Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 40
Filter
1.
Mol Psychiatry ; 24(10): 1566, 2019 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30890763

ABSTRACT

Following publication of this article, the authors noticed an error in the abstract, where they incorrectly stated that: "Direct application of IL-1ß to ex vivo hippocampal slices induced non-synaptic depolarisation and irreversible loss of membrane potential in CA1 neurons from diseased animals and systemic LPS increased apoptosis in the degenerating brain, in an IL-1RI-/--dependent fashion". This has now been corrected to: "Direct application of IL-1ß to ex vivo hippocampal slices induced non-synaptic depolarisation and irreversible loss of membrane potential in CA1 neurons from diseased animals and systemic LPS increased apoptosis in the degenerating brain, in an IL-1RI-dependent fashion". The authors would like to apologise for this error. This has been corrected in both the PDF and HTML versions of the article.

2.
Mol Psychiatry ; 24(10): 1533-1548, 2019 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29875474

ABSTRACT

Systemic inflammation can impair cognition with relevance to dementia, delirium and post-operative cognitive dysfunction. Episodes of delirium also contribute to rates of long-term cognitive decline, implying that these acute events induce injury. Whether systemic inflammation-induced acute dysfunction and acute brain injury occur by overlapping or discrete mechanisms remains unexplored. Here we show that systemic inflammation, induced by bacterial LPS, produces both working-memory deficits and acute brain injury in the degenerating brain and that these occur by dissociable IL-1-dependent processes. In normal C57BL/6 mice, LPS (100 µg/kg) did not affect working memory but impaired long-term memory consolidation. However prior hippocampal synaptic loss left mice selectively vulnerable to LPS-induced working memory deficits. Systemically administered IL-1 receptor antagonist (IL-1RA) was protective against, and systemic IL-1ß replicated, these working memory deficits. Dexamethasone abolished systemic cytokine synthesis and was protective against working memory deficits, without blocking brain IL-1ß synthesis. Direct application of IL-1ß to ex vivo hippocampal slices induced non-synaptic depolarisation and irreversible loss of membrane potential in CA1 neurons from diseased animals and systemic LPS increased apoptosis in the degenerating brain, in an IL-1RI-dependent fashion. The data suggest that LPS induces working memory dysfunction via circulating IL-1ß but direct hippocampal action of IL-1ß causes neuronal dysfunction and may drive neuronal death. The data suggest that acute systemic inflammation produces both reversible cognitive deficits, resembling delirium, and acute brain injury contributing to long-term cognitive impairment but that these events are mechanistically dissociable. These data have significant implications for management of cognitive dysfunction during acute illness.


Subject(s)
Brain Injuries/immunology , Cognitive Dysfunction/immunology , Interleukin-1/metabolism , Animals , Brain/metabolism , Cognition/physiology , Cognition Disorders/immunology , Cognitive Dysfunction/chemically induced , Cognitive Dysfunction/metabolism , Cytokines/metabolism , Dementia/immunology , Female , Hippocampus/metabolism , Inflammation/complications , Inflammation/metabolism , Interleukin-1/immunology , Lipopolysaccharides/pharmacology , Memory Disorders/immunology , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Mice , Mice, Inbred C57BL , Neurons/metabolism
3.
Nat Rev Neurosci ; 15(3): 181-92, 2014 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24552786

ABSTRACT

Recent studies using transgenic mice lacking NMDA receptors in the hippocampus challenge the long-standing hypothesis that hippocampal long-term potentiation-like mechanisms underlie the encoding and storage of associative long-term spatial memories. However, it may not be the synaptic plasticity-dependent memory hypothesis that is wrong; instead, it may be the role of the hippocampus that needs to be re-examined. We present an account of hippocampal function that explains its role in both memory and anxiety.


Subject(s)
Anxiety/physiopathology , Hippocampus/physiology , Memory/physiology , Neuronal Plasticity/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Synapses/physiology , Animals , Behavior, Animal/physiology , Hippocampus/physiopathology , Mice , Mice, Knockout , Receptors, N-Methyl-D-Aspartate/genetics , Receptors, N-Methyl-D-Aspartate/metabolism , Synaptic Transmission/physiology
4.
Hippocampus ; 33(1): 3-5, 2023 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36453879
5.
FASEB J ; 30(12): 4021-4032, 2016 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27528626

ABSTRACT

Ketone bodies are the most energy-efficient fuel and yield more ATP per mole of substrate than pyruvate and increase the free energy released from ATP hydrolysis. Elevation of circulating ketones via high-fat, low-carbohydrate diets has been used for the treatment of drug-refractory epilepsy and for neurodegenerative diseases, such as Parkinson's disease. Ketones may also be beneficial for muscle and brain in times of stress, such as endurance exercise. The challenge has been to raise circulating ketone levels by using a palatable diet without altering lipid levels. We found that blood ketone levels can be increased and cholesterol and triglycerides decreased by feeding rats a novel ketone ester diet: chow that is supplemented with (R)-3-hydroxybutyl (R)-3-hydroxybutyrate as 30% of calories. For 5 d, rats on the ketone diet ran 32% further on a treadmill than did control rats that ate an isocaloric diet that was supplemented with either corn starch or palm oil (P < 0.05). Ketone-fed rats completed an 8-arm radial maze test 38% faster than did those on the other diets, making more correct decisions before making a mistake (P < 0.05). Isolated, perfused hearts from rats that were fed the ketone diet had greater free energy available from ATP hydrolysis during increased work than did hearts from rats on the other diets as shown by using [31P]-NMR spectroscopy. The novel ketone diet, therefore, improved physical performance and cognitive function in rats, and its energy-sparing properties suggest that it may help to treat a range of human conditions with metabolic abnormalities.-Murray, A. J., Knight, N. S., Cole, M. A., Cochlin, L. E., Carter, E., Tchabanenko, K., Pichulik, T., Gulston, M. K., Atherton, H. J., Schroeder, M. A., Deacon, R. M. J., Kashiwaya, Y., King, M. T., Pawlosky, R., Rawlins, J. N. P., Tyler, D. J., Griffin, J. L., Robertson, J., Veech, R. L., Clarke, K. Novel ketone diet enhances physical and cognitive performance.


Subject(s)
Cognition/physiology , Diet , Energy Intake/physiology , Energy Metabolism/physiology , Feeding Behavior/physiology , Ketones/administration & dosage , Animals , Cholesterol/blood , Dietary Carbohydrates/metabolism , Dietary Fats/metabolism , Insulin/metabolism , Male , Rats, Wistar , Triglycerides/blood
6.
Hum Mol Genet ; 22(11): 2247-62, 2013 Jun 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23424202

ABSTRACT

Kv10.1 (Eag1), member of the Kv10 family of voltage-gated potassium channels, is preferentially expressed in adult brain. The aim of the present study was to unravel the functional role of Kv10.1 in the brain by generating knockout mice, where the voltage sensor and pore region of Kv10.1 were removed to render non-functional proteins through deletion of exon 7 of the KCNH1 gene using the '3 Lox P strategy'. Kv10.1-deficient mice show no obvious alterations during embryogenesis and develop normally to adulthood; cortex, hippocampus and cerebellum appear anatomically normal. Other tests, including general health screen, sensorimotor functioning and gating, anxiety, social behaviour, learning and memory did not show any functional aberrations in Kv10.1 null mice. Kv10.1 null mice display mild hyperactivity and longer-lasting haloperidol-induced catalepsy, but there was no difference between genotypes in amphetamine sensitization and withdrawal, reactivity to apomorphine and haloperidol in the prepulse inhibition tests or to antidepressants in the haloperidol-induced catalepsy. Furthermore, electrical properties of Kv10.1 in cerebellar Purkinje cells did not show any difference between genotypes. Bearing in mind that Kv10.1 is overexpressed in over 70% of all human tumours and that its inhibition leads to a reduced tumour cell proliferation, the fact that deletion of Kv10.1 does not show a marked phenotype is a prerequisite for utilizing Kv10.1 blocking and/or reduction techniques, such as siRNA, to treat cancer.


Subject(s)
Potassium Channels, Voltage-Gated/genetics , Potassium Channels, Voltage-Gated/metabolism , Action Potentials , Amphetamine/administration & dosage , Amphetamine/metabolism , Animals , Antidepressive Agents/administration & dosage , Behavior, Animal/drug effects , Brain/metabolism , Catalepsy/chemically induced , Catalepsy/drug therapy , Cerebellum/metabolism , Gene Knockout Techniques , Gene Order , Gene Targeting , Genotype , Haloperidol/adverse effects , Mice , Mice, Knockout , Phenotype
7.
Am J Geriatr Psychiatry ; 23(4): 403-415, 2015 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25239680

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Delirium is a profound neuropsychiatric disturbance precipitated by acute illness. Although dementia is the major risk factor this has typically been considered a binary quantity (i.e., cognitively impaired versus cognitively normal) with respect to delirium risk. We used humans and mice to address the hypothesis that the severity of underlying neurodegenerative changes and/or cognitive impairment progressively alters delirium risk. METHODS: Humans in a population-based longitudinal study, Vantaa 85+, were followed for incident delirium. Odds for reporting delirium at follow-up (outcome) were modeled using random-effects logistic regression, where prior cognitive impairment measured by Mini-Mental State Exam (MMSE) (exposure) was considered. To address whether underlying neurodegenerative pathology increased susceptibility to acute cognitive change, mice at three stages of neurodegenerative disease progression (ME7 model of neurodegeneration: controls, 12 weeks, and 16 weeks) were assessed for acute cognitive dysfunction upon systemic inflammation induced by bacterial lipopolysaccharide (LPS; 100 µg/kg). Synaptic and axonal correlates of susceptibility to acute dysfunction were assessed using immunohistochemistry. RESULTS: In the Vantaa cohort, 465 persons (88.4 ± 2.8 years) completed MMSE at baseline. For every MMSE point lost, risk of incident delirium increased by 5% (p = 0.02). LPS precipitated severe and fluctuating cognitive deficits in 16-week ME7 mice but lower incidence or no deficits in 12-week ME7 and controls, respectively. This was associated with progressive thalamic synaptic loss and axonal pathology. CONCLUSION: A human population-based cohort with graded severity of existing cognitive impairment and a mouse model with progressing neurodegeneration both indicate that the risk of delirium increases with greater severity of pre-existing cognitive impairment and neuropathology.


Subject(s)
Axons/pathology , Cognition Disorders/pathology , Delirium/epidemiology , Inflammation/pathology , Synapses/pathology , Aged, 80 and over , Animals , Cognition Disorders/chemically induced , Cognition Disorders/complications , Cognition Disorders/diagnosis , Cognition Disorders/psychology , Delirium/complications , Delirium/diagnosis , Disease Models, Animal , Disease Progression , Finland/epidemiology , Hippocampus/drug effects , Hippocampus/pathology , Humans , Inflammation/chemically induced , Inflammation/psychology , Lipopolysaccharides , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Maze Learning , Mice , Psychiatric Status Rating Scales , Thalamus/drug effects , Thalamus/pathology
8.
Nat Genet ; 38(8): 879-87, 2006 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16832355

ABSTRACT

Difficulties in fine-mapping quantitative trait loci (QTLs) are a major impediment to progress in the molecular dissection of complex traits in mice. Here we show that genome-wide high-resolution mapping of multiple phenotypes can be achieved using a stock of genetically heterogeneous mice. We developed a conservative and robust bootstrap analysis to map 843 QTLs with an average 95% confidence interval of 2.8 Mb. The QTLs contribute to variation in 97 traits, including models of human disease (asthma, type 2 diabetes mellitus, obesity and anxiety) as well as immunological, biochemical and hematological phenotypes. The genetic architecture of almost all phenotypes was complex, with many loci each contributing a small proportion to the total variance. Our data set, freely available at http://gscan.well.ox.ac.uk, provides an entry point to the functional characterization of genes involved in many complex traits.


Subject(s)
Mice/genetics , Quantitative Trait Loci , Animals , Breeding , Chromosome Mapping , Female , Genomics , Genotype , Humans , Male , Models, Genetic , Phenotype , Polymorphism, Single Nucleotide
9.
Nat Genet ; 36(11): 1197-202, 2004 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15489855

ABSTRACT

Here we present a strategy to determine the genetic basis of variance in complex phenotypes that arise from natural, as opposed to induced, genetic variation in mice. We show that a commercially available strain of outbred mice, MF1, can be treated as an ultrafine mosaic of standard inbred strains and accordingly used to dissect a known quantitative trait locus influencing anxiety. We also show that this locus can be subdivided into three regions, one of which contains Rgs2, which encodes a regulator of G protein signaling. We then use quantitative complementation to show that Rgs2 is a quantitative trait gene. This combined genetic and functional approach should be applicable to the analysis of any quantitative trait.


Subject(s)
Anxiety/genetics , Chromosome Mapping/methods , Quantitative Trait, Heritable , RGS Proteins/physiology , Animals , Animals, Outbred Strains , Base Sequence , Genetic Complementation Test , Mice , Mice, Inbred Strains , Mosaicism , RGS Proteins/genetics
10.
Hippocampus ; 22(7): 1553-66, 2012 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22161993

ABSTRACT

Spatial properties of stimuli are sometimes encoded even when incidental to the demands of a particular learning task. Incidental encoding of spatial information may interfere with learning by (i) causing a failure to generalize learning between trials in which a cue is presented in different spatial locations and (ii) adding common spatial features to stimuli that predict different outcomes. Hippocampal lesions have been found to facilitate acquisition of certain tasks. This facilitation may occur because hippocampal lesions impair incidental encoding of spatial information that interferes with learning. To test this prediction mice with lesions of the hippocampus were trained on appetitive simple simultaneous discrimination tasks using inserts in the goal arms of a T-maze. It was found that hippocampal lesioned mice were facilitated at learning the discriminations, but they were sensitive to changes in spatial information in a manner that was similar to control mice. In a second experiment it was found that both control and hippocampal lesioned mice showed equivalent incidental encoding of egocentric spatial properties of the inserts, but both groups did not encode the allocentric information. These results demonstrate that mice show incidental encoding of egocentric spatial information that decreases the ability to solve simultaneous discrimination tasks. The normal egocentric spatial encoding in hippocampal lesioned mice contradicts theories of hippocampal function that suggest that the hippocampus is necessary for incidental learning per se, or is required for modulating stimulus representations based on the relevancy of information. The facilitated learning suggests that the hippocampal lesions can enhance learning of the same qualitative information as acquired by control mice.


Subject(s)
Discrimination Learning/physiology , Hippocampus/injuries , Space Perception/physiology , Animals , Discrimination, Psychological , Female , Maze Learning , Mice , Mice, Inbred C57BL , Random Allocation
11.
Curr Biol ; 18(5): 354-62, 2008 Mar 11.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18328704

ABSTRACT

The most well-described example of an inherited speech and language disorder is that observed in the multigenerational KE family, caused by a heterozygous missense mutation in the FOXP2 gene. Affected individuals are characterized by deficits in the learning and production of complex orofacial motor sequences underlying fluent speech and display impaired linguistic processing for both spoken and written language. The FOXP2 transcription factor is highly similar in many vertebrate species, with conserved expression in neural circuits related to sensorimotor integration and motor learning. In this study, we generated mice carrying an identical point mutation to that of the KE family, yielding the equivalent arginine-to-histidine substitution in the Foxp2 DNA-binding domain. Homozygous R552H mice show severe reductions in cerebellar growth and postnatal weight gain but are able to produce complex innate ultrasonic vocalizations. Heterozygous R552H mice are overtly normal in brain structure and development. Crucially, although their baseline motor abilities appear to be identical to wild-type littermates, R552H heterozygotes display significant deficits in species-typical motor-skill learning, accompanied by abnormal synaptic plasticity in striatal and cerebellar neural circuits.


Subject(s)
Forkhead Transcription Factors/genetics , Learning/physiology , Motor Skills/physiology , Neuronal Plasticity/genetics , Point Mutation , Repressor Proteins/genetics , Speech Disorders/genetics , Alleles , Animals , Heterozygote , Humans , Mice , Mice, Knockout , Vocalization, Animal/physiology
12.
FASEB J ; 23(12): 4353-60, 2009 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19667117

ABSTRACT

Efficiency, defined as the amount of work produced for a given amount of oxygen consumed, is a key determinant of endurance capacity, and can be altered by metabolic substrate supply, in that fatty acid oxidation is less efficient than glucose oxidation. It is unclear, however, whether consumption of a high-fat diet would be detrimental or beneficial for endurance capacity, due to purported glycogen-sparing properties. In addition, a high-fat diet over several months leads to cognitive impairment. Here, we tested the hypothesis that short-term ingestion of a high-fat diet (55% kcal from fat) would impair exercise capacity and cognitive function in rats, compared with a control chow diet (7.5% kcal from fat) via mitochondrial uncoupling and energy deprivation. We found that rats ran 35% less far on a treadmill and showed cognitive impairment in a maze test with 9 d of high-fat feeding, with respiratory uncoupling in skeletal muscle mitochondria, associated with increased uncoupling protein (UCP3) levels. Our results suggest that high-fat feeding, even over short periods of time, alters skeletal muscle UCP3 expression, affecting energy production and physical performance. Optimization of nutrition to maximize the efficiency of mitochondrial ATP production could improve energetics in athletes and patients with metabolic abnormalities.


Subject(s)
Cognition Disorders/chemically induced , Dietary Fats/toxicity , Physical Endurance/drug effects , Animals , Diet , Energy Metabolism , Fatty Acids/metabolism , Ion Channels/metabolism , Male , Mitochondria, Muscle/metabolism , Mitochondrial Proteins/metabolism , Muscle, Skeletal/metabolism , Palmitoyl-CoA Hydrolase , Rats , Rats, Wistar , Thiolester Hydrolases/metabolism , Time Factors , Uncoupling Protein 3
13.
Learn Mem ; 16(6): 379-86, 2009 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19470654

ABSTRACT

The GluA1 AMPA receptor subunit is a key mediator of hippocampal synaptic plasticity and is especially important for a rapidly-induced, short-lasting form of potentiation. GluA1 gene deletion impairs hippocampus-dependent, spatial working memory, but spares hippocampus-dependent spatial reference memory. These findings may reflect the necessity of GluA1-dependent synaptic plasticity for short-term memory of recently visited places, but not for the ability to form long-term associations between a particular spatial location and an outcome. This hypothesis is in concordance with the theory that short-term and long-term memory depend on dissociable psychological processes. In this study we tested GluA1-/- mice on both short-term and long-term spatial memory using a simple novelty preference task. Mice were given a series of repeated exposures to a particular spatial location (the arm of a Y-maze) before their preference for a novel spatial location (the unvisited arm of the maze) over the familiar spatial location was assessed. GluA1-/- mice were impaired if the interval between the trials was short (1 min), but showed enhanced spatial memory if the interval between the trials was long (24 h). This enhancement was caused by the interval between the exposure trials rather than the interval prior to the test, thus demonstrating enhanced learning and not simply enhanced performance or expression of memory. This seemingly paradoxical enhancement of hippocampus-dependent spatial learning may be caused by GluA1 gene deletion reducing the detrimental effects of short-term memory on subsequent long-term learning. Thus, these results support a dual-process model of memory in which short-term and long-term memory are separate and sometimes competitive processes.


Subject(s)
Memory Disorders/genetics , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Models, Biological , Receptors, AMPA/deficiency , Analysis of Variance , Animals , Behavior, Animal , Exploratory Behavior/physiology , Female , Hippocampus/injuries , Hippocampus/physiology , Male , Maze Learning/physiology , Memory Disorders/pathology , Mice , Mice, Inbred C57BL , Mice, Knockout , Sex Factors , Time Factors
14.
Eur J Neurosci ; 29(6): 1141-52, 2009 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19302150

ABSTRACT

Long-term potentiation (LTP) at hippocampal CA3-CA1 synapses is thought to be mediated, at least in part, by an increase in the postsynaptic surface expression of alpha-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazole proprionic acid (AMPA) receptors induced by N-methyl-d-aspartate (NMDA) receptor activation. While this process was originally attributed to the regulated synaptic insertion of GluA1 (GluR-A) subunit-containing AMPA receptors, recent evidence suggests that regulated synaptic trafficking of GluA2 subunits might also contribute to one or several phases of potentiation. However, it has so far been difficult to separate these two mechanisms experimentally. Here we used genetically modified mice lacking the GluA1 subunit (Gria1(-/-) mice) to investigate GluA1-independent mechanisms of LTP at CA3-CA1 synapses in transverse hippocampal slices. An extracellular, paired theta-burst stimulation paradigm induced a robust GluA1-independent form of LTP lacking the early, rapidly decaying component characteristic of LTP in wild-type mice. This GluA1-independent form of LTP was attenuated by inhibitors of neuronal nitric oxide synthase and protein kinase C (PKC), two enzymes known to regulate GluA2 surface expression. Furthermore, the induction of GluA1-independent potentiation required the activation of GluN2B (NR2B) subunit-containing NMDA receptors. Our findings support and extend the evidence that LTP at hippocampal CA3-CA1 synapses comprises a rapidly decaying, GluA1-dependent component and a more sustained, GluA1-independent component, induced and expressed via a separate mechanism involving GluN2B-containing NMDA receptors, neuronal nitric oxide synthase and PKC.


Subject(s)
Gene Expression/physiology , Hippocampus/metabolism , Long-Term Potentiation/genetics , Receptors, AMPA/metabolism , Analysis of Variance , Animals , Biophysics , Electric Stimulation/methods , Enzyme Inhibitors/pharmacology , Excitatory Amino Acid Antagonists/pharmacology , Excitatory Postsynaptic Potentials/drug effects , Excitatory Postsynaptic Potentials/genetics , Gene Expression/drug effects , In Vitro Techniques , Long-Term Potentiation/drug effects , Male , Mice , Mice, Inbred C57BL , Mice, Knockout , Nerve Net/drug effects , Nerve Net/physiology , Nitric Oxide Synthase Type I/metabolism , Protein Kinase C/metabolism , Receptors, AMPA/antagonists & inhibitors , Receptors, AMPA/deficiency
15.
Front Mol Neurosci ; 11: 199, 2018.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29988555

ABSTRACT

The GluA1 subunit of the L-α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid receptor (AMPAR) plays a crucial, but highly selective, role in cognitive function. Here we analyzed AMPAR expression, AMPAR distribution and spatial learning in mice (Gria1R/R ), expressing the "trafficking compromised" GluA1(Q600R) point mutation. Our analysis revealed somatic accumulation and reduction of GluA1(Q600R) and GluA2, but only slightly reduced CA1 synaptic localization in hippocampi of adult Gria1R/R mice. These immunohistological changes were accompanied by a strong reduction of somatic AMPAR currents in CA1, and a reduction of plasticity (short-term and long-term potentiation, STP and LTP, respectively) in the CA1 subfield following tetanic and theta-burst stimulation. Nevertheless, spatial reference memory acquisition in the Morris water-maze and on an appetitive Y-maze task was unaffected in Gria1R/R mice. In contrast, spatial working/short-term memory during both spontaneous and rewarded alternation tasks was dramatically impaired. These findings identify the GluA1(Q600R) mutation as a loss of function mutation that provides independent evidence for the selective role of GluA1 in the expression of short-term memory.

16.
Genetics ; 174(2): 959-84, 2006 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16888333

ABSTRACT

The interaction between genotype and environment is recognized as an important source of experimental variation when complex traits are measured in the mouse, but the magnitude of that interaction has not often been measured. From a study of 2448 genetically heterogeneous mice, we report the heritability of 88 complex traits that include models of human disease (asthma, type 2 diabetes mellitus, obesity, and anxiety) as well as immunological, biochemical, and hematological phenotypes. We show that environmental and physiological covariates are involved in an unexpectedly large number of significant interactions with genetic background. The 15 covariates we examined have a significant effect on behavioral and physiological tests, although they rarely explain >10% of the variation. We found that interaction effects are more frequent and larger than the main effects: half of the interactions explained >20% of the variance and in nine cases exceeded 50%. Our results indicate that assays of gene function using mouse models should take into account interactions between gene and environment.


Subject(s)
Environment , Multifactorial Inheritance/genetics , Animals , Disease Models, Animal , Female , Humans , Male , Mice , Phenotype
17.
J Neurosci ; 23(9): 3953-9, 2003 May 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12736365

ABSTRACT

Gene-targeted mice lacking the AMPA receptor subunit glutamate receptor-A (GluRA) (GluR1) and wild-type controls were compared on a radial-maze task in which the same three of six arms were always baited, but in which the rewards of milk were not replaced within a trial. This procedure allowed not only a within-subjects but also a within-trials assessment of both spatial working memory (WM) and reference memory (RM) in GluRA-/- mice, using identical spatial cues. In experiment 1, the GluRA-/- mice made more WM and RM errors during task acquisition. However, separate groups of GluRA-/- and wild-type mice (experiment 2) acquired a purely RM version of the task at a similar rate, using a paradigm with which it was not possible to make WM errors (doors prevented mice from re-entering an arm that they had already visited on that trial). In contrast, mice with hippocampal lesions were dramatically impaired. These results are consistent with the possibility that the WM impairment in the GluRA-/- mice during experiment 1 produced interference that disrupted RM acquisition. A WM component was therefore introduced after RM acquisition in experiment 2 (i.e., the mice were no longer prevented from re-entering a previously visited arm). The GluRA-/- mice now made considerably more WM errors than did wild-type mice, but simultaneously, RM was only mildly and transiently impaired. These experiments provide additional evidence of a selective spatial WM deficit coexisting with intact spatial RM acquisition in GluRA-/- mice, suggesting that different neuronal mechanisms within the hippocampus may support these different kinds of information processing.


Subject(s)
Maze Learning , Memory Disorders/physiopathology , Memory, Short-Term , Receptors, AMPA/deficiency , Animals , Behavior, Animal , Exploratory Behavior , Female , Memory Disorders/genetics , Mice , Mice, Knockout , Receptors, AMPA/genetics , Spatial Behavior
18.
J Neurosci ; 24(33): 7277-82, 2004 Aug 18.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15317854

ABSTRACT

Gene-targeted mice lacking the AMPA receptor subunit glutamate receptor-A (GluR-A or GluR1) and mice with cytotoxic hippocampal lesions were compared with wild-type and sham-operated controls, respectively, on a conditional learning task using an elevated T-maze. Floor inserts (white perspex vs wire mesh) provided a conditional cue indicating in which goal arm a food reward was to be found. The relationship between the floor insert and the rewarded goal arm was constant throughout the experiment. Both lesioned and knock-out mice were able to acquire the task if the floor inserts extended throughout the entire maze, including the start arm and both goal arms. In contrast, both lesioned and knock-out mice were unable to acquire the task if the floor inserts were only present in the start arm of the maze. The absence of the conditional cue (the floor insert) at the time when the place-reward association was experienced thus critically determined whether or not the mice were impaired. We suggest that hippocampal GluR-A-dependent synaptic plasticity contributes to a memory system in rodents for encoding both the spatial and temporal contexts (the where and the when) associated with a particular event.


Subject(s)
Hippocampus/physiology , Maze Learning/physiology , Neuronal Plasticity , Receptors, AMPA/physiology , Animals , Female , Hippocampus/anatomy & histology , Mice , Mice, Inbred C57BL , Mice, Knockout , Receptors, AMPA/genetics , Reward , Synaptic Transmission
19.
Behav Brain Res ; 156(2): 241-9, 2005 Jan 30.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15582110

ABSTRACT

The hippocampus is believed to play an important role in spatial cognition and anxiety. Much of the supporting evidence is derived from rat studies. Recent reports on hippocampal lesioned mice also showed impairments in spatial function, but anxiety was not uniformly diminished. There were, however, striking impairments in several "species typical" behaviours; lesioned mice made poorer nests, and hoarded and burrowed less. In the present experiments, mice with excitotoxic hippocampal lesions were tested in a well-established anxiety paradigm, the light-dark box. As in previous anxiety tests, the results were mixed; some measures (reduced dark time) suggested lesioned mice were less anxious; others (fewer light-dark transits) suggested greater anxiety. However, lesioned mice only made fewer transits when the door was small. This suggested that the tendency to enter small holes, so characteristic of small rodents, was reduced; subsequent tests showed lesioned mice preferred to explore in an alley rather than enter its attached tunnels. Further tests of "species typical" behaviours revealed that lesioned mice spent less time digging and climbing, and made less use of cardboard shelters in their cages. This was not due to inactivity; lesions did not reduce grooming or locomotion. Finally, tests of hyponeophagia showed hippocampal lesions reduced this measure of anxiety, so long as the control baseline was sufficiently high. Overall, the results suggest that the hippocampus is important in many species-typical behaviours, potentially influencing performance in a range of behavioural tests. However, species-typical behaviours offer easy and economical ways to test for hippocampal dysfunction, for example, in genetically modified mice.


Subject(s)
Anxiety/physiopathology , Behavior, Animal/physiology , Hippocampus/physiology , Animals , Anxiety/pathology , Body Weight/physiology , Brain Diseases/physiopathology , Drinking/physiology , Eating/physiology , Exploratory Behavior/physiology , Feeding Behavior/physiology , Female , Hippocampus/pathology , Maze Learning/physiology , Mice , Mice, Inbred C57BL/physiology , Motor Activity/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Statistics, Nonparametric
20.
Behav Neurosci ; 116(3): 472-8, 2002 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12049328

ABSTRACT

Control mice rapidly learned to escape from shallow water in a paddling pool, which combined elements of the Morris water maze and the Barnes holeboard maze. The pool's transparent perimeter wall contained 12 exits, only 1 of which led to an escape tunnel. Learning was impaired in mice with cytotoxic lesions of the hippocampus. Probe trials suggested that the controls were using extramaze cues. When the exit was blocked, controls, but not hippocampals, spent more time searching in this previously correct sector. When the spatial location of the exit was changed, hippocampals escaped more quickly, as they showed no preference for the old location. These results may be useful in the assessment of hippocampal dysfunction, particularly in genetically manipulated mice.


Subject(s)
Hippocampus/physiology , Maze Learning/physiology , Animals , Female , Memory Disorders/physiopathology , Mice , Mice, Inbred C57BL , Swimming/physiology
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL