Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 10 de 10
Filter
1.
J Neurosci ; 38(13): 3265-3272, 2018 03 28.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29467145

ABSTRACT

Environmental boundaries play a crucial role in spatial navigation and memory across a wide range of distantly related species. In rodents, boundary representations have been identified at the single-cell level in the subiculum and entorhinal cortex of the hippocampal formation. Although studies of hippocampal function and spatial behavior suggest that similar representations might exist in humans, boundary-related neural activity has not been identified electrophysiologically in humans until now. To address this gap in the literature, we analyzed intracranial recordings from the hippocampal formation of surgical epilepsy patients (of both sexes) while they performed a virtual spatial navigation task and compared the power in three frequency bands (1-4, 4-10, and 30-90 Hz) for target locations near and far from the environmental boundaries. Our results suggest that encoding locations near boundaries elicited stronger theta oscillations than for target locations near the center of the environment and that this difference cannot be explained by variables such as trial length, speed, movement, or performance. These findings provide direct evidence of boundary-dependent neural activity localized in humans to the subiculum, the homolog of the hippocampal subregion in which most boundary cells are found in rodents, and indicate that this system can represent attended locations that rather than the position of one's own body.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Spatial computations using environmental boundaries are an integral part of the brain's spatial mapping system. In rodents, border/boundary cells in the subiculum and entorhinal cortex reveal boundary coding at the single-neuron level. Although there is good reason to believe that such representations also exist in humans, the evidence has thus far been limited to functional neuroimaging studies that broadly implicate the hippocampus in boundary-based navigation. By combining intracranial recordings with high-resolution imaging of hippocampal subregions, we identified a neural marker of boundary representation in the human subiculum.


Subject(s)
Hippocampus/physiology , Spatial Navigation , Theta Rhythm , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged
2.
Behav Res Methods ; 45(4): 1293-312, 2013 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23549683

ABSTRACT

Recent advances in neuroimaging and neural recording techniques have enabled researchers to make significant progress in understanding the neural mechanisms underlying human spatial navigation. Because these techniques generally require participants to remain stationary, computer-generated virtual environments are used. We introduce PandaEPL, a programming library for the Python language designed to simplify the creation of computer-controlled spatial-navigation experiments. PandaEPL is built on top of Panda3D, a modern open-source game engine. It allows users to construct three-dimensional environments that participants can navigate from a first-person perspective. Sound playback and recording and also joystick support are provided through the use of additional optional libraries. PandaEPL also handles many tasks common to all cognitive experiments, including managing configuration files, logging all internal and participant-generated events, and keeping track of the experiment state. We describe how PandaEPL compares with other software for building spatial-navigation experiments and walk the reader through the process of creating a fully functional experiment.


Subject(s)
Behavioral Sciences/instrumentation , Behavioral Sciences/methods , Libraries, Digital , Software , Spatial Behavior/physiology , User-Computer Interface , Adult , Computers , Data Display , Environment Design , Humans , Research Design
3.
Mem Cognit ; 2012 Jan 31.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22290593

ABSTRACT

Although much is known about the dynamics of memory search in the free recall task, relatively little is known about the factors related to recall termination. Reanalyzing individual trial data from 14 prior studies (1,079 participants in 28,015 trials) and defining termination as occurring when a final response is followed by a long nonresponse interval, we observed that termination probability increased throughout the recall period and that retrieval was more likely to terminate following an error than following a correct response. Among errors, termination probability was higher following prior-list intrusions and repetitions than following extralist intrusions. To verify that this pattern of results can be seen in a single study, we report a new experiment in which 80 participants contributed recall data from a total of 9,122 trials. This experiment replicated the pattern observed in the aggregate analysis of the prior studies.

4.
Mem Cognit ; 38(6): 689-99, 2010 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20852233

ABSTRACT

One way to study the associative processes at work during episodic memory is to examine the order of participant responses, which reveal the strong tendency to transition between temporally contiguous or semantically proximal items on the study list. Here, we assessed the correlation between participants' recall performance and their use of semantic and temporal associations to guide retrieval across nine delayed free recall studies. The size of the participants' temporal contiguity effects predicted their recall performance. When interpreted in terms of two models of episodic memory, these results suggest that participants who more effectively form and retrieve associations between items that occur nearby in time perform better on episodic recall tasks. Sample code may be downloaded as a supplement for this article from http://mc.psychonomic-journals.org/content/supplemental.


Subject(s)
Association Learning , Mental Recall , Retention, Psychology , Serial Learning , Attention , Humans , Judgment , Proactive Inhibition , Semantics , Verbal Learning
5.
Brain Stimul ; 13(5): 1183-1195, 2020.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32446925

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Researchers have used direct electrical brain stimulation to treat a range of neurological and psychiatric disorders. However, for brain stimulation to be maximally effective, clinicians and researchers should optimize stimulation parameters according to desired outcomes. OBJECTIVE: The goal of our large-scale study was to comprehensively evaluate the effects of stimulation at different parameters and locations on neuronal activity across the human brain. METHODS: To examine how different kinds of stimulation affect human brain activity, we compared the changes in neuronal activity that resulted from stimulation at a range of frequencies, amplitudes, and locations with direct human brain recordings. We recorded human brain activity directly with electrodes that were implanted in widespread regions across 106 neurosurgical epilepsy patients while systematically stimulating across a range of parameters and locations. RESULTS: Overall, stimulation most often had an inhibitory effect on neuronal activity, consistent with earlier work. When stimulation excited neuronal activity, it most often occurred from high-frequency stimulation. These effects were modulated by the location of the stimulating electrode, with stimulation sites near white matter more likely to cause excitation and sites near gray matter more likely to inhibit neuronal activity. CONCLUSION: By characterizing how different stimulation parameters produced specific neuronal activity patterns on a large scale, our results provide an electrophysiological framework that clinicians and researchers may consider when designing stimulation protocols to cause precisely targeted changes in human brain activity.


Subject(s)
Brain/diagnostic imaging , Brain/physiology , Deep Brain Stimulation/methods , White Matter/diagnostic imaging , White Matter/physiology , Adult , Brain Mapping/methods , Drug Resistant Epilepsy/diagnostic imaging , Drug Resistant Epilepsy/physiopathology , Drug Resistant Epilepsy/therapy , Electrocorticography/methods , Electrodes, Implanted , Female , Gray Matter/diagnostic imaging , Gray Matter/physiology , Humans , Male , Stereotaxic Techniques
6.
Curr Biol ; 25(8): 1080-5, 2015 Apr 20.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25843029

ABSTRACT

The ability to remember and navigate spatial environments is critical for everyday life. A primary mechanism by which the brain represents space is through hippocampal place cells, which indicate when an animal is at a particular location. An important issue is understanding how the hippocampal place-cell network represents specific properties of the environment, such as signifying that a particular position is near a doorway or that another position is near the end of a corridor. The entorhinal cortex (EC), as the main input to the hippocampus, may play a key role in coding these properties because it contains neurons that activate at multiple related positions per environment. We examined the diversity of spatial coding across the human medial temporal lobe by recording neuronal activity during virtual navigation of an environment containing four similar paths. Neurosurgical patients performed this task as we recorded from implanted microelectrodes, allowing us to compare the human neuronal representation of space with that of animals. EC neurons activated in a repeating manner across the environment, with individual cells spiking at the same relative location across multiple paths. This finding indicates that EC cells represent non-specific information about location relative to an environment's geometry, unlike hippocampal place cells, which activate at particular random locations. Given that spatial navigation is considered to be a model of how the brain supports non-spatial episodic memory, these findings suggest that EC neuronal activity is used by the hippocampus to represent the properties of different memory episodes.


Subject(s)
Entorhinal Cortex/physiology , Neurons/cytology , Space Perception/physiology , Spatial Navigation/physiology , Adult , Entorhinal Cortex/cytology , Environment , Female , Hippocampus/cytology , Hippocampus/physiology , Humans , Male , Neurons/physiology , Neuropsychological Tests , Temporal Lobe/cytology , Temporal Lobe/physiology
7.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 39(3): 773-81, 2013 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22905933

ABSTRACT

In recalling a list of previously experienced items, participants are known to organize their responses on the basis of the items' semantic and temporal similarities. Here, we examine how spatial information influences the organization of responses in free recall. In Experiment 1, participants studied and subsequently recalled lists of landmarks. In Experiment 2, participants played a game in which they delivered objects to landmarks in a virtual environment and later recalled the delivered objects. Participants in both experiments were simply asked to recall as many items as they could remember in any order. By analyzing the conditional probabilities of recall transitions, we demonstrate strong spatial and temporal organization of studied items in both experiments.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Mental Recall/physiology , Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Cluster Analysis , Female , Humans , Male , Probability , Time Factors , Young Adult
8.
Nat Neurosci ; 16(9): 1188-90, 2013 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23912946

ABSTRACT

Grid cells in the entorhinal cortex appear to represent spatial location via a triangular coordinate system. Such cells, which have been identified in rats, bats and monkeys, are believed to support a wide range of spatial behaviors. Recording neuronal activity from neurosurgical patients performing a virtual-navigation task, we identified cells exhibiting grid-like spiking patterns in the human brain, suggesting that humans and simpler animals rely on homologous spatial-coding schemes.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping , Entorhinal Cortex/cytology , Neurons/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Spatial Behavior/physiology , Action Potentials/physiology , Entorhinal Cortex/diagnostic imaging , Entorhinal Cortex/pathology , Epilepsy/pathology , Epilepsy/surgery , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Models, Neurological , Movement , Tomography, X-Ray Computed , User-Computer Interface
9.
Science ; 342(6162): 1111-4, 2013 Nov 29.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24288336

ABSTRACT

In many species, spatial navigation is supported by a network of place cells that exhibit increased firing whenever an animal is in a certain region of an environment. Does this neural representation of location form part of the spatiotemporal context into which episodic memories are encoded? We recorded medial temporal lobe neuronal activity as epilepsy patients performed a hybrid spatial and episodic memory task. We identified place-responsive cells active during virtual navigation and then asked whether the same cells activated during the subsequent recall of navigation-related memories without actual navigation. Place-responsive cell activity was reinstated during episodic memory retrieval. Neuronal firing during the retrieval of each memory was similar to the activity that represented the locations in the environment where the memory was initially encoded.


Subject(s)
Hippocampus/physiology , Memory, Episodic , Neurons/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Cell Separation , Electrodes, Implanted , Epilepsy , Hippocampus/cytology , Humans , Temporal Lobe/cytology , Temporal Lobe/physiology , User-Computer Interface
10.
Neuroimage ; 32(3): 1422-31, 2006 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16814568

ABSTRACT

Both intracranial and scalp EEG studies have demonstrated that oscillatory activity, especially in the gamma band (28 to 100 Hz), can differentiate successful and unsuccessful episodic encoding [Sederberg, P.B., Kahana, M.J., Howard, M.W., Donner, E.J., Madsen, J.R., 2003. Theta and gamma oscillations during encoding predict subsequent recall. Journal of Neuroscience, 23(34), 10809-10814; Fell, J., Klaver, P., Lehnertz, K., Grunwald, T., Schaller, C., Elger, C.E., Fernandez, G., 2001. Human memory formation is accompanied by rhinal-hippocampal coupling and decoupling. Nature Neuroscience, 4 (12), 1259-1264; Gruber, T., Tsivilis, D., Montaldi, D., and Müller, M. (2004). Induced gamma band responses: An early marker of memory encoding and retrieval. Neuroreport, 15, 1837-1841; Summerfield, C., Mangels, J.A., in press. Dissociable neural mechanisms for encoding predictable and unpredictable events. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience]. Although the probability of recalling an item varies as a function of where it appeared in the list, the relation between the oscillatory dynamics of successful encoding and serial position remains unexplored. We recorded scalp EEG as participants studied lists of common nouns in a delayed free-recall task. Because early list items were recalled better than items from later serial positions (the primacy effect), we analyzed encoding-related changes in 2 to 100 Hz oscillatory power as a function of serial position. Increases in gamma power in posterior regions predicted successful encoding at early serial positions; widespread low-frequency (4-14 Hz) power decreases predicted successful memory formation for later serial positions. These results suggest that items in early serial positions receive an encoding boost due to focused encoding without having to divide resources among numerous list items. Later in the list, as memory load increases, encoding is divided between multiple items.


Subject(s)
Electroencephalography , Memory/physiology , Mental Recall/physiology , Adult , Data Interpretation, Statistical , Delta Rhythm , Female , Humans , Male , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Theta Rhythm
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL