the kind of stimulus in memory, whereas the role of the hippocampus in recollection is independent of the type of cuing stimulus. © 2016 The Authors Hippocampus Published by Wiley Periodicals, Inc.


Subject(s)
Hippocampus/physiology , Mental Recall/physiology , Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Temporal Lobe/physiology , Brain Mapping , Female , Hippocampus/diagnostic imaging , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Multivariate Analysis , Neuropsychological Tests , Temporal Lobe/diagnostic imaging , Visual Perception/physiology , Young Adult
12.
Epilepsy Behav ; 77: 30-32, 2017 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29080418

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVES: This research investigated forgetting rates of patients with temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE) at brief and longer intervals. METHODS: The sample is formed by 5 patients with TLE and 10 healthy individuals. One of the patients received the diagnosis of transient epileptic amnesia (TEA). All patients underwent a standardized clinical protocol for diagnosis including a comprehensive neuropsychological assessment. In addition, two experimental tasks were used to assess the forgetting rates at 4 intervals (30s, 10min, 1day, and 1week): a story task to evaluate verbal cued recall and a route task to assess visuospatial cued recall. RESULTS: There were no significant differences between groups in forgetting rates. CONCLUSIONS: These findings suggest that forgetting patterns in patients with TLE may be heterogeneous, and the presence of accelerated long-term forgetting is not universal.


Subject(s)
Amnesia/diagnosis , Epilepsy, Temporal Lobe/psychology , Mental Recall/physiology , Adult , Amnesia/complications , Amnesia/psychology , Cues , Epilepsy, Temporal Lobe/complications , Female , Humans , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Young Adult
13.
Neuropsychol Rehabil ; 25(2): 159-88, 2015.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24894460

ABSTRACT

The term prospective memory (PM) refers to memory for future intentions. PM problems are frequent in people with cognitive impairment and, because they are central to the realisation of many everyday goals, are important in rehabilitation. Event-based PM tasks (EBPM) are environmentally-cued and have primarily mnemonic demands, whereas time-based PM tasks (TBPM) require self-initiated retrieval, and have greater executive demands. Errorless learning (EL) is an encoding method that results in superior retrospective memory compared with "errorful" learning (EF). As this EL advantage (ELA) likely stems from its reduced explicit memory demands, and there is no such advantage for executive tasks, a greater ELA for EBPM than TBPM was predicted. Fourteen adults with neurological memory impairment completed PM tasks under four counterbalanced conditions: EL of EBPM, EL of TBPM, EF of EBPM, and EF of TBPM. A significant ELA was observed for EBPM (d = .63), but not TBPM (d = -.01). These results extend the evidence for EL within cognitive rehabilitation, by showing for the first time that the method can benefit future action in addition to retrospective memory. The clinical implications are also clear: errorless learning techniques may be usefully employed to support completion of day-to-day EBPM tasks.


Subject(s)
Learning , Memory Disorders/rehabilitation , Memory, Episodic , Adult , Aged , Cues , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Psychological Tests
14.
J Ment Health ; 24(5): 266-70, 2015.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25188922

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: The use of mobile devices to deliver healthcare has not yet been exploited in neuropsychological rehabilitation. Smartphones have the potential to serve as multi-functional memory aids. AIMS: To investigate whether patients attending a clinic for mixed memory problems own smartphones, to determine whether this could be a widely applicable medium to use as a memory aids device. METHODS: A questionnaire on smartphone ownership was given to an opportunity sample of consecutive patients attending a neuropsychiatry and memory disorders outpatient clinic. Data were collected in 2012 and repeated 12 months later in 2013 to assess changes over time. RESULTS: Ownership of mobile phones was stable between 2012 (81%) and 2013 (85%), but ownership of smartphones showed a significant increase (from 26% to 40%). Age negatively predicted smartphone ownership. CONCLUSION: Despite cognitive or psychiatric problems, our patient group are as likely to own a mobile phone as a member of the general population. Ownership levels are at 40% and likely to increase in the future. Exploring how smartphones and their apps could function as memory aids is likely to be useful for a large enough number of patients to be clinically worthwhile.


Subject(s)
Memory Disorders/psychology , Smartphone/statistics & numerical data , Telemedicine/statistics & numerical data , Female , Humans , Male , Memory Disorders/prevention & control , Middle Aged , Surveys and Questionnaires
15.
Health Qual Life Outcomes ; 12: 164, 2014 Nov 30.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25433373

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Enabling people with dementia and carers to 'live well' with the condition is a key United Kingdom policy objective. The aim of this project is to identify what helps people to live well or makes it difficult to live well in the context of having dementia or caring for a person with dementia, and to understand what 'living well' means from the perspective of people with dementia and carers. METHODS/DESIGN: Over a two-year period, 1500 people with early-stage dementia throughout Great Britain will be recruited to the study, together with a carer wherever possible. All the participants will be visited at home initially and again 12 months and 24 months later. This will provide information about the way in which well-being, life satisfaction and quality of life are affected by social capitals, assets and resources, the challenges posed by dementia, and the ways in which people adjust to and cope with these challenges. A smaller group will be interviewed in more depth. DISCUSSION: The findings will lead to recommendations about what can be done by individuals, communities, health and social care practitioners, care providers and policy-makers to improve the likelihood of living well with dementia.


Subject(s)
Dementia/psychology , Personal Satisfaction , Quality of Life/psychology , Self Concept , Activities of Daily Living/psychology , Adaptation, Psychological , Caregivers/psychology , Dementia/nursing , Female , Humans , Interpersonal Relations , Male , Middle Aged , Residence Characteristics , United Kingdom
16.
Can J Ophthalmol ; 2024 Apr 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38580217

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: Recent research has helped to develop a more detailed understanding of many functional neurologic disorders. The aim of this study was to increase our knowledge of functional visual loss and to compare the findings with those of other functional syndromes. DESIGN: Prospective and retrospective observational cohort study. METHODS: This study took place at neuro-ophthalmology clinics at 3 major hospitals in London, United Kingdom, over a 12-month period. The study population consisted of 157 participants, 100 with functional visual loss, 21 pathologic control subjects with organic visual loss, and 36 healthy nonpathologic control subjects. All participants had their diagnosis confirmed with a full neuro-ophthalmic examination, neuroimaging, and visual electrophysiology. A full assessment of all participants' medical history was obtained from their general practitioner, and all participants completed a series of questionnaires assessing relevant associations. RESULTS: Data were obtained on 157 participants, 100 with functional visual loss, 21 pathologic control subjects with organic visual loss, and 36 healthy nonpathologic control subjects. Participants with functional visual loss were typically female (74%) with a mean age at vision loss of 40.0 ± 16 years. Sixty-four percent of participants had bilateral vision loss; the remainder, unilateral loss. Twenty-six percent of the total cohort had organic visual loss with functional overlay. Fifty percent of participants with functional visual loss had a preexisting psychiatric diagnosis, the most common being a depressive disorder. Sixty-two percent of participants had an ocular history, and 87% had a previously diagnosed medical illness, most commonly neurologic (45%). Thirty-five percent of participants self-reported at least 1 additional functional symptom. CONCLUSIONS: Our population of functional visual loss subjects shares many similarities with the majority of patients with other functional neurologic disorders. They are generally young and female and have a greater than expected rate of psychiatric, medical, and coexisting ocular conditions. We found increased rates of precipitating stressors, clinical depression, and organic eye problems in subjects with functional visual loss.

17.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 34(10): 2484-93, 2013 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22496057

ABSTRACT

Despite advances in the treatment of patients with human immunodeficiency virus (HIV), HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder occurs in 15-50% of HIV-infected individuals, and may become more apparent as ageing advances. In the present study we investigated regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) and regional cerebral metabolic rate of glucose uptake (rCMRglc) in medically and psychiatrically stable HIV-1-infected participants in two age-groups. Positron emission tomography (PET) and magnetic resonance imaging (MRI)-based arterial spin labeling (ASL) were used to measure rCMRglc and rCBF, respectively, in 35 HIV-infected participants and 37 HIV-negative matched controls. All participants were currently asymptomatic with undetectable HIV-1 viral loads, without medical or psychiatric comorbidity, alcohol or substance misuse, stable on medication for at least 6 months before enrolment in the study. We found significant age effects on both ASL and PET with reduced rCBF and rCMRglc in related frontal brain regions, and consistent, although small, reductions in rCBF and rCMRglc in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) in HIV, a finding of potential clinical significance. There was no significant interaction between HIV status and the ageing process, and no significant HIV-related changes elsewhere in the brain on PET or ASL. This is the first paper to combine evidence from ASL and PET method in HIV participants. These finding provide evidence of crossvalidity between the two techniques, both in ageing and a clinical condition (HIV).


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping/methods , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Cerebrovascular Circulation , Fluorine Radioisotopes , Fluorodeoxyglucose F18 , HIV Infections/physiopathology , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Multimodal Imaging , Positron-Emission Tomography , Radiopharmaceuticals , Adult , Affect , Aged , Aging/pathology , Antiretroviral Therapy, Highly Active , Asymptomatic Diseases , Brain/blood supply , Brain/metabolism , Corpus Striatum/pathology , Corpus Striatum/physiopathology , Fluorine Radioisotopes/pharmacokinetics , Fluorodeoxyglucose F18/pharmacokinetics , HIV Infections/drug therapy , HIV Infections/psychology , HIV-1 , Humans , Intelligence Tests , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Radiopharmaceuticals/pharmacokinetics , Spin Labels , Young Adult
18.
J Clin Med ; 12(19)2023 Sep 30.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37834954

ABSTRACT

This paper begins with a short case report of florid, spontaneous confabulation in a 61-year-old man with an alcohol-induced Wernicke-Korsakoff syndrome. His confabulation extended across episodic and personal semantic memory, as well as orientation in time and place, as measured on Dalla Barba's Confabulation Battery. Five other brief case summaries will then be presented, followed by a summary of the clinical, neurological, and background neuropsychological findings in three earlier series of Korsakoff patients. These observations will be considered in light of Wijnia's recent and my own, earlier reviews of the Korsakoff syndrome. Taken together, they indicate the need for a multi-faceted approach (clinical, neurological, neuropsychological, and neuroimaging) to the assessment and diagnosis of the disorder.

19.
Neuropsychol Rev ; 22(2): 117-31, 2012 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22580849

ABSTRACT

Memory for contextual information and target-context integration are crucial for successful episodic memory formation and are impaired in patients with Korsakoff's syndrome. In this paper we review the evidence for the notion that a context memory deficit makes an important contribution to the amnesia in these patients. First, we focus on anterograde memory for contextual (spatial and temporal) information. Next, the use of contextual cues in memory retrieval is examined and their role in retrograde amnesia and confabulation. Evidence on the role of contextual cues and associations in working memory is discussed in relation to the underlying neurocognitive mechanisms and their dissociation from long-term encoding. Finally, we focus on implicit learning of contextual information in Korsakoff patients. It can be concluded that Korsakoff patients are impaired in the explicit processing of contextual information and in target-context binding, both in long-term (retrograde and anterograde) memory and in working memory. These results extend the context memory deficit hypothesis. In contrast, implicit contextual learning is relatively preserved in these patients. These findings are discussed in relation to evidence of dysfunction of the extended diencephalic-hippocampal memory circuit in Korsakoff's syndrome.


Subject(s)
Korsakoff Syndrome/physiopathology , Memory, Episodic , Memory/physiology , Humans
20.
Cortex ; 150: 137-148, 2022 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35390739

ABSTRACT

Research on autobiographical memory loss in Alzheimer's disease (AD) is characterized by conflicting findings concerning a possible sparing of older memories. The literature shows evidence for both a negative temporal gradient, a flat gradient and a reminiscence bump - that is, a disproportionally high frequency of memories from early adulthood relative to surrounding periods. Here, we expanded the number of lifetime periods of the Autobiographical Memory Interview (AMI; Kopelman, Wilson & Baddeley, 1989, 1990) from the standard three to seven in order to increase the sensitivity of the test to variations in the temporal distribution of autobiographical memories across the life span. Twenty-five older adults diagnosed with AD (MMMSE = 21.16, SD = 5.08) and a matched sample of 30 healthy, older adults were assessed. The temporal distribution for personal semantic information in AD showed a temporal gradient steadily decreasing from middle childhood to present life, consistent with predictions derived from consolidation theories. In comparison, the temporal distribution of incidents/episodic memories produced by AD patients in response to the expanded AMI showed a predominance of autobiographical memories from age 6 to 30, followed by a steep drop in memory referring to events that had occurred after age 30. This distribution challenges standard theories of retrograde amnesia in AD by showing neither a temporal gradient, decreasing progressively from early to later life, nor a flat gradient. In contrast, the distribution is consistent with the reminiscence bump identified in autobiographical memory research. Schematization and retrieval support provided by cultural life scripts are discussed.


Subject(s)
Alzheimer Disease , Memory, Episodic , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Child , Humans , Mental Recall , Semantics , Young Adult
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL