Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 107
Filtrar
Más filtros

Bases de datos
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
J Sleep Res ; : e14161, 2024 Feb 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38308529

RESUMEN

The detrimental effects of sleep loss on overall decision-making have been well described. Due to the complex nature of decisions, there remains a need for studies to identify specific mechanisms of decision-making vulnerable to sleep loss. Bayesian perspectives of decision-making posit judgement formation during decision-making occurs via a process of integrating knowledge gleaned from past experiences (priors) with new information from current observations (likelihoods). We investigated the effects of sleep loss on the ability to integrate multiple sources of information during decision-making by reporting results from two experiments: the first implementing both sleep restriction (SR) and total sleep deprivation (TSD) protocols, and the second implementing an SR protocol. In both experiments, participants were administered the Bayes Decisions Task on which optimal performance requires the integration of Bayesian prior and likelihood information. Participants in Experiment 1 showed reduced reliance on both information sources after SR, while no significant change was observed after TSD. Participants in Experiment 2 showed reduced reliance on likelihood after SR, especially during morning testing sessions. No accuracy-related impairments resulting from SR and TSD were observed in both experiments. Our findings show SR affects decision-making through altering the way individuals integrate available sources of information. Additionally, the ability to integrate information during SR may be influenced by time of day. Broadly, our findings carry implications for working professionals who are required to make high-stakes decisions on the job, yet consistently receive insufficient sleep due to work schedule demands.

2.
J Sleep Res ; 33(1): e13933, 2024 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37315929

RESUMEN

Understanding whether drivers can accurately assess sleepiness is essential for educational campaigns advising drivers to stop driving when feeling sleepy. However, few studies have examined this in real-world driving environments, particularly among older drivers who comprise a large proportion of all road users. To examine the accuracy of subjective sleepiness ratings in predicting subsequent driving impairment and physiological drowsiness, 16 younger (21-33 years) and 17 older (50-65 years) adults drove an instrumented vehicle for 2 h on closed loop under two conditions: well-rested and 29 h sleep deprivation. Sleepiness ratings (Karolinska Sleepiness Scale, Likelihood of Falling Asleep scale, Sleepiness Symptoms Questionnaire) were obtained every 15min, alongside lane deviations, near crash events, and ocular indices of drowsiness. All subjective sleepiness measures increased with sleep deprivation for both age groups (p < 0.013). While most subjective sleepiness ratings significantly predicted driving impairment and drowsiness in younger adults (OR: 1.7-15.6, p < 0.02), this was only apparent for KSS, likelihood of falling asleep, and "difficulty staying in the lane for the older adults" (OR: 2.76-2.86, p = 0.02). This may be due to an altered perception of sleepiness in older adults, or due to lowered objective signs of impairment in the older group. Our data suggest that (i) younger and older drivers are aware of sleepiness; (ii) the best subjective scale may differ across age groups; and (iii) future research should expand on the best subjective measures to inform of crash risk in older adults to inform tailored educational road safety campaigns on signs of sleepiness.


Asunto(s)
Conducción de Automóvil , Privación de Sueño , Humanos , Anciano , Somnolencia , Vigilia/fisiología , Accidentes de Tránsito/prevención & control
3.
PLoS Comput Biol ; 19(10): e1010508, 2023 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37797040

RESUMEN

Epilepsy is a serious neurological disorder characterised by a tendency to have recurrent, spontaneous, seizures. Classically, seizures are assumed to occur at random. However, recent research has uncovered underlying rhythms both in seizures and in key signatures of epilepsy-so-called interictal epileptiform activity-with timescales that vary from hours and days through to months. Understanding the physiological mechanisms that determine these rhythmic patterns of epileptiform discharges remains an open question. Many people with epilepsy identify precipitants of their seizures, the most common of which include stress, sleep deprivation and fatigue. To quantify the impact of these physiological factors, we analysed 24-hour EEG recordings from a cohort of 107 people with idiopathic generalized epilepsy. We found two subgroups with distinct distributions of epileptiform discharges: one with highest incidence during sleep and the other during day-time. We interrogated these data using a mathematical model that describes the transitions between background and epileptiform activity in large-scale brain networks. This model was extended to include a time-dependent forcing term, where the excitability of nodes within the network could be modulated by other factors. We calibrated this forcing term using independently-collected human cortisol (the primary stress-responsive hormone characterised by circadian and ultradian patterns of secretion) data and sleep-staged EEG from healthy human participants. We found that either the dynamics of cortisol or sleep stage transition, or a combination of both, could explain most of the observed distributions of epileptiform discharges. Our findings provide conceptual evidence for the existence of underlying physiological drivers of rhythms of epileptiform discharges. These findings should motivate future research to explore these mechanisms in carefully designed experiments using animal models or people with epilepsy.


Asunto(s)
Epilepsia Generalizada , Epilepsia , Animales , Humanos , Hidrocortisona , Convulsiones , Electroencefalografía
4.
Psychophysiology ; 61(5): e14523, 2024 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38238554

RESUMEN

The ability to detect and subsequently correct errors is important in preventing the detrimental consequences of sleep loss. The Error Related Negativity (ERN), and the error positivity (Pe) are established neural correlates of error processing. Previous work has shown sleep loss reduces ERN and Pe, indicating sleep loss impairs error-monitoring processes. However, no previous work has examined behavioral error awareness, in conjunction with EEG measures, under sleep loss conditions, and studies of sleep restriction are lacking. Using combined behavioral and EEG measures, we report two studies investigating the impact of total sleep deprivation (TSD) and sleep restriction (SR) on error awareness. Fourteen healthy participants completed the Error Awareness Task under conditions of TSD and 27 completed the same task under conditions of SR. It was found that TSD did not influence behavioral error awareness or ERN or Pe amplitude, however, SR reduced behavioral error awareness, increased the time taken to detect errors, and reduced Pe amplitude. Findings indicate individuals who are chronically sleep restricted are at risk for reduced recognition of errors. Reduced error awareness may be one factor contributing to the increased accidents and injuries seen in contexts where sleep loss is prevalent.


Asunto(s)
Electroencefalografía , Potenciales Evocados , Humanos , Tiempo de Reacción , Privación de Sueño , Sueño , Desempeño Psicomotor , Concienciación
5.
J Pineal Res ; 76(2): e12936, 2024 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39041348

RESUMEN

Women typically sleep and wake earlier than men and have been shown to have earlier circadian timing relative to the light/dark cycle that synchronizes the clock. A potential mechanism for earlier timing in women is an altered response of the circadian system to evening light. We characterized individual-level dose-response curves for light-induced melatonin suppression using a within-subjects protocol. Fifty-six participants (29 women, 27 men; aged 18-30 years) were exposed to a range of light illuminances (10, 30, 50, 100, 200, 400, and 2000 lux) using melatonin suppression relative to a dim control (<1 lux) as a marker of light sensitivity. Women were free from hormonal contraception. To examine the potential influence of sex hormones, estradiol and progesterone was examined in women and testosterone was examined in a subset of men. Menstrual phase was monitored using self-reports and estradiol and progesterone levels. Women exhibited significantly greater melatonin suppression than men under the 400-lux and 2000-lux conditions, but not under lower light conditions (10-200 lux). Light sensitivity did not differ by menstrual phase, nor was it associated with levels of estradiol, progesterone, or testosterone, suggesting the sex differences in light sensitivity were not acutely driven by circulating levels of sex hormones. These results suggest that sex differences in circadian timing are not due to differences in the response to dim/moderate light exposures typically experienced in the evening. The finding of increased bright light sensitivity in women suggests that sex differences in circadian timing could plausibly instead be driven by a greater sensitivity to phase-advancing effects of bright morning light.


Asunto(s)
Ritmo Circadiano , Luz , Melatonina , Humanos , Femenino , Adulto , Ritmo Circadiano/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto Joven , Masculino , Melatonina/metabolismo , Estradiol/sangre , Progesterona/sangre , Progesterona/metabolismo , Testosterona/sangre , Ciclo Menstrual/fisiología
6.
J Am Chem Soc ; 145(24): 13069-13080, 2023 Jun 21.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37279356

RESUMEN

Lewis acid-catalyzed carbonyl-olefin metathesis has introduced a new means for revealing the behavior of Lewis acids. In particular, this reaction has led to the observation of new solution behaviors for FeCl3 that may qualitatively change how we think of Lewis acid activation. For example, catalytic metathesis reactions operate in the presence of superstoichiometric amounts of carbonyl, resulting in the formation of highly ligated (octahedral) iron geometries. These structures display reduced activity, decreasing catalyst turnover. As a result, it is necessary to steer the Fe-center away from inhibiting pathways to improve the reaction efficiency and augment yields for recalcitrant substrates. Herein, we examine the impact of the addition of TMSCl to FeCl3-catalyzed carbonyl-olefin metathesis, specifically for substrates that are prone to byproduct inhibition. Through kinetic, spectroscopic, and colligative experiments, significant deviations from the baseline metathesis reactivity are observed, including mitigation of byproduct inhibition as well as an increase in the reaction rate. Quantum chemical simulations are used to explain how TMSCl induces a change in catalyst structure that leads to these kinetic differences. Collectively, these data are consistent with the formation of a silylium catalyst, which induces the reaction through carbonyl binding. The FeCl3 activation of Si-Cl bonds to give the silylium active species is expected to have significant utility in enacting carbonyl-based transformations.

7.
J Sleep Res ; 32(2): e13730, 2023 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36193767

RESUMEN

This study aimed to examine the impact of break duration between consecutive shifts, time of break onset, and prior shift duration on total sleep time (TST) between shifts in heavy vehicle drivers (HVDs), and to assess the interaction between break duration and time of break onset. The sleep (actigraphy and sleep diaries) and work shifts (work diaries) of 27 HVDs were monitored during their usual work schedule for up to 9 weeks. Differences in TST between consecutive shifts and days off were assessed. Linear mixed models (followed by pairwise comparisons) assessed whether break duration, prior shift duration, time of break onset, and the interaction between break duration and break onset were related to TST between shifts. Investigators found TST between consecutive shifts (mean [SD] 6.38 [1.38] h) was significantly less than on days off (mean [SD] 7.63 [1.93] h; p < 0.001). Breaks starting between 12:01 and 8:00 a.m. led to shorter sleep (p < 0.05) compared to breaks starting between 4:01 and 8:00 p.m. Break durations up to 7, 9, and 11 h (Australian and European minimum break durations) resulted in a mean (SD) of 4.76 (1.06), 5.66 (0.77), and 6.41 (1.06) h of sleep, respectively. The impact of shift duration prior to the break and the interaction between break duration and time of break were not significant. HVDs' sleep between workdays is influenced independently by break duration and time of break onset. This naturalistic study provides evidence that current break regulations prevent sufficient sleep duration in this industry. Work regulations should evaluate appropriate break durations and break onset times to allow longer sleep opportunities for HVDs.


Asunto(s)
Sueño , Tolerancia al Trabajo Programado , Humanos , Australia , Duración del Sueño , Actigrafía
8.
Hum Factors ; : 187208231194543, 2023 Aug 20.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37599390

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: examine the prevalence of driver distraction in naturalistic driving when implementing European New Car Assessment Program (Euro NCAP)-defined distraction behaviours. BACKGROUND: The 2023 introduction of Occupant Status monitoring (OSM) into Euro NCAP will accelerate uptake of Driver State Monitoring (DSM). Euro NCAP outlines distraction behaviours that DSM must detect to earn maximum safety points. Distraction behaviour prevalence and driver alerting and intervention frequency have yet to be examined in naturalistic driving. METHOD: Twenty healthcare workers were provided with an instrumented vehicle for approximately two weeks. Data were continuously monitored with automotive grade DSM during daily work commutes, resulting in 168.8 hours of driver head, eye and gaze tracking. RESULTS: Single long distraction events were the most prevalent, with .89 events/hour. Implementing different thresholds for driving-related and driving-unrelated glance regions impacts alerting rates. Lizard glances (primarily gaze movement) occurred more frequently than owl glances (primarily head movement). Visual time-sharing events occurred at a rate of .21 events/hour. CONCLUSION: Euro NCAP-described driver distraction occurs naturalistically. Lizard glances, requiring gaze tracking, occurred in high frequency relative to owl glances, which only require head tracking, indicating that less sophisticated DSM will miss a substantial amount of distraction events. APPLICATION: This work informs OEMs, DSM manufacturers and regulators of the expected alerting rate of Euro NCAP defined distraction behaviours. Alerting rates will vary with protocol implementation, technology capability, and HMI strategies adopted by the OEMs, in turn impacting safety outcomes, user experience and acceptance of DSM technology.

9.
J Sleep Res ; 31(5): e13545, 2022 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35080060

RESUMEN

Acoustic stimulation has been shown to enhance slow wave sleep and in turn, cognition, and now cardiac outcomes in young adults. With the emergence of commercial acoustic devices in the home, we sought to examine the impact of an acoustic, slow wave enhancing device on heart rate variability in healthy, middle-aged males (n = 24, 39.92 ± 4.15 years). Under highly controlled conditions, the participants were randomised to receive closed-loop brain state-dependent stimulation in the form of auditory tones (STIM), or no tones (SHAM), in a crossover design, separated by a 1 week washout period. STIM and SHAM were compared on measures of heart rate variability for the whole night and over the first three sleep cycles. We found an increase in slow wave activity following STIM compared with SHAM. There was a significant increase in high frequency power and standard deviation of the normalised RR-intervals (SDNN) during the STIM condition compared with SHAM (p < 0.05), due to changes observed specifically during N3. In conclusion, heart rate variability appears to improve following acoustic slow wave sleep enhancement.


Asunto(s)
Sueño de Onda Lenta , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Adulto Joven , Estimulación Acústica , Acústica , Electroencefalografía , Frecuencia Cardíaca , Sueño/fisiología , Sueño de Onda Lenta/fisiología
10.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 116(24): 12019-12024, 2019 06 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31138694

RESUMEN

Before the invention of electric lighting, humans were primarily exposed to intense (>300 lux) or dim (<30 lux) environmental light-stimuli at extreme ends of the circadian system's dose-response curve to light. Today, humans spend hours per day exposed to intermediate light intensities (30-300 lux), particularly in the evening. Interindividual differences in sensitivity to evening light in this intensity range could therefore represent a source of vulnerability to circadian disruption by modern lighting. We characterized individual-level dose-response curves to light-induced melatonin suppression using a within-subjects protocol. Fifty-five participants (aged 18-30) were exposed to a dim control (<1 lux) and a range of experimental light levels (10-2,000 lux for 5 h) in the evening. Melatonin suppression was determined for each light level, and the effective dose for 50% suppression (ED50) was computed at individual and group levels. The group-level fitted ED50 was 24.60 lux, indicating that the circadian system is highly sensitive to evening light at typical indoor levels. Light intensities of 10, 30, and 50 lux resulted in later apparent melatonin onsets by 22, 77, and 109 min, respectively. Individual-level ED50 values ranged by over an order of magnitude (6 lux in the most sensitive individual, 350 lux in the least sensitive individual), with a 26% coefficient of variation. These findings demonstrate that the same evening-light environment is registered by the circadian system very differently between individuals. This interindividual variability may be an important factor for determining the circadian clock's role in human health and disease.


Asunto(s)
Relojes Circadianos/fisiología , Ritmo Circadiano/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Individualidad , Luz , Iluminación/métodos , Masculino , Melatonina/metabolismo , Adulto Joven
11.
J Sports Sci ; 40(20): 2343-2352, 2022 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36512468

RESUMEN

Studies have consistently shown crowds contribute to home advantage in the National Basketball Association (NBA) by inspiring home team effort, distracting opponents, and influencing referees. Quantifying the effect of crowds is challenging, however, due to potential co-occurring drivers of home advantage (e.g., travel, location familiarity). Our aim was to isolate the crowd effect using a "natural experiment" created by the Coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic, which eliminated crowds in 53.4% of 2020/2021 NBA regular season games (N = 1080). Using mixed linear models, we show, in games with crowds, home teams won 58.65% of games and, on average, outrebounded and outscored their opponents. This was a significant improvement compared to games without crowds, of which home teams won 50.60% of games and, on average, failed to outrebound or outscore their opponents. Further, the crowd-related increase in rebound differential mediated the relationship between crowds and points differential. Taken together, these results suggest home advantage in the 2020/2021 NBA season was predominately driven by the presence of home crowds and their influence on the effort exerted to rebound the basketball. These findings are of considerable significance to a league where marginal gains can have immense competitive, financial, and historic consequences.


Asunto(s)
Baloncesto , COVID-19 , Humanos , Viaje
12.
J Sleep Res ; 30(5): e13312, 2021 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33734527

RESUMEN

We aimed to investigate the impact of the Wake Maintenance Zone (WMZ) on measures of drowsiness, attention, and subjective performance under rested and sleep deprived conditions. We studied 23 healthy young adults (18 males; mean age = 25.41 ± 5.73 years) during 40 hr of total sleep deprivation under constant routine conditions. Participants completed assessments of physiological drowsiness (EEG-scored slow eye movements and microsleeps), sustained attention (PVT), and subjective task demands every two hours, and four-hourly ocular motor assessment of inhibitory control (inhibition of reflexive saccades on an anti-saccade task). Tests were analyzed relative to dim light melatonin onset (DLMO); the WMZ was defined as the 3 hr prior to DLMO, and the preceding 3 hr window was deemed the pre-WMZ. The WMZ did not mitigate the adverse impact of ~37 hr sleep deprivation on drowsiness, sustained attention, response inhibition, and self-rated concentration and difficulty, relative to rested WMZ performance (~13 hr of wakefulness). Compared to the pre-WMZ, though, the WMZ improved measures of sustained attention, and subjective concentration and task difficulty, during sleep deprivation. Cumulatively, these results expand on previous work by characterizing the beneficial effects of the WMZ on operationally-relevant indices of drowsiness, inhibitory attention control, and self-rated concentration and task difficulty relative to the pre-WMZ during sleep deprivation. These results may inform scheduling safety-critical tasks at more optimal circadian times to improve workplace performance and safety.


Asunto(s)
Melatonina , Vigilia , Adulto , Atención , Ritmo Circadiano , Humanos , Masculino , Sueño , Privación de Sueño , Adulto Joven
13.
Behav Res Methods ; 53(4): 1488-1501, 2021 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33230709

RESUMEN

The pupillographic sleepiness test (PST) is an accurate predictor of alertness failure and performance impairment across sleep deprivation. At 11 min in duration, the task is considered too long to be used in occupational or roadside settings. We therefore investigated the predictive capacity of the PST at seven shortened test durations. Eighteen healthy young adults (aged 21.4 ± 3.2 years, 10 men) underwent 40 h of continuous wakefulness, completing an 11-min PST and a 10-min psychomotor vigilance task (PVT) every 2 h. Waking electroencephalography was recorded and scored for microsleeps during PVTs. The PST was divided into eight equal 82-s blocks and the predictive capacity of the pupillary unrest index (PUI) calculated at descending PST durations by systematically removing blocks. PUI increased significantly with time awake for all test durations (p < .0001), with a similar amplitude of PUI observed for test durations of 5.5 min and longer. While all test durations accurately predicted PVT impairment (AUC: 0.72-0.86, p < .001) and microsleep (AUC: 0.74-0.84, p < .0001), 5.5 min was the shortest duration where accuracy remained high across level and type of impairment (AUC: 0.79-0.86). For the 5.5-min duration, the positive predictive value (PPV) and negative predictive value (NPV) were on average 50.1% and 89.4%, respectively, and were comparable to the full 11-min task (PPV: 49.2%; NPV: 91%). The PST can be shortened to 5.5 min without compromising accuracy in detecting performance impairment or physiological drowsiness. The PST is an ideal candidate for fitness-for-duty or fitness-to-drive testing, and future studies should examine its predictive capacity, at shorter durations, against operationally relevant outcomes.


Asunto(s)
Somnolencia , Vigilia , Atención , Humanos , Masculino , Desempeño Psicomotor , Pupila , Privación de Sueño , Adulto Joven
14.
Neurobiol Learn Mem ; 166: 107086, 2019 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31491555

RESUMEN

Age-related reductions in slow wave activity (SWA) and increased fragmentation during sleep play a key role in memory impairment. As the prefrontal cortex is necessary for the control processes relevant to memory encoding, including utilisation of internal heuristics such as semantic clustering, and is preferentially vulnerable to sleep disturbance, our study examined how SWA and sleep fragmentation relates to memory performance in individuals with Subjective Cognitive Decline (SCD). Thirty older adults with SCD (Mean Age = 69.34, SD = 5.34) completed a neurocognitive test battery, including the California Verbal Learning Test, which was used to assess semantic clustering. One week later, participants were admitted to the laboratory for a two night visit. SWA and sleep fragmentation were captured using sleep polysomnography. Next-day memory performance was tested using the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test. Poorer sleep (reduced SWA; increased arousals) was associated with reduced semantic clustering, which mediated impairment on verbal memory and learning tests conducted both the day after sleep was recorded (for both SWA and arousals), and a week prior (for arousals only). We demonstrate semantic clustering mediated the well described associations between sleep and verbal memory. As these strategies are a component of cognitive training interventions, future research may examine the role of simultaneous sleep interventions for improving cognitive training outcomes.


Asunto(s)
Disfunción Cognitiva/fisiopatología , Memoria/fisiología , Privación de Sueño/fisiopatología , Sueño/fisiología , Anciano , Ritmo Circadiano/fisiología , Cognición , Disfunción Cognitiva/psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Polisomnografía , Privación de Sueño/psicología
15.
J Sleep Res ; 28(2): e12739, 2019 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30062813

RESUMEN

Pupillary instability reflects alterations in autonomic nervous system activity and has been shown to reflect change in alertness. However, the extent to which it can predict subsequent performance impairment and alertness failure is not clear. Eighteen healthy young adults (group age = 21.44 ± 3.24 years, 10 men) underwent 40 hr of continuous wakefulness, completing an 11-min Pupillographic Sleepiness Test (PST), the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale and a 10-min Psychomotor Vigilance Task (PVT) every 2 hr. Waking electroencephalography was recorded continuously and scored for microsleeps and slow eye movements (SEMs) during PVTs. Pupillary instability was sensitive to time awake, significantly increasing after 18 hr of wakefulness. The time course of impairment was almost identical to PVT lapses, microsleeps and SEMs. Receiver operating characteristic curve analysis demonstrated reasonable sensitivity and specificity of pupillary instability in correctly classifying PVT lapses, microsleeps and SEMs above individual baseline thresholds (all AUC values >0.78, p < 0.0001). Preliminary cut-off scores ranging from 10 to 11.5 mm/min for varying impairment thresholds are proposed for young adults. If reproducible in field settings, the PST may be a strong candidate as a fitness for duty/fitness to drive tool for detecting drowsiness-related impairment.


Asunto(s)
Electroencefalografía/métodos , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Pupila/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Vigilia/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
16.
BMC Psychiatry ; 19(1): 48, 2019 01 29.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30696418

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION: Parents are vulnerable to psychological distress symptoms in the postpartum period. It is routine to screen for depressive symptoms, but anxiety, stress, fatigue, irritability and insomnia symptoms are less often assessed despite their prevalence. This study aimed to assess multiple dimensions of psychological distress, and their reliable change and clinically significant change among women admitted to a residential program for assistance with unsettled infant behaviors (UIB). METHOD: Women admitted to a five-night residential early parenting program completed self-report measures: the Depression Anxiety Stress Scale, Irritability Depression Anxiety Scale, Fatigue Severity Scale, and Insomnia Severity Index. A sub-group completed a computerized emotional Go-NoGo (EGNG) task as a measure of emotional impulsivity. RESULTS: Seventy-eight women were recruited (Mage = 34.46, SDage = 4.16). On admission, 48% of women reported clinically elevated depressive symptoms and 97.5% of women not reporting elevated depressive symptoms reported clinical elevations in at least one other form of distress. Upon discharge, all self-report distress symptoms were significantly reduced (all p-values <.001), but reliable and clinically significant change only occurred in a subgroup of women. There were no significant changes in indicators of impulsivity based on the EGNG. CONCLUSIONS: In addition to, and often in the absence of, depressive symptoms, women attending an early parenting program experienced a wide range of psychological distress, including fatigue, insomnia, anxiety and stress. Different forms of distress improved in different magnitudes to the treatment provided. These findings highlight the need for a multi-dimensional approach in the assessment and treatment of postpartum distress.


Asunto(s)
Depresión Posparto/psicología , Depresión Posparto/terapia , Educación no Profesional/métodos , Responsabilidad Parental/psicología , Estrés Psicológico/psicología , Estrés Psicológico/terapia , Adulto , Depresión Posparto/epidemiología , Femenino , Humanos , Lactante , Pacientes Internos/psicología , Periodo Posparto/psicología , Prevalencia , Distribución Aleatoria , Tratamiento Domiciliario/métodos , Estrés Psicológico/epidemiología , Victoria/epidemiología
17.
Intern Med J ; 49(9): 1181-1184, 2019 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31507045

RESUMEN

The Australian National Road Safety Strategy 2011-2020 framed a 19-point action plan targeting a 30% reduction in road deaths by 2020. In achieving a 9% reduction to date, it is likely to fail this target. Sleep disorders and sleep loss did not feature prominently in this strategy, despite their high prevalence and effect on drowsiness and crashes. This article gathers sleep experts to provide recommendations addressing driver education and legislation to assist the response to this public health problem. Developments in drowsiness detection and sleep disorders management are also presented that offer innovative countermeasures that could enhance road safety beyond 2020.


Asunto(s)
Accidentes de Tránsito/prevención & control , Conducción de Automóvil/educación , Conducción de Automóvil/legislación & jurisprudencia , Conducción de Automóvil/psicología , Trastornos del Sueño-Vigilia/terapia , Australia , Humanos , Fases del Sueño , Trastornos del Sueño-Vigilia/diagnóstico , Vigilia
18.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 113(1): 176-81, 2016 Jan 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26699470

RESUMEN

Night-shift workers are at high risk of drowsiness-related motor vehicle crashes as a result of circadian disruption and sleep restriction. However, the impact of actual night-shift work on measures of drowsiness and driving performance while operating a real motor vehicle remains unknown. Sixteen night-shift workers completed two 2-h daytime driving sessions on a closed driving track at the Liberty Mutual Research Institute for Safety: (i) a postsleep baseline driving session after an average of 7.6 ± 2.4 h sleep the previous night with no night-shift work, and (ii) a postnight-shift driving session following night-shift work. Physiological measures of drowsiness were collected, including infrared reflectance oculography, electroencephalography, and electrooculography. Driving performance measures included lane excursions, near-crash events, and drives terminated because of failure to maintain control of the vehicle. Eleven near-crashes occurred in 6 of 16 postnight-shift drives (37.5%), and 7 of 16 postnight-shift drives (43.8%) were terminated early for safety reasons, compared with zero near-crashes or early drive terminations during 16 postsleep drives (Fishers exact: P = 0.0088 and P = 0.0034, respectively). Participants had a significantly higher rate of lane excursions, average Johns Drowsiness Scale, blink duration, and number of slow eye movements during postnight-shift drives compared with postsleep drives (3.09/min vs. 1.49/min; 1.71 vs. 0.97; 125 ms vs. 100 ms; 35.8 vs. 19.1; respectively, P < 0.05 for all). Night-shift work increases driver drowsiness, degrading driving performance and increasing the risk of near-crash drive events. With more than 9.5 million Americans working overnight or rotating shifts and one-third of United States commutes exceeding 30 min, these results have implications for traffic and occupational safety.


Asunto(s)
Accidentes de Tránsito , Conducción de Automóvil/psicología , Trastornos del Sueño del Ritmo Circadiano/fisiopatología , Fases del Sueño/fisiología , Adulto , Parpadeo/fisiología , Electroencefalografía , Electrooculografía , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Estados Unidos
19.
J Physiol ; 596(24): 6249-6261, 2018 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30281150

RESUMEN

KEY POINTS: This is the first study to demonstrate an altered circadian phase shifting response in a circadian rhythm sleep disorder. Patients with delayed sleep-wake phase disorder (DSWPD) demonstrate greater sensitivity of the circadian system to the phase-delaying effects of light. Increased circadian sensitivity to light is associated with later circadian timing within both control and DSWPD groups. DSWPD patients had a greater sustained pupil response after light exposure. Treatments for DSWPD should consider sensitivity of the circadian system to light as a potential underlying vulnerability, making patients susceptible to relapse. ABSTRACT: Patients with delayed sleep-wake phase disorder (DSWPD) exhibit delayed sleep-wake behaviour relative to desired bedtime, often leading to chronic sleep restriction and daytime dysfunction. The majority of DSWPD patients also display delayed circadian timing in the melatonin rhythm. Hypersensitivity of the circadian system to phase-delaying light is a plausible physiological basis for DSWPD vulnerability. We compared the phase shifting response to a 6.5 h light exposure (∼150 lux) between male patients with diagnosed DSWPD (n = 10; aged 20.8 ± 2.3 years) and male healthy controls (n = 11; aged 22.4 ± 3.3 years). Salivary dim light melatonin onset (DLMO) was measured under controlled conditions in dim light (<3 lux) before and after light exposure. Correcting for the circadian time of the light exposure, DSWPD patients exhibited 31.5% greater phase delay shifts than healthy controls. In both groups, a later initial melatonin phase was associated with a greater magnitude phase shift, indicating that increased circadian sensitivity to light may be a factor that contributes to delayed phase, even in non-clinical groups. DSWPD patients also had reduced pupil size following the light exposure, and showed a trend towards increased melatonin suppression during light exposure. These findings indicate that, for patients with DSWPD, assessment of light sensitivity may be an important factor that can inform behavioural therapy, including minimization of exposure to phase-delaying night-time light.


Asunto(s)
Ritmo Circadiano , Trastornos del Sueño del Ritmo Circadiano , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Melatonina/metabolismo , Adulto Joven
20.
J Sleep Res ; 27(4): e12633, 2018 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29159907

RESUMEN

We aimed to investigate whether self-monitoring of performance is altered during 60 h of total sleep deprivation, following 2 nights of recovery sleep, and by task difficulty and/or subjective sleepiness. Forty adults (22 females, aged 19-39 years) underwent a 5-day protocol, with a well-rested day, 66 h total sleep deprivation (last test session at 60 h), and 2 nights of 8 h recovery sleep. An arithmetic task (MATH) with three difficulty levels assessed working memory. The Psychomotor Vigilance Task assessed sustained attention. Arithmetic accuracy and Psychomotor Vigilance Task median reaction time measured objective performance. Subjective performance was measured with self-reported accuracy and speed. Objective-subjective differences assessed self-monitoring ability. The performance on both tasks declined during total sleep deprivation and improved following recovery. During total sleep deprivation, participants accurately self-monitored performance on the Psychomotor Vigilance Task; however, they overestimated cognitive deficits on MATH, self-reporting performance as worse than actually observed. Following recovery, participants overestimated the extent of performance improvement on the Psychomotor Vigilance Task. Task difficulty influenced self-monitoring ability, with greater overestimation of performance deficits during total sleep deprivation as difficulty increased. Subjective sleepiness predicted subjective performance ratings at several time points, only for the Psychomotor Vigilance Task. The ability to self-monitor performance was impaired during total sleep deprivation for working memory and after recovery sleep for the Psychomotor Vigilance Task, but was otherwise accurate. The development of self-monitoring strategies, assessing both subjective perceptions of performance and subjective sleepiness, within operational contexts may help reduce the consequences of sleep-related impairments.


Asunto(s)
Cognición/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Recuperación de la Función/fisiología , Privación de Sueño/psicología , Sueño/fisiología , Adulto , Atención/fisiología , Disfunción Cognitiva/fisiopatología , Disfunción Cognitiva/psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Descanso/fisiología , Descanso/psicología , Autoinforme , Privación de Sueño/fisiopatología , Factores de Tiempo , Vigilia/fisiología , Adulto Joven
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA