Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 48
Filtrar
Más filtros

Bases de datos
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Mem Cognit ; 52(1): 57-72, 2024 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37440162

RESUMEN

The production effect-that reading aloud leads to better memory than does reading silently-has been defined narrowly with reference to memory; it has been explored largely using word lists as the material to be read and remembered. But might the benefit of production extend beyond memory and beyond individual words? In a series of four experiments, passages from reading comprehension tests served as the study material. Participants read some passages aloud and others silently. After each passage, they completed multiple-choice questions about that passage. Separating the multiple-choice questions into memory-focused versus comprehension-focused questions, we observed a consistent production benefit only for the memory-focused questions. Production clearly improves memory for text, not just for individual words, and also extends to multiple-choice testing. The overall pattern of findings fits with the distinctiveness account of production-that information read aloud stands out at study and at test from information read silently. Only when the tested information is a very close match to the studied information, as is the case for memory questions but not for comprehension questions, does production improve accuracy.


Asunto(s)
Comprensión , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Humanos , Lectura , Recuerdo Mental , Proyectos de Investigación
2.
Memory ; 31(7): 905-917, 2023 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37165509

RESUMEN

ABSTRACTWe report three experiments designed to reveal the mechanisms that underlie subjective experiences of recognition by examining effects of how those experiences are measured. Prior research has explored the potential influences of collecting metacognitive measures on memory performance. Building on this work, here we systematically evaluated whether cross-measure contamination occurs when remember-know (RK) and/or confidence (C) judgments are made after old/new recognition decisions. In Experiment 1, making either RK or C judgments did not significantly influence recognition relative to a standard no-judgment condition. In Experiment 2, making RK judgments in addition to C judgments did not significantly affect recognition or confidence. In Experiment 3, making C judgments in addition to RK judgments did not significantly affect recognition or patterns of RK responses. Cross-contamination was not apparent regardless of whether items were studied using a shallow or deep levels-of-processing task - a manipulation that yielded robust effects on recognition, RK judgments, and C. Our results indicate that under some conditions, participants can independently evaluate their recognition, subjective recognition experience, and confidence. Though contamination across measures of metamemory and memory is always possible, it may not be inevitable. This has implications for the mechanisms that underlie subjective experiences that accompany recognition judgments.


Asunto(s)
Recuerdo Mental , Metacognición , Humanos , Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Juicio/fisiología , Cognición
3.
Brain Cogn ; 152: 105757, 2021 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34130081

RESUMEN

Recognition memory is improved for items produced at study (e.g., by reading them aloud) relative to a non-produced control condition (e.g., silent reading). This production effect is typically attributed to the extra elements in the production task (e.g., motor activation, auditory perception) enhancing item distinctiveness. To evaluate this claim, the present study examined the neural mechanisms underlying the production effect. Prior to a recognition memory test, different words within a study list were read either aloud, silently, or while saying "check" (as a sensorimotor control condition). Production improved recognition, and aloud words yielded higher rates of both recollection and familiarity judgments than either silent or control words. During encoding, fMRI revealed stronger activation in regions associated with motor, somatosensory, and auditory processing for aloud items than for either silent or control items. These activations were predictive of recollective success for aloud items at test. Together, our findings are compatible with a distinctiveness-based account of the production effect, while also pointing to the possible role of other processing differences during the aloud trials as compared to silent and control.


Asunto(s)
Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Humanos , Juicio , Recuerdo Mental , Lectura
4.
Psychol Res ; 85(1): 280-290, 2021 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31463566

RESUMEN

Prior research has emphasized that performing distinctive encoding on a subset of lists in the DRM paradigm suppresses false recognition; we show that its benefits can be mitigated by costs and spillover effects. Within groups read half the DRM lists and solved anagrams for the other half using a strategy that emphasized either item-specific or relational processing. Their recognition was compared to three pure-list control groups (read, item-specific generation, relational generation). Correct recognition in the within groups showed a benefit for generate items and a cost for read items, resulting in little net improvement relative to pure reading. False recognition in the within groups was reduced following item-specific vs. relational generation, but there was again little net improvement. Most surprisingly, false recognition in the within groups was greater for generate than read lists. This pattern suggests that relational processing of read lists spilled over to generate lists, boosting false recognition for generate lists. Distinctive encoding of a subset of items does not appear to globally improve memory accuracy.


Asunto(s)
Cognición/fisiología , Memoria/fisiología , Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Lectura , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Vocabulario , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
5.
Mem Cognit ; 48(7): 1281-1294, 2020 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32399916

RESUMEN

Relative to reading silently, reading words aloud (a type of "production") typically enhances item recognition, even when production is manipulated between groups using pure lists. We investigated whether pure-list production also enhances memory for various item details (i.e., source memory). Screen side (Experiment 1), font size (Experiment 2), or reading versus generating from anagrams (Experiments 3-4) were the sources varied within-subject, and aloud versus silent reading was varied across groups. Thus, the manipulation of source was apparent to participants, whereas the manipulation of production was not. Traditional measures and multinomial modeling established that the aloud groups generally showed improved item recognition-and showed improved source memory when steps were taken to enhance the salience of the source manipulation (Experiment 4). In summary, reading an entire list of items improves item recognition and can also improve memory for some types of source details.


Asunto(s)
Memoria , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Humanos , Lectura
6.
Behav Brain Sci ; 42: e284, 2020 01 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31896358

RESUMEN

The integrative memory model combines five core memory systems with an attributional system. We agree with Bastin et al. that this melding is the most novel aspect of the model. But we await further evidence that the model's substantial complexity informs our understanding of false memories or of the development of recollection and familiarity.


Asunto(s)
Recuerdo Mental , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Humanos , Memoria , Trastornos de la Memoria
7.
Memory ; 25(5): 647-655, 2017 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27387376

RESUMEN

Studying Deese-Roediger-McDermott (DRM) lists using a distinctive encoding task can reduce the DRM false memory illusion. Reductions for both distinctively encoded lists and non-distinctively encoded lists in a within-group design have been ascribed to use of a distinctiveness heuristic by which participants monitor their memories at test for distinctive-task details. Alternatively, participants might simply set a more conservative response criterion, which would be exceeded by distinctive list items more often than all other test items, including the critical non-studied items. To evaluate these alternatives, we compared a within-group who studied 5 lists by reading, 5 by anagram generation, and 5 by imagery, relative to a control group who studied all 15 lists by reading. Generation and imagery improved recognition accuracy by impairing relational encoding, but the within group did not show greater memory monitoring at test relative to the read control group. Critically, the within group's pattern of list-based source judgments provided new evidence that participants successfully monitored for distinctive-task details at test. Thus, source judgments revealed evidence of qualitative, recollection-based monitoring in the within group, to which our quantitative signal-detection measure of monitoring was blind.


Asunto(s)
Imaginación/fisiología , Juicio/fisiología , Memoria/fisiología , Detección de Señal Psicológica/fisiología , Humanos , Lectura , Vocabulario
8.
Conscious Cogn ; 33: 78-89, 2015 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25543993

RESUMEN

The influence of test context on reports of recollection and familiarity depends on how these subjective recognition experiences are conceptualized and measured. Bodner and Lindsay (2003) found that critical items elicited more remember judgments but fewer know judgments in a less (vs. more) memorable context. In contrast, Tousignant and Bodner (2012) found that independent ratings of recollection and familiarity were both higher in a less memorable context. We replicated the dissociative pattern with judgments using recollect/familiar labels (Experiment 1), and in a novel R/F/B task that added a "both" option to eliminate the mutual exclusivity between the recollect and familiar options (Experiment 2). Adding a "guess" option eliminated these context effects (Experiment 3), however whether allowing guesses "cleans up" or "desensitizes" recollection and familiarity judgments remains unclear. Determining which task variants provide appropriate measures of subjective recognition experiences will require an examination of additional dissociations and triangulation with other measures.


Asunto(s)
Recuerdo Mental , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Discriminación en Psicología , Humanos , Juicio
9.
Percept Mot Skills ; 119(1): 59-68, 2014 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25153739

RESUMEN

Unconscious priming is sensitive to contextual factors. The present study examined this adaptive process using masked arrow primes (<< or >>). Some targets required specific "fixed" left/right responses (<< or >>) and others required "free" left/right responses (<>). Different groups (n = 30 each) received response-congruent primes (SOA = 75 msec.) on 0.2, 0.5, or 0.8 of the fixed-response trials. Fixed responses were facilitated by congruent primes and free responses were faster when congruent with the prime. Critically, these masked priming effects emerged only in the 0.8 group. The pattern of extant prime-proportion effects in this paradigm best supports an adaptive associative-strength account rather than memory-recruitment or response-bias-suppression accounts.


Asunto(s)
Enmascaramiento Perceptual/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Memoria Implícita/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Distribución Aleatoria , Adulto Joven
10.
Cortex ; 165: 57-69, 2023 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37267658

RESUMEN

The production effect (PE) is the finding that reading words aloud rather than silently during study leads to improved memory. We used electroencephalography (EEG) techniques to detect the contributions of recollection, familiarity, and attentional processes to the PE in recognition memory, using Chinese stimuli. During the study phase, participants encoded each list item aloud, silently, or by performing a non-unique aloud (control) task. During the test phase, they made remember/know/new recognition judgments. We recorded EEG data in both phases. The behavioral results replicated the typical pattern with English stimuli: Recognition was better in the aloud condition than in the silent (and control) condition, and this PE was due to enhanced recollection and familiarity. At study, the amplitude of the P3b ERP component was greater in the aloud than in the silent/control conditions, suggesting that reading aloud increases attention or preparatory processing during the intention phase. At test, the recollection-based LPC old/new effect was largest in the aloud condition; however, the familiarity-based FN400 old/new effect was equivalent between the aloud condition and the silent/control conditions. Only the LPC effect correlated with the behavioral effect. Moreover, multivariate pattern analysis (MVPA) showed that accurate classification of items as 'aloud' versus 'new' mainly occurred in the later period of the recognition response, consistent with the LPC old/new effect. Our findings suggest that the within-subject PE in recognition memory reflects enhanced attention and distinctiveness, rather than increased memory strength. More broadly, our findings suggest that encoding strategies such as production enhance recollection more than familiarity.


Asunto(s)
Recuerdo Mental , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Humanos , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Electroencefalografía , Atención , Lectura
11.
Conscious Cogn ; 21(2): 994-1000, 2012 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22309813

RESUMEN

The binary remember/know task requires participants to dichotomize their subjective recognition experiences into those with recollection and those only with familiarity. Many variables have produced dissociative effects on remember/know judgments. In contrast, having participants make independent recollection/familiarity ratings has consistently produced parallel effects, suggesting the dissociations may be artifacts of using binary judgments. Bodner and Lindsay (2003) reported a test-list context effect with binary judgments: Increased remembering but decreased knowing for a set of critical items tested with a set of less-memorable (vs. more-memorable) items. Here we report a parallel effect of test-list context on recollection and familiarity ratings, induced by a shift in response bias. We argue that independent ratings are preferable to binary judgments because they allow participants to directly report the co-occurrence of recollection and familiarity for each item. Implications for the measurement of self-reported recognition experiences, and for accounts of recognition memory, are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Reconocimiento en Psicología , Discriminación en Psicología , Humanos , Recuerdo Mental , Detección de Señal Psicológica , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas
12.
Can J Exp Psychol ; 76(3): 186-192, 2022 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35549359

RESUMEN

Pupil dilation provides a window into recognition memory processes. During a recognition test, the pupil dilates more in response to a recognized studied item than to a correctly rejected new item. Various explanations for this pupil old/new effect have been offered. By a retrieval effort account, the pupil's response on a recognition test reflects the cognitive effort needed to retrieve items from memory. By a memory strength account, pupil dilation reflects the strength of the subjective memory experience elicited by items at test. To compare these accounts, we varied levels of processing (LOP) at study, then measured pupil dilation on a delayed recognition test during which participants made recollection/familiarity judgements. Pupil dilation at test was similar whether test items had been studied in a deep or shallow LOP task, but was greater when deep, shallow, and new test items were experienced as recollected rather than as familiar. This pattern supports the memory strength account rather than the retrieval effort account of pupil dilation during a recognition test. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Pupila , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Humanos , Juicio , Memoria/fisiología , Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Pupila/fisiología , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología
13.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 29(6): 2256-2263, 2022 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35819588

RESUMEN

The production effect-better memory for words read aloud rather than silently-has been attributed to responses at test being guided by memory for the act of production. In Experiment 1, we evaluated this distinctiveness account by comparing production effects in forced-choice recognition when lures were either homophones of the targets (toad or towed?) or unrelated words (toad or seam?). If the production effect at test was driven solely by memory for the productive act (e.g., articulation, auditory processing), then the effect should be reduced with homophone lures. Contrary to that prediction, the production effect did not differ credibly between homophone-lure and unrelated-lure groups. Experiment 1 led us to hypothesize that production may also boost semantic encoding, and that participants use memory of semantic encoding to guide their forced-choice responses. Consistent with these hypotheses, using synonym lures to interfere with semantic-based decisions (poison or venom?) reduced the production effect relative to using unrelated lures (poison or ethics?) in Experiment 2. Our findings suggest that enhanced conceptual encoding may be another useful product of production.


Asunto(s)
Venenos , Semántica , Humanos , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Lectura , Percepción Auditiva
14.
Memory ; 18(7): 763-73, 2010 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20924949

RESUMEN

Costs and benefits of directed forgetting are observed when a between-list instruction to forget List 1 impairs List 1 recall while enhancing List 2 recall. These effects are often ascribed to intentional inhibition of List 1. Contrary to this inhibition account, we found that a forget instruction did not produce costs unless an explicit instruction to concentrate on List 2 was used (Experiment 1). Alternatively, costs may be ascribed to a shift in mental context between encoding and retrieval. Consistent with this mental context-change account, an unexpected task (wiping the computer screen and one's hands) produced costs comparable to a forget instruction, as did as a brief chat between lists (Experiment 2). A number-search task between lists produced neither costs nor benefits (Experiment 3), suggesting that mere distraction is insufficient for inducing mental context change. Our findings support the claim that mental context change underlies both intentional and unintentional forgetting.


Asunto(s)
Memoria/fisiología , Atención/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Lectura , Aprendizaje Verbal , Adulto Joven
15.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 46(11): 2207-2225, 2020 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32658545

RESUMEN

Based on the classic distinction between semantic and episodic memory, people answer general-knowledge questions by querying their semantic memory. And yet, an appeal of trivia games is the variety of memory experiences they arouse-including the recollection of episodic details. We report the first in-depth exploration of the memory states that arise for recalled answers to general-knowledge questions. In 2 experiments, participants classified their answers as learning memory or related memory forms of recollection, as feels familiar or just know forms of nonrecollection, or as a guess. A recollection state was reported for nearly half of the correct answers. Learning memory, related memory, and just know states showed similarly high accuracy and confidence-whereas the feels familiar state was much lower. The differences between familiarity and knowing highlight the importance of distinguishing these oft-conflated states. Our study establishes that episodic memory often contributes to retrieval of general-knowledge, and that the memory states arising during retrieval can be diagnostic of accuracy. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Memoria Episódica , Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Metacognición/fisiología , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
16.
Front Psychol ; 11: 602347, 2020.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33329270

RESUMEN

In the Deese-Roediger/McDermott (DRM) paradigm, distinctive encoding of list items typically reduces false recognition of critical lures relative to a read-only control. This reduction can be due to enhanced item-specific processing, reduced relational processing, and/or increased test-based monitoring. However, it is unclear whether distinctive encoding reduces false recognition in a selective or global manner. To examine this question, participants studied DRM lists using a distinctive item-specific anagram generation task and then completed a recognition test which included both DRM critical lures and either strongly related lures (Experiment 1) or weakly related lures (Experiment 2). Compared to a read-control group, the generate groups showed increased correct recognition and decreased false recognition of all lure types. We then estimated the separate contributions of encoding and retrieval processes using signal-detection indices. Generation improved correct recognition by both increasing encoding of memory information for list words and by increasing memory monitoring at test. Generation reduced false recognition by reducing the encoding of memory information and by increasing memory monitoring at test. The reduction in false recognition was equivalent for critical lures and related lures, indicating that generation globally reduces the encoding of related non-presented items at study (not just critical lures), while globally increasing list-theme-based monitoring at test.

17.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 73(3): 425-441, 2020 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31561745

RESUMEN

Many studies have demonstrated retrieval-enhanced suggestibility (RES), in which taking an initial recall test after witnessing an event increases suggestibility to subsequent misinformation introduced via a narrative. Recently, however, initial testing has been found to have a protective effect against misinformation introduced via cued-recall questions. We examined whether misinformation format (narrative vs. cued-recall questions) yields a similar dissociation in a paradigm that, to date, has consistently yielded a protective effect of testing (PET). After studying photos of household scenes (e.g., kitchen), some participants took an initial recall test. After a 48-hr delay, items not presented in the scenes (e.g., knives/plates) were suggested either via narrative or questions. Regardless of the misinformation format, we found a PET on both initial-test-conditionalised free recall and source-monitoring tests. However, initial testing also yielded memory costs, such that suggested items reported on the initial test were likely to persist on a final recall test. Thus, initial testing can protect against suggestibility, but can also precipitate memory errors when intrusions emerge on an initial test.


Asunto(s)
Señales (Psicología) , Decepción , Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Práctica Psicológica , Sugestión , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Narración , Adulto Joven
18.
Nutrients ; 12(6)2020 Jun 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32498296

RESUMEN

Short food questions (SFQ) allow for rapid reporting of food intake across a variety of settings but are limited by poor validity and reliability. Understanding the recall process used by parents to report children's food intake can improve question design and psychometric performance. This study aimed to improve understanding of how parents report children's dietary intake using SFQ. Semi-structured, cognitive interviews were conducted with 21 mothers of 3-7-year-old children. Mothers were asked to 'think-aloud' while answering SFQ about their child's food intake. Thematic analysis identified themes relating to parent's question and answer process and barriers to recall. Information retrieval strategies focused on 'use-of-time' and 'sphere of food provision' and differed for core versus unhealthy foods. Recall of routine and home food provision were used to report core food intake, whereas recall of special occasions and food provision outside the home guided recall of discretionary foods. Mothers utilize different recall strategies for core and discretionary foods based on use of time and the sphere of food provision. The ease of reporting children's dietary intake may be improved by utilizing a shorter recall time frame, clear and direct question wording, and use of food examples and recall prompts.


Asunto(s)
Ingestión de Alimentos/psicología , Conducta Alimentaria/psicología , Recuerdo Mental , Relaciones Madre-Hijo/psicología , Madres/psicología , Adulto , Niño , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Entrevista Psicológica , Masculino , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
19.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 16(3): 497-502, 2009 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19451375

RESUMEN

Masked repetition priming is often greater when a larger proportion of trials involve repetition primes, suggesting that a context-sensitive unconscious process may be operating. Two recent studies have failed to obtain an effect of prime proportion in the perceptual identification (PI) task, suggesting that the effect may not occur in nonspeeded tasks. Contrary to this possibility, we report proportion effects with masked repetition primes in two nonspeeded tasks: PI and fragment completion (FC). A proportion effect occurred in the accuracy measure only in the FC task, but it occurred in the reaction time measure in both tasks. Prior failures to find proportion effects in the PI task thus may have been due, in part, to the dependent measure used. We interpret our findings in light of several recent accounts of prime proportion effects with brief primes.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Señales (Psicología) , Recuerdo Mental , Aprendizaje por Asociación de Pares , Enmascaramiento Perceptual , Tiempo de Reacción , Inconsciente en Psicología , Toma de Decisiones , Humanos , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Semántica
20.
Mem Cognit ; 37(8): 1069-76, 2009 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19933452

RESUMEN

Witnesses sometimes report event details that are acquired solely from another witness. We reevaluated the potency of this memory conformity effect. After viewing a crime video, some participants learned about nonwitnessed details via discussion (dyad group), reading another participant's report (read group), or watching another version of the video (both-video group). In Experiment 1, these participants often reported nonwitnessed details, but on a source-judgment test most details were attributed primarily to the actual source rather than to the video. In addition, the dyad group was not more likely than the read or both-video groups to report nonwitnessed details. Participants in Experiment 2 were explicitly discouraged from providing details that were remembered from the secondary source only. These postwarning instructions substantially reduced the memory conformity effect, and a dyad group was not more likely than a read group to report nonwitnessed details. Encouraging source monitoring at test can reduce the negative consequences of co-witness collaboration.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Recuerdo Mental , Conformidad Social , Sugestión , Robo/psicología , Grabación en Video , Percepción Visual , Discriminación en Psicología , Humanos , Imaginación , Juicio
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA