Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 24
Filter
1.
Mol Psychiatry ; 28(9): 3688-3697, 2023 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37903876

ABSTRACT

Psychotic experiences (PEs) occur in 5-10% of the general population and are associated with exposure to childhood trauma and obstetric complications. However, the neurobiological mechanisms underlying these associations are unclear. Using the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC), we studied 138 young people aged 20 with PEs (n = 49 suspected, n = 53 definite, n = 36 psychotic disorder) and 275 controls. Voxel-based morphometry assessed whether MRI measures of grey matter volume were associated with (i) PEs, (ii) cumulative childhood psychological trauma (weighted summary score of 6 trauma types), (iii) cumulative pre/peri-natal risk factors for psychosis (weighted summary score of 16 risk factors), and (iv) the interaction between PEs and cumulative trauma or pre/peri-natal risk. PEs were associated with smaller left posterior cingulate (pFWE < 0.001, Z = 4.19) and thalamus volumes (pFWE = 0.006, Z = 3.91). Cumulative pre/perinatal risk was associated with smaller left subgenual cingulate volume (pFWE < 0.001, Z = 4.54). A significant interaction between PEs and cumulative pre/perinatal risk found larger striatum (pFWE = 0.04, Z = 3.89) and smaller right insula volume extending into the supramarginal gyrus and superior temporal gyrus (pFWE = 0.002, Z = 4.79), specifically in those with definite PEs and psychotic disorder. Cumulative childhood trauma was associated with larger left dorsal striatum (pFWE = 0.002, Z = 3.65), right prefrontal cortex (pFWE < 0.001, Z = 4.63) and smaller left insula volume in all participants (pFWE = 0.03, Z = 3.60), and there was no interaction with PEs group. In summary, pre/peri-natal risk factors and childhood psychological trauma impact similar brain pathways, namely smaller insula and larger striatum volumes. The effect of pre/perinatal risk was greatest in those with more severe PEs, whereas effects of trauma were seen in all participants. In conclusion, environmental risk factors affect brain networks implicated in schizophrenia, which may increase an individual's propensity to develop later psychotic disorders.


Subject(s)
Adverse Childhood Experiences , Psychotic Disorders , Schizophrenia , Child , Humans , Adolescent , Longitudinal Studies , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Brain
2.
Neuroimage ; 179: 102-116, 2018 10 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29902586

ABSTRACT

In navigating our environment, we rapidly process and extract meaning from visual cues. However, the relationship between visual features and categorical representations in natural scene perception is still not well understood. Here, we used natural scene stimuli from different categories and filtered at different spatial frequencies to address this question in a passive viewing paradigm. Using representational similarity analysis (RSA) and cross-decoding of magnetoencephalography (MEG) data, we show that categorical representations emerge in human visual cortex at ∼180 ms and are linked to spatial frequency processing. Furthermore, dorsal and ventral stream areas reveal temporally and spatially overlapping representations of low and high-level layer activations extracted from a feedforward neural network. Our results suggest that neural patterns from extrastriate visual cortex switch from low-level to categorical representations within 200 ms, highlighting the rapid cascade of processing stages essential in human visual perception.


Subject(s)
Nerve Net/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Signal Processing, Computer-Assisted , Visual Cortex/physiology , Adult , Brain Mapping/methods , Female , Humans , Magnetoencephalography , Male , Photic Stimulation
3.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 39(10): 3993-4006, 2018 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29885055

ABSTRACT

Recognizing emotion in faces is important in human interaction and survival, yet existing studies do not paint a consistent picture of the neural representation supporting this task. To address this, we collected magnetoencephalography (MEG) data while participants passively viewed happy, angry and neutral faces. Using time-resolved decoding of sensor-level data, we show that responses to angry faces can be discriminated from happy and neutral faces as early as 90 ms after stimulus onset and only 10 ms later than faces can be discriminated from scrambled stimuli, even in the absence of differences in evoked responses. Time-resolved relevance patterns in source space track expression-related information from the visual cortex (100 ms) to higher-level temporal and frontal areas (200-500 ms). Together, our results point to a system optimised for rapid processing of emotional faces and preferentially tuned to threat, consistent with the important evolutionary role that such a system must have played in the development of human social interactions.


Subject(s)
Emotions/physiology , Facial Expression , Facial Recognition/physiology , Functional Neuroimaging/methods , Magnetoencephalography/methods , Social Perception , Visual Cortex/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Spatio-Temporal Analysis , Young Adult
4.
Neuroimage ; 159: 302-324, 2017 10 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28735011

ABSTRACT

Magnetoencephalography (MEG) is increasingly being used to study brain function because of its excellent temporal resolution and its direct association with brain activity at the neuronal level. One possible cause of error in the analysis of MEG data comes from the fact that participants, even MEG-experienced ones, move their head in the MEG system. Head movement can cause source localization errors during the analysis of MEG data, which can result in the appearance of source variability that does not reflect brain activity. The MEG community places great importance in eliminating this source of possible errors as is evident, for example, by recent efforts to develop head casts that limit head movement in the MEG system. In this work we use software tools to identify, assess and eliminate from the analysis of MEG data any possible correlations between head movement in the MEG system and widely-used measures of brain activity derived from MEG resting-state recordings. The measures of brain activity we study are a) the Hilbert-transform derived amplitude envelope of the beamformer time series and b) functional networks; both measures derived by MEG resting-state recordings. Ten-minute MEG resting-state recordings were performed on healthy participants, with head position continuously recorded. The sources of the measured magnetic signals were localized via beamformer spatial filtering. Temporal independent component analysis was subsequently used to derive resting-state networks. Significant correlations were observed between the beamformer envelope time series and head movement. The correlations were substantially reduced, and in some cases eliminated, after a participant-specific temporal high-pass filter was applied to those time series. Regressing the head movement metrics out of the beamformer envelope time series had an even stronger effect in reducing these correlations. Correlation trends were also observed between head movement and the activation time series of the default-mode and frontal networks. Regressing the head movement metrics out of the beamformer envelope time series completely eliminated these correlations. Additionally, applying the head movement correction resulted in changes in the network spatial maps for the visual and sensorimotor networks. Our results a) show that the results of MEG resting-state studies that use the above-mentioned analysis methods are confounded by head movement effects, b) suggest that regressing the head movement metrics out of the beamformer envelope time series is a necessary step to be added to these analyses, in order to eliminate the effect that head movement has on the amplitude envelope of beamformer time series and the network time series and c) highlight changes in the connectivity spatial maps when head movement correction is applied.


Subject(s)
Artifacts , Brain/physiology , Head Movements , Magnetoencephalography/methods , Signal Processing, Computer-Assisted , Adult , Female , Humans , Male
5.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 37(12): 4597-4614, 2016 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27593199

ABSTRACT

This article describes the first application of a generic (empirical) Bayesian analysis of between-subject effects in the dynamic causal modeling (DCM) of electrophysiological (MEG) data. It shows that (i) non-invasive (MEG) data can be used to characterize subject-specific differences in cortical microcircuitry and (ii) presents a validation of DCM with neural fields that exploits intersubject variability in gamma oscillations. We find that intersubject variability in visually induced gamma responses reflects changes in the excitation-inhibition balance in a canonical cortical circuit. Crucially, this variability can be explained by subject-specific differences in intrinsic connections to and from inhibitory interneurons that form a pyramidal-interneuron gamma network. Our approach uses Bayesian model reduction to evaluate the evidence for (large sets of) nested models-and optimize the corresponding connectivity estimates at the within and between-subject level. We also consider Bayesian cross-validation to obtain predictive estimates for gamma-response phenotypes, using a leave-one-out procedure. Hum Brain Mapp 37:4597-4614, 2016. © The Authors Human Brain Mapping Published by Wiley Periodicals, Inc.


Subject(s)
Gamma Rhythm/physiology , Magnetoencephalography , Signal Processing, Computer-Assisted , Visual Cortex/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Bayes Theorem , Female , Humans , Magnetoencephalography/methods , Male , Models, Neurological , Young Adult
6.
Eur J Neurosci ; 41(11): 1484-95, 2015 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25919220

ABSTRACT

Electrophysiological recordings in primates indicate that visual gamma contains distinct broad- and narrowband components that reflect different neuronal processes. Evidence suggests that cross-orientation masking of luminance-defined gratings should differentially modulate these two components. To test this we measured the effect of cross-orientation masking on the gamma response in 12 human participants using magentoencephalography (MEG). Although both the amplitude and the frequency of gamma were modulated by the presence of a cross-orientation mask, we failed to find evidence for distinguishable components: both broadband gamma at stimulus onset and sustained narrowband gamma were similarly modulated by mask contrast. However, we could not confirm the presence of masking effects due to mask contrast being confounded with the contrast of the stimulus as a whole. We therefore tested a further 12 participants in a second experiment in which the stimuli were: a plaid stimulus, the two component gratings which formed the plaid and the same two gratings but with Michelson contrast matched to the plaid. We found that gamma amplitude was reduced and gamma frequency increased to the plaid stimulus when compared with the contrast-matched gratings or with the sum of the two component gratings, indicating that visual gamma was indeed modulated by cross-orientation masking. Surprisingly, masking did not affect the pattern-onset evoked response, challenging previous hypotheses that cross-orientation suppression - the phenomenon by which the response to an orientated grating is suppressed by a cross-orientation mask - is driven by feedforward inputs to V1.


Subject(s)
Evoked Potentials, Visual , Gamma Rhythm , Visual Cortex/physiology , Adult , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Female , Humans , Magnetoencephalography , Male , Perceptual Masking/physiology , Young Adult
7.
J Anat ; 227(4): 409-17, 2015 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26352409

ABSTRACT

Neuronal oscillations in the gamma frequency range play an important role in stimulus processing in the brain. The frequency of these oscillations can vary widely between participants and is strongly genetically determined, but the cause of this variability is not understood. Previous studies have reported correlations between individual differences in gamma frequency and the concentration of the inhibitory neurotransmitter, gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA), as well as with age and primary visual cortex (V1) area and thickness. This study assessed the relationships between all of these variables in the same group of participants. There were no significant correlations between gamma frequency and GABA+ concentration, V1 area or V1 thickness, although the relationship with GABA+/Cr approached significance. Considering age as a covariate further reduced the strength of all correlations and, in an additional dataset with a larger age range, gamma frequency was strongly inversely correlated with age but not V1 thickness or area, suggesting that age modulates gamma frequency via an additional, as yet unknown, mechanism. Consistent with other recent studies, these findings do not demonstrate a clear relationship between gamma frequency and GABA+ concentration. Further investigation of additional variables and the interactions between them will be necessary in order to more accurately determine predictors of the frequency of gamma oscillations.


Subject(s)
Visual Cortex/anatomy & histology , Visual Cortex/physiology , gamma-Aminobutyric Acid/metabolism , Adult , Electroencephalography , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Magnetoencephalography , Male , Photic Stimulation
8.
Eur J Neurosci ; 39(9): 1517-27, 2014 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24617643

ABSTRACT

A rich pattern of responses in frequency, time and space are known to be generated in the visual cortex in response to faces. Recently, a number of studies have used magnetoencephalography (MEG) to try to record these responses non-invasively - in many cases using source analysis techniques based on the beamforming method. Here we sought both to characterize best practice for measuring face-specific responses using MEG beamforming, and to determine whether the results produced by the beamformer match evidence from other modalities. We measured activity to visual presentation of face stimuli and phase-scrambled control stimuli, and performed source analyses of both induced and evoked responses using Synthetic Aperture Magnetometry. We localized the gamma-band response to bilateral lateral occipital cortex, and both the gamma-band response and the M170-evoked response to the right fusiform gyrus. Differences in the gamma-band response between faces and scrambled stimuli were confined to the frequency range 50-90 Hz; gamma-band activity at higher frequencies did not differ between the two stimulus categories. We additionally identified a component of the M220-evoked response - localized to the parieto-occipital sulcus - which was enhanced for scrambled vs. unscrambled faces. These findings help to establish that MEG beamforming can localize face-specific responses in time, frequency and space with good accuracy (when validated against established findings from functional magnetic resonance imaging and intracranial recordings), as well as contributing to the establishment of best methodological practice for the use of the beamformer method to measure face-specific responses.


Subject(s)
Evoked Potentials, Visual , Gamma Rhythm , Magnetoencephalography/methods , Visual Cortex/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Face , Female , Humans , Male , Young Adult
9.
Neuroimage ; 68: 83-92, 2013 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23220427

ABSTRACT

The role of gamma-band (typically 30-100 Hz) oscillations in visual processing is a topic of increasing interest. One hypothesis is that gamma oscillations reflect the action of GABAergic inhibitory processes in the visual cortex responsible for surround-suppression. Evidence from primate neurophysiology [Gieselmann & Thiele, A., 2008. European Journal of Neuroscience 28, 447-459.] suggests that the amplitude of the gamma-band response increases as a visual grating stimulus expands outside of the classical receptive field into the inhibitory surround; with the amplitude of the response increasing, and the frequency of the response decreasing, monotonically with stimulus size. In this study, we tested the relationship between the gamma-band response and the size of visual grating stimuli in humans using MEG. In two initial experiments we found that, while the absolute magnitude of the gamma-band response varied considerably across participants, in all cases the amplitude of the response had a monotonically increasing relationship with size. In contrast, we did not find any relationship between the frequency of the response and the size of the stimulus. Previously, the frequency of the visual gamma-band response has been found to correlate across individuals with the surface area of cortical area V1 [Schwarzkopf et al., 2012. Journal of Neuroscience 32, 1507-12.] We, however, were unable to find any correlation between the frequency or the magnitude of the gamma-band response and the dimensions of V1 cortical gray matter as measured from participants' MR images. Consistent with a saturation of the gamma-band response found for some individuals in the first two experiments, in a third experiment we found that the magnitude of the response to our largest stimulus (8°) was less than that predicted from the response to the stimulus' parts.


Subject(s)
Photic Stimulation/methods , Visual Cortex/physiology , Adult , Evoked Potentials, Visual , Female , Humans , Magnetoencephalography , Male , Signal Processing, Computer-Assisted , Young Adult
10.
Schizophr Bull ; 48(2): 524-532, 2022 03 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34662406

ABSTRACT

Schizophrenia (SCZ) is associated with structural brain changes, with considerable variation in the extent to which these cortical regions are influenced. We present a novel metric that summarises individual structural variation across the brain, while considering prior effect sizes, established via meta-analysis. We determine individual participant deviation from a within-sample-norm across structural MRI regions of interest (ROIs). For each participant, we weight the normalised deviation of each ROI by the effect size (Cohen's d) of the difference between SCZ/control for the corresponding ROI from the SCZ Enhancing Neuroimaging Genomics through Meta-Analysis working group. We generate a morphometric risk score (MRS) representing the average of these weighted deviations. We investigate if SCZ-MRS is elevated in a SCZ case/control sample (NCASE = 50; NCONTROL = 125), a replication sample (NCASE = 23; NCONTROL = 20) and a sample of asymptomatic young adults with extreme SCZ polygenic risk (NHIGH-SCZ-PRS = 95; NLOW-SCZ-PRS = 94). SCZ cases had higher SCZ-MRS than healthy controls in both samples (Study 1: ß = 0.62, P < 0.001; Study 2: ß = 0.81, P = 0.018). The high liability SCZ-PRS group also had a higher SCZ-MRS (Study 3: ß = 0.29, P = 0.044). Furthermore, the SCZ-MRS was uniquely associated with SCZ status, but not attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), whereas an ADHD-MRS was linked to ADHD status, but not SCZ. This approach provides a promising solution when considering individual heterogeneity in SCZ-related brain alterations by identifying individual's patterns of structural brain-wide alterations.


Subject(s)
Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Schizophrenia/physiopathology , Adult , Case-Control Studies , Female , Genetic Predisposition to Disease , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/statistics & numerical data , Male , Middle Aged , Neuroimaging/methods , Neuroimaging/statistics & numerical data , Schizophrenia/complications
11.
Eur J Neurosci ; 34(4): 652-61, 2011 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21749494

ABSTRACT

Magnetoencephalography (MEG) can be used to reconstruct neuronal activity with high spatial and temporal resolution. However, this reconstruction problem is ill-posed, and requires the use of prior constraints in order to produce a unique solution. At present there are a multitude of inversion algorithms, each employing different assumptions, but one major problem when comparing the accuracy of these different approaches is that often the true underlying electrical state of the brain is unknown. In this study, we explore one paradigm, retinotopic mapping in the primary visual cortex (V1), for which the ground truth is known to a reasonable degree of accuracy, enabling the comparison of MEG source reconstructions with the true electrical state of the brain. Specifically, we attempted to localize, using a beanforming method, the induced responses in the visual cortex generated by a high contrast, retinotopically varying stimulus. Although well described in primate studies, it has been an open question whether the induced gamma power in humans due to high contrast gratings derives from V1 rather than the prestriate cortex (V2). We show that the beanformer source estimate in the gamma and theta bands does vary in a manner consistent with the known retinotopy of V1. However, these peak locations, although retinotopically organized, did not accurately localize to the cortical surface. We considered possible causes for this discrepancy and suggest that improved MEG/magnetic resonance imaging co-registration and the use of more accurate source models that take into account the spatial extent and shape of the active cortex may, in future, improve the accuracy of the source reconstructions.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping/methods , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted/methods , Visual Cortex/physiology , Algorithms , Humans , Magnetoencephalography , Photic Stimulation
12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33524599

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Altered functional brain connectivity has been proposed as an intermediate phenotype between genetic risk loci and clinical expression of schizophrenia. Genetic high-risk groups of healthy subjects are particularly suited for the investigation of this proposition because they can be tested in the absence of medication or other secondary effects of schizophrenia. METHODS: Here, we applied dynamic functional connectivity analysis to functional magnetic resonance imaging data to reveal the reconfiguration of brain networks during a cognitive task. We recruited healthy carriers of common risk variants using the recall-by-genotype design. We assessed 197 individuals: 99 individuals (52 female, 47 male) with low polygenic risk scores (schizophrenia risk profile scores [SCZ-PRSs]) and 98 individuals (52 female, 46 male) with high SCZ-PRSs from both tails of the SCZ-PRS distribution from a genotyped population cohort, the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (N = 8169). We compared groups both on conventional brain activation profiles, using the general linear model of the experiment, and on the neural flexibility index, which quantifies how frequent a brain region's community affiliation changes over experimental time. RESULTS: Behavioral performance and standard brain activation profiles did not differ significantly between groups. High SCZ-PRS was associated with reduced flexibility index and network modularity across n-back levels. The whole-brain flexibility index and that of the frontoparietal working memory network was associated with n-back performance. We identified a dynamic network phenotype related to high SCZ-PRS. CONCLUSIONS: Such neurophysiological markers can become important for the elucidation of biological mechanisms of schizophrenia and, particularly, the associated cognitive deficit.


Subject(s)
Schizophrenia , Brain , Female , Genetic Predisposition to Disease , Humans , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Memory, Short-Term/physiology
13.
PLoS One ; 15(12): e0243237, 2020.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33332389

ABSTRACT

It has recently been demonstrated through invasive electrophysiology that visual stimulation with extended patches of uniform colour generates pronounced gamma oscillations in the visual cortex of both macaques and humans. In this study we sought to discover if this oscillatory response to colour can be measured non-invasively in humans using magnetoencephalography. We were able to demonstrate increased gamma (40-70 Hz) power in response to full-screen stimulation with four different colour hues and found that the gamma response is particularly strong for long wavelength (i.e. red) stimulation, as was found in previous studies. However, we also found that gamma power in response to colour was generally weaker than the response to an identically sized luminance-defined grating. We also observed two additional responses in the gamma frequency: a lower frequency response around 25-35 Hz that showed fewer clear differences between conditions than the gamma response, and a higher frequency response around 70-100 Hz that was present for red stimulation but not for other colours. In a second experiment we sought to test whether differences in the gamma response between colour hues could be explained by their chromatic separation from the preceding display. We presented stimuli that alternated between each of the three pairings of the three primary colours (red, green, blue) at two levels of chromatic separation defined in the CIELUV colour space. We observed that the gamma response was significantly greater to high relative to low chromatic separation, but that at each level of separation the response was greater for both red-blue and red-green than for blue-green stimulation. Our findings suggest that the stronger gamma response to red stimulation cannot be wholly explained by the chromatic separation of the stimuli.


Subject(s)
Color Perception , Photic Stimulation , Visual Cortex/physiology , Adult , Color , Female , Humans , Magnetoencephalography , Male , Young Adult
14.
Transl Psychiatry ; 10(1): 324, 2020 09 21.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32958742

ABSTRACT

Rare copy number variants associated with increased risk for neurodevelopmental and psychiatric disorders (referred to as ND-CNVs) are characterized by heterogeneous phenotypes thought to share a considerable degree of overlap. Altered neural integration has often been linked to psychopathology and is a candidate marker for potential convergent mechanisms through which ND-CNVs modify risk; however, the rarity of ND-CNVs means that few studies have assessed their neural correlates. Here, we used magnetoencephalography (MEG) to investigate resting-state oscillatory connectivity in a cohort of 42 adults with ND-CNVs, including deletions or duplications at 22q11.2, 15q11.2, 15q13.3, 16p11.2, 17q12, 1q21.1, 3q29, and 2p16.3, and 42 controls. We observed decreased connectivity between occipital, temporal, and parietal areas in participants with ND-CNVs. This pattern was common across genotypes and not exclusively characteristic of 22q11.2 deletions, which were present in a third of our cohort. Furthermore, a data-driven graph theory framework enabled us to successfully distinguish participants with ND-CNVs from unaffected controls using differences in node centrality and network segregation. Together, our results point to alterations in electrophysiological connectivity as a putative common mechanism through which genetic factors confer increased risk for neurodevelopmental and psychiatric disorders.


Subject(s)
DNA Copy Number Variations , Mental Disorders , Adult , Cohort Studies , Genetic Predisposition to Disease , Genotype , Humans , Phenotype
15.
Wellcome Open Res ; 5: 203, 2020.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33043145

ABSTRACT

Neuroimaging offers a valuable insight into human brain development by allowing in vivo assessment of structure, connectivity and function. Multimodal neuroimaging data have been obtained as part of three sub-studies within the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children, a prospective multigenerational pregnancy and birth cohort based in the United Kingdom. Brain imaging data were acquired when offspring were between 18 and 24 years of age, and included acquisition of structural, functional and magnetization transfer magnetic resonance, diffusion tensor, and magnetoencephalography imaging. This resource provides a unique opportunity to combine neuroimaging data with extensive phenotypic and genotypic measures from participants, their mothers, and fathers.

16.
Exp Eye Res ; 88(3): 483-94, 2009 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19041865

ABSTRACT

The mechanism by which the eye dynamically changes focal distance (accommodation), and the mechanism by which this ability is lost with age (presbyopia), are still contested. Due to inherent confounding factors in vivo, in vitro measurements have been undertaken using a robotic lens stretcher to examine these mechanisms as well as the efficacy of lens refilling - a proposed treatment for presbyopia. Dynamic forces, anterior and posterior curvatures, and lens thickness are all correlated for young natural and refilled porcine lenses. Comparisons are made to lenses refilled with a homogeneous polymer system. The amplitude of accommodation of the young porcine lens is very small such that it may be a suitable model for presbyopia. The behavior of refilled lenses was highly dependent on the refill volume. The volume could be tuned to maximize accommodative amplitude in the refilled lens.


Subject(s)
Lens, Crystalline/physiology , Presbyopia/physiopathology , Accommodation, Ocular , Animals , Elasticity , Equipment Design , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted/methods , Lens, Crystalline/physiopathology , Lens, Crystalline/surgery , Presbyopia/surgery , Refraction, Ocular , Robotics/instrumentation , Sus scrofa
17.
Schizophr Bull ; 45(2): 405-414, 2019 03 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29608775

ABSTRACT

Risk profile scores (RPS) derived from genome-wide association studies (GWAS) explain a considerable amount of susceptibility for schizophrenia (SCZ). However, little is known about how common genetic risk factors for SCZ influence the structure and function of the human brain, largely due to the constraints of imaging sample sizes. In the current study, we use a novel recall-by-genotype (RbG) methodological approach, where we sample young adults from a population cohort (Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children: N genotyped = 8365) based on their SCZ-RPS. We compared 197 healthy individuals at extremes of low (N = 99) or high (N = 98) SCZ-RPS with behavioral tests, and structural and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). We first provide methodological details that will inform the design of future RbG studies for common SCZ genetic risk. We further provide an between group analysis of the RbG individuals (low vs high SCZ-RPS) who underwent structural neuroimaging data (T1-weighted scans) and fMRI data during a reversal learning task. While we found little evidence for morphometric differences between the low and high SCZ-RPS groups, we observed an impact of SCZ-RPS on blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) signal during reward processing in the ventral striatum (PFWE-VS-CORRECTED = .037), a previously investigated broader reward-related network (PFWE-ROIS-CORRECTED = .008), and across the whole brain (PFWE-WHOLE-BRAIN-CORRECTED = .013). We also describe the study strategy and discuss specific challenges of RbG for SCZ risk (such as SCZ-RPS related homoscedasticity). This study will help to elucidate the behavioral and imaging phenotypes that are associated with SCZ genetic risk.


Subject(s)
Brain , Genetic Predisposition to Disease , Genotype , Neuroimaging/methods , Reversal Learning/physiology , Schizophrenia , Ventral Striatum , Adult , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Brain/pathology , Brain/physiopathology , Cohort Studies , Female , Functional Neuroimaging/methods , Genetic Predisposition to Disease/genetics , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Reward , Risk , Schizophrenia/genetics , Schizophrenia/pathology , Schizophrenia/physiopathology , Ventral Striatum/diagnostic imaging , Ventral Striatum/pathology , Ventral Striatum/physiopathology , Young Adult
18.
Epilepsy Res ; 74(2-3): 201-9, 2007 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17448638

ABSTRACT

The therapy of focal epilepsy remains unsatisfactory for as many as 25% of patients. We tested the hypothesis that an efficient, ultraviolet light emitting diode (UV LED), coupled with a newly developed "caged" gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA), might be capable of terminating "ictal-like" events in cultured murine neurons. GABA was released from BC204, a recently described caged GABA, using a small, ultraviolet (UV) LED. Ictal-like events were provoked by removal of extracellular magnesium. In preliminary control experiments, the concentration of GABA released from our caged compound was dependent upon the strength and duration of the illumination, and readily achieved micromolar (microM) levels that are known to activate tonic, extrasynaptic GABA(A) receptors. Ultraviolet illumination had no effect when BC204 was not present in the perfusate and the currents produced by BC204 were eliminated by picrotoxin. Within a few seconds of UV illumination, BC204 rapidly terminated ictal-like events at low microM concentration. Uncaging of BC204 also blocked the elevation of intracellular calcium induced by seizure-like discharges in our cultures. While much more technical development is clearly required to extend our observations to a more intact preparation, these results suggest the intriguing possibility of constructing an implantable device to "optically suppress" focal human seizures under closed loop control.


Subject(s)
Anticonvulsants/radiation effects , Anticonvulsants/therapeutic use , Epilepsies, Partial/therapy , Seizures/drug therapy , gamma-Aminobutyric Acid/analogs & derivatives , Animals , Anticonvulsants/administration & dosage , Calcium/metabolism , Cells, Cultured , Electrophysiology , GABA Antagonists/pharmacology , Hippocampus/cytology , Hippocampus/drug effects , Mice , Microscopy, Fluorescence , Neurons/drug effects , Picrotoxin/pharmacology , Seizures/physiopathology , Ultraviolet Rays , gamma-Aminobutyric Acid/administration & dosage , gamma-Aminobutyric Acid/radiation effects , gamma-Aminobutyric Acid/therapeutic use
19.
Vision Res ; 46(23): 3994-4006, 2006 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16996556

ABSTRACT

We show in a 4-layer competitive neuronal network that continuous transformation learning, which uses spatial correlations and a purely associative (Hebbian) synaptic modification rule, can build view invariant representations of complex 3D objects. This occurs even when views of the different objects are interleaved, a condition where temporal trace learning fails. Human psychophysical experiments showed that view invariant object learning can occur when spatial but not temporal continuity applies because of interleaving of stimuli, although sequential presentation, which produces temporal continuity, can facilitate learning. Thus continuous transformation learning is an important principle that may contribute to view invariant object recognition.


Subject(s)
Computer Simulation , Form Perception/physiology , Learning , Models, Neurological , Retention, Psychology , Humans , Neural Networks, Computer , Photic Stimulation , Psychophysics , Time , Visual Cortex/physiology , Visual Pathways/physiology
20.
R Soc Open Sci ; 3(3): 150593, 2016 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27069648

ABSTRACT

It has been suggested that gamma (30-100 Hz) oscillations mediate awareness of visual stimuli, but tests of this hypothesis have produced differing results. We used phase scrambling to vary the perceptibility of face stimuli in order to determine whether gamma is indeed linked to perceptual awareness. Magnetoencephalography was used to measure the gamma response in 25 participants while viewing three conditions in which faces were presented either above, below or at the threshold for detection. In each of 400 trials (100 each for the sub- and suprathreshold conditions, 200 for the threshold condition), participants indicated whether they perceived a face in the stimulus. Gamma-band activity during the task was localized to bilateral ventral occipito-temporal cortex. For the threshold condition, we failed to find a significant difference in gamma amplitude between trials in which a face was perceived relative to those in which no face was perceived. However, we did find that gamma amplitude was significantly increased for threshold relative to subthreshold stimuli and for suprathreshold relative to threshold stimuli. This leads us to conclude that the gamma response to faces is primarily modulated by the presence of sensory evidence of a face rather by perceptual awareness.

SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL