Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 13 de 13
Filter
1.
Exp Brain Res ; 236(11): 3031-3041, 2018 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30120498

ABSTRACT

Full-field visual rotation around the vertical axis induces a sense of self-motion (vection), optokinetic nystagmus (OKN), and, eventually, also motion sickness (MS). If the lights are then suddenly switched off, optokinetic afternystagmus (OKAN) occurs. This is due to the discharge of the velocity storage mechanism (VSM), a central integrative network that has been suggested to be involved in motion sickness. We previously showed that visually induced motion sickness (VIMS) following optokinetic stimulation is dependent on vection intensity. To shed light on this relationship, the current study investigated whether vection intensity is related to VSM activity, and thus, to the OKAN. In repetitive trials (eight per condition), 15 stationary participants were exposed to 120 s of visual yaw rotation (60°/s), followed by 90 s in darkness. The visual stimulus either induced strong vection (i.e., scene rotating normally) or weak vection (central and peripheral part moving in opposite directions). Eye movements and subjective vection intensity were continuously measured. Results showed that OKAN occurred less frequently and with lower initial magnitude in the weak-vection condition compared to the strong-vection condition. OKAN decay time constants were not significantly different. The results suggest that the stimuli that produced strong vection also enhanced the charging of the VSM. As VSM activity presumably is a factor in motion sickness, the enhanced VSM activity in our strong-vection condition hints at an involvement of the VSM in VIMS, and could explain why visual stimuli producing a strong sense of vection also elicit high levels of VIMS.


Subject(s)
Motion Sickness/physiopathology , Nystagmus, Optokinetic/physiology , Rotation , Vision, Ocular/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Visual Fields/physiology , Young Adult
2.
Exp Brain Res ; 234(8): 2323-37, 2016 08.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27056085

ABSTRACT

When in darkness, humans can perceive the direction and magnitude of rotations and of linear translations in the horizontal plane. The current paper addresses the integrated perception of combined translational and rotational motion, as it occurs when moving along a curved trajectory. We questioned whether the perceived motion through the environment follows the predictions of a self-motion perception model (e.g., Merfeld et al. in J Vestib Res 3:141-161, 1993; Newman in A multisensory observer model for human spatial orientation perception, 2009), which assume linear addition of rotational and translational components. For curved motion in darkness, such models predict a non-veridical motion percept, consisting of an underestimation of the perceived rotation, a distortion of the perceived travelled path, and a bias in the perceived heading (i.e., the perceived instantaneous direction of motion with respect to the body). These model predictions were evaluated in two experiments. In Experiment 1, seven participants were moved along a circular trajectory in darkness while facing the motion direction. They indicated perceived yaw rotation using an online tracking task, and perceived travelled path by drawings. In Experiment 2, the heading was systematically varied, and six participants indicated, in a 2-alternative forced-choice task, whether they perceived facing inward or outward of the circular path. Overall, we found no evidence for the heading bias predicted by the model. This suggests that the sum of the perceived rotational and translational components alone cannot adequately explain the overall perceived motion through the environment. Possibly, knowledge about motion dynamics and familiar stimuli combinations may play an important additional role in shaping the percept.


Subject(s)
Motion Perception/physiology , Proprioception/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Young Adult
3.
Aviat Space Environ Med ; 82(5): 505-12, 2011 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21614863

ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION: Spatial disorientation (SD) is still a contributing factor in many aviation accidents, stressing the need for adequate SD training scenarios. In this article we focused on the post-roll effect (the sensation of rolling back after a roll maneuver, such as an entry of a coordinated turn) and investigated the effect of roll stimuli on the pilot's ability to stabilize their roll attitude. This resulted in a ground-based demonstration scenario for pilots. METHODS: The experiments took place in the advanced 6-DOF Desdemona motion simulator, with the subject in a supine position. Roll motions were either fully automated with the subjects blindfolded (BLIND), automated with the subject viewing the cockpit interior (COCKPIT), or self-controlled (LEAD). After the roll stimulus subjects had to cancel all perceived simulator motion without any visual feedback. Both the roll velocity and duration were varied. RESULTS: In 68% of all trials subjects corrected for the perceived motion of rolling back by initiating a roll motion in the same direction as the preceeding roll. The effect was dependent on both rate and duration, in a manner consistent with semicircular canal dynamics. The effect was smallest in the BLIND scenario, but differences between simulation scenarios were non-significant. DISCUSSION: The results show that the effects of the post-roll illusion on aircraft control can be demonstrated adequately in a flight simulator using an attitude control task. The effect is present even after short roll movements, occurring frequently in flight. Therefore this demonstration is relevant for spatial disorientation training programs for pilots.


Subject(s)
Aerospace Medicine , Space Perception , Accidents, Aviation , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Humans , Illusions , Rotation , Semicircular Canals/physiology , Supine Position/physiology
4.
PLoS One ; 16(1): e0245295, 2021.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33465124

ABSTRACT

Illusory self-motion often provokes motion sickness, which is commonly explained in terms of an inter-sensory conflict that is not in accordance with previous experience. Here we address the influence of cognition in motion sickness and show that such a conflict is not provocative when the observer believes that the motion illusion is indeed actually occurring. Illusory self-motion and motion sickness were elicited in healthy human participants who were seated on a stationary rotary chair inside a rotating optokinetic drum. Participants knew that both chair and drum could rotate but were unaware of the actual motion stimulus. Results showed that motion sickness was correlated with the discrepancy between participants' perceived self-motion and participants' beliefs about the actual motion. Together with the general motion sickness susceptibility, this discrepancy accounted for 51% of the variance in motion sickness intensity. This finding sheds a new light on the causes of visually induced motion sickness and suggests that it is not governed by an inter-sensory conflict per se, but by beliefs concerning the actual self-motion. This cognitive influence provides a promising tool for the development of new countermeasures.


Subject(s)
Motion Perception/physiology , Motion Sickness/physiopathology , Adult , Cognition/physiology , Female , Healthy Volunteers , Humans , Likelihood Functions , Male , Visual Fields , Young Adult
5.
Appl Ergon ; 90: 103282, 2021 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33065467

ABSTRACT

The risk of motion sickness is considerably higher in autonomous vehicles than it is in human-operated vehicles. Their introduction will therefore require systems that mitigate motion sickness. We investigated whether this can be achieved by augmenting the vehicle interior with additional visualizations. Participants were immersed in motion simulations on a moving-base driving simulator, where they were backward-facing passengers of an autonomous vehicle. Using a Head-Mounted Display, they were presented either with a regular view from inside the vehicle, or with augmented views that offered additional cues on the vehicle's present motion or motion 500ms into the future, displayed on the vehicle's interior panels. In contrast to the hypotheses and other recent studies, no difference was found between conditions. The absence of differences between conditions suggests a ceiling effect: providing a regular view may limit motion sickness, but presentation of additional visual information beyond this does not further reduce sickness.


Subject(s)
Automobile Driving , Motion Sickness , Cues , Forecasting , Humans , Motion , Motion Sickness/etiology , Motion Sickness/prevention & control
6.
Exp Brain Res ; 190(2): 165-77, 2008 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18566806

ABSTRACT

Prolonged exposure to hypergravity in a human centrifuge can lead to post-rotary spatial disorientation and motion sickness. These symptoms are mainly provoked by tilting head movements and resemble the Space Adaptation Syndrome. We hypothesized that the occurrence of these post-rotary effects might be related to changes in the velocity storage (VS) mechanism, which is suggested to play an important role in spatial orientation. In particular, we investigated whether the re-orientation of the eye velocity vector (EVV) towards gravity during off-vertical optokinetic stimulation was affected by centrifugation. Twelve human subjects were exposed to a hypergravity load of 3G (G-load directed along the naso-occipetal axis) for a duration of 90 min. Before and after centrifugation we recorded optokinetic nystagmus (OKN) elicited by a stimulus pattern moving about the subject's yaw axis, with the head erect and tilted 45 degrees to both sides. During OKN with the head erect, we observed a pitch-down component, reorienting the EVV on average 4.5 degrees (SD 3.6, pretest values) away from the stimulus axis. Head tilt induced an additional shift of the EVV towards the spatial vertical of 6.4 degrees on average (SD 3.2). This head-tilt induced reorientation was significantly decreased after centrifugation to 4.7 degrees (SD 2.9), suggesting a reduction of VS-activity. By means of a vector model we estimated the reduction in VS-activity at 31%. Such a decrease in VS-activity might reflect a deterioration of the ability to integrate sensory signals to obtain an estimate of gravity during tilting head movements, resulting in motion sickness in susceptible subjects.


Subject(s)
Confusion/physiopathology , Hypergravity/adverse effects , Motion Sickness/physiopathology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Space Motion Sickness/physiopathology , Space Perception/physiology , Adult , Centrifugation/adverse effects , Confusion/etiology , Eye Movements/physiology , Head Movements/physiology , Humans , Male , Motion Sickness/etiology , Nystagmus, Optokinetic/physiology , Orientation/physiology , Postural Balance/physiology , Space Motion Sickness/etiology , Vestibule, Labyrinth/physiology , Vestibule, Labyrinth/physiopathology
7.
J Vestib Res ; 18(2-3): 97-105, 2008.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19126980

ABSTRACT

Adaptation to a novel gravitational state involves adaptation of vestibular mediated responses, in particular those mediated by the otolith organs. The present paper investigates whether the orientation of Listing's plane, which is under control of otolith signals, is affected by sustained exposure to hypergravity. Subjects were exposed to four G-loads differing in duration (45 or 90 min) and magnitude (2 or 3G). During centrifugation subjects were in a supine position, directing the gravito-inertial acceleration along the naso-occipetal axis. We determined the orientation of Listing's plane before and after each centrifuge run, with the head erect and tilted in pitch. Head tilt in pitch induced a counter-pitch of Listing's plane, which was found to be less pronounced after centrifugation. In addition, exposure to 3G for 90 min induced a small backward tilt of Listing's plane compared to the pretest orientation (head erect). In order to explain these results a hypothesis is discussed, proposing that the orientation of Listing's plane in the head is governed by a head fixed orientation vector that is modulated by the direction of gravity relative to the head. Sustained centrifugation is proposed to decrease this gravitational modulation, leading to the effects observed. This could reflect a shift towards a more body centered frame of reference.


Subject(s)
Eye Movements/physiology , Hypergravity , Orientation/physiology , Proprioception/physiology , Reflex, Vestibulo-Ocular/physiology , Adaptation, Physiological , Adult , Aerospace Medicine , Humans , Male , Otolithic Membrane/physiology , Reference Values , Vestibule, Labyrinth/physiology , Young Adult
8.
J Vestib Res ; 17(5-6): 323-32, 2007.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18626142

ABSTRACT

It has been found that sustained centrifugation on Earth may evoke sickness symptoms that are similar to those of the Space Adaptation Syndrome (SAS). As in SAS, incidence of this 'Sickness Induced by Centrifugation' (SIC) is about 50% and the symptoms are particularly evoked by head movements. By systematically varying the G-load and duration of centrifugation, the current study investigated the characteristics of the gravitational stimulus that is required for SIC to occur. Subjects were exposed to centrifugation at 2 and 3Gx, for a duration of 45 and 90 minutes. A standardized head movement protocol was used to evoke SIC after centrifugation. The results show that in six out of 12 subjects (50%) no serious symptoms were elicited. In the remaining subjects, the effects of the 3G runs exceeded those of the 2G runs, and within each G-level symptom intensity was higher for the 90 min than for the 45 min exposure. An exponential fit on this data showed that the time constant of adaptation to the gravitational stimulus was about 60 minutes. This suggests that short duration exposures (i.e. <60 min) are not likely to induce serious SIC.


Subject(s)
Head Movements/physiology , Hypergravity/adverse effects , Space Motion Sickness/physiopathology , Adult , Centrifugation , Humans , Male
9.
Aviat Space Environ Med ; 78(5): 505-9, 2007 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17539445

ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION: In motion sickness desensitization programs, the motion sickness provocative stimulus is often a forward bending of the trunk on a rotating chair, inducing Coriolis effects. Since respiratory relaxation techniques are applied successfully in these courses, we investigated whether these repetitive trunk movements by themselves may induce hyperventilation and consequently add to the motion sickness. METHODS: There were 12 healthy subjects who participated in our study. In the Baseline condition, subjects sat relaxed on the stationary chair. In the Hypervent condition, subjects performed voluntary hyperventilation (the level was prescribed). In two other conditions subjects rhythmically bent their trunk on a stationary chair (Tilt-Stat condition) and on a rotating chair (Tilt-Rot condition). In all conditions we measured respiratory and cardiovascular activity (heart frequency, tidal volume, end-tidal CO2, and respiration frequency). RESULTS: Of the 12 subjects, 9 had to stop prematurely in the Tilt-Rot condition because of moderate nausea. Except for heart rate in the Tilt-Rot condition, the measured physiological parameters in these subjects in the Tilt-Stat and Tilt-Rot conditions were not statistically different from the Baseline condition. Only in the Hypervent condition were significant differences observed, but no nausea. DISCUSSION: The findings show that hyperventilation is not the main cause of nausea during the Coriolis effects. We conclude that during the pilot desensitization program with Coriolis stimuli, measurement of cardiovascular and respiratory parameters is not necessary; however, in those cases that do not respond to the intervention, we recommend paying attention to respiratory parameters because hyperventilation does occur on an individual basis.


Subject(s)
Alkalosis, Respiratory/etiology , Coriolis Force , Hyperventilation/complications , Motion Sickness/etiology , Nausea/etiology , Vestibular Diseases , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Posture , Prospective Studies , Rotation
10.
PLoS One ; 12(4): e0175305, 2017.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28380077

ABSTRACT

This study investigated the role of vection (i.e., a visually induced sense of self-motion), optokinetic nystagmus (OKN), and inadvertent head movements in visually induced motion sickness (VIMS), evoked by yaw rotation of the visual surround. These three elements have all been proposed as contributing factors in VIMS, as they can be linked to different motion sickness theories. However, a full understanding of the role of each factor is still lacking because independent manipulation has proven difficult in the past. We adopted an integrative approach to the problem by obtaining measures of potentially relevant parameters in four experimental conditions and subsequently combining them in a linear mixed regression model. To that end, participants were exposed to visual yaw rotation in four separate sessions. Using a full factorial design, the OKN was manipulated by a fixation target (present/absent), and vection strength by introducing a conflict in the motion direction of the central and peripheral field of view (present/absent). In all conditions, head movements were minimized as much as possible. Measured parameters included vection strength, vection variability, OKN slow phase velocity, OKN frequency, the number of inadvertent head movements, and inadvertent head tilt. Results show that VIMS increases with vection strength, but that this relation varies among participants (R2 = 0.48). Regression parameters for vection variability, head and eye movement parameters were not significant. These results may seem to be in line with the Sensory Conflict theory on motion sickness, but we argue that a more detailed definition of the exact nature of the conflict is required to fully appreciate the relationship between vection and VIMS.


Subject(s)
Eye Movements/physiology , Motion Sickness/etiology , Adult , Female , Head Movements/physiology , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Models, Biological , Motion , Motion Sickness/physiopathology , Nystagmus, Optokinetic/physiology , Rotation/adverse effects , Visual Perception/physiology , Young Adult
12.
J Mot Behav ; 36(4): 425-41, 2004 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15695231

ABSTRACT

The shifts in relative phase that are observed when rhythmically coordinated limbs are submitted to asymmetric mass perturbations have typically been attributed to the induced eigenfrequency difference (delta omega) between limbs. Modeling the moving limbs as forced linear oscillators, however, reveals that asymmetric mass perturbations may induce a difference not only in eigenfrequency (i.e., delta omega not equal 0) but also in the covarying low-frequency control gains (i.e., delta k not equal 0). Because the inverse of the low-frequency control gain (k) reflects the level of muscular torque (input) required for a particular displacement from equilibrium (output), asymmetric mass perturbations may result in an imbalance in the muscular torques required for task performance (related to delta k not equal 0). Thus, it is possible that the effects attributed to delta omega were in fact mediated by delta k. In 2 experiments, the authors manipulated delta k and delta omega separately by applying mass perturbations to the lower legs of 9 participants. The relative phasing between the legs was not affected by delta k, but manipulation of delta omega (while delta k remained approximately 0) induced systematic relative phase shifts that were more pronounced for antiphase than for in-phase coordination. That indication that the coordination dynamics is indeed influenced by an imbalance in eigenfrequency is discussed vis-a-vis the question of how such a merely peripheral property may affect the underlying coordination process.


Subject(s)
Extremities/physiology , Movement/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Humans , Models, Biological
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL