Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 7 de 7
Filtrar
1.
Child Dev ; 85(1): 220-36, 2014.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23638912

RESUMEN

Stress responses to social evaluation are thought to increase during adolescence, which may be due to pubertal maturation. However, empirical evidence is scarce. This study is the first to investigate the relation between pubertal development and biological responses to a social-evaluative stressor longitudinally. Participants performed the Leiden Public Speaking Task twice, with a 2-year interval (N = 217; age at Time 1: 8-17 years). The results support an increase in sensitivity to social evaluation during adolescence. The overall cortisol and alpha-amylase responses increased-both between and within participants-and were more strongly related to self-reported pubertal development than to age. The cortisol response shifted from speech delivery toward anticipation. The alpha-amylase response increased in both phases.


Asunto(s)
Hidrocortisona/metabolismo , Pubertad/fisiología , alfa-Amilasas Salivales/metabolismo , Conducta Social , Estrés Psicológico/metabolismo , Adolescente , Desarrollo del Adolescente/fisiología , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Estrés Psicológico/enzimología
2.
J Adolesc ; 32(4): 1009-21, 2009 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18992936

RESUMEN

Common folklore seems to suggest that adolescents are particularly susceptible to peer influence. However, from the literature the exact age differences in susceptibility to peer influence remain unclear. The current study's main focus was to chart the development of general susceptibility to peer pressure in a community sample of 10-18 year olds (N =464) with the recently developed Resistance to Peer Influence Scale (RPI). The one-factor structure of the RPI was cross-validated in the present sample, and the RPI was equally reliable at all ages. As expected, general resistance to peer influence increased during adolescence. In addition, gender differences were most pronounced during mid-adolescence, when girls were more resistant to peer influence than boys. These findings are explained in terms of psychosocial maturation during adolescence.


Asunto(s)
Conducta del Adolescente , Conducta de Elección , Grupo Paritario , Poder Psicológico , Adolescente , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Países Bajos , Psicometría , Factores Sexuales , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
3.
J Anxiety Disord ; 22(1): 135-42, 2008.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17339097

RESUMEN

The (in)variance of the structure of fear across child and adolescent development was investigated using the response of 3,803 young people (aged 6-18 years) to the Fear Survey Schedule for Children-Revised (FSSC-R). It was hypothesized that the structure of fear, particularly social fear, would become more differentiated during adolescence. Confirmatory factor analysis was utilized to test the goodness-of-fit of a 5-factor solution (with one social factor, that is Failure and Criticism) and a 7-factor solution (with social items divided into three distinct subfactors) among three age groups. Results indicated that a 5-factor solution (including just one social factor) adequately characterizes the structure of fear in childhood (6-9 years), whereas a 7-factor solution (including three social factors) more appropriately characterizes the structure of fear in adolescence (10-13 and 14-18 years). It appears that a global social fear exists until around the age of 9, and that social fears becomes more differentiated thereafter.


Asunto(s)
Desarrollo Humano , Trastornos Fóbicos/psicología , Pruebas Psicológicas , Adolescente , Factores de Edad , Niño , Análisis Factorial , Humanos , Países Bajos , Trastornos Fóbicos/diagnóstico , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados
4.
Dev Psychol ; 42(6): 1026-40, 2006 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17087539

RESUMEN

The current article presents results from a twin study of genetic and environmental components of maternal sensitivity and infant attachment and their association. The sample consisted of 136 twin pairs from 2 sites: Leiden, the Netherlands, and London, UK. Maternal sensitivity was assessed in the home at 9-10 months, and infant attachment security was observed in the laboratory at 12 months. The study yielded little evidence that genetic factors are involved in variations between twins in maternal sensitivity ratings but did find that shared variance in maternal sensitivity was able to account for some of the similarity between twins in attachment security. Weak nonshared associations between sensitivity and attachment appeared to suppress the magnitude of the correlation between attachment and sensitivity in twin children. The results could indicate that the attachment security of one twin may depend on the relationship the parent has with the other twin. The results are brought to bear on the validity of attachment theory as a theory of primarily shared environmental effects in children's development and the continuing challenge posed to attachment theory by within-family differences in socioemotional processes.


Asunto(s)
Genética Conductual , Conducta Materna/psicología , Relaciones Madre-Hijo , Apego a Objetos , Adulto , Animales , Femenino , Humanos , Lactante , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Modelos Psicológicos , Estudios en Gemelos como Asunto
5.
Behav Res Ther ; 47(12): 1043-9, 2009 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19682671

RESUMEN

The current study investigated whether negatively biased self-evaluations of nervousness and social skills are related to how well an individual actually performs, that is performance level. Sixty-eight high socially anxious and 68 control participants (age range 9-17 years) gave a 5 min speech in front of a pre-recorded audience of same age peers and a teacher. Participants' evaluations immediately after the task were measured on a number of performance dimensions. Three independent observers also evaluated recordings of the speech performances. Participants were further divided into good and bad performers based on their actual performance level as judged by the observers. Self-evaluations of the high socially anxious participants were negatively biased for nervous appearance regardless of how well they actually performed. In contrast, a negative bias for social skills only occurred in the high anxious participants with a good performance. The social skill evaluations of the poor performers appear warranted. Taking actual performance level into account may help to clarify the exact nature of a negative bias in socially anxious youth and has clear implications for the choice of treatment approach.


Asunto(s)
Ansiedad de Desempeño/psicología , Autoevaluación (Psicología) , Habla , Adolescente , Actitud , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
6.
Biol Psychol ; 82(2): 116-24, 2009 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19576261

RESUMEN

This study describes a new public speaking protocol for youth. The main question asked whether a speech prepared at home and given in front of a pre-recorded audience creates a condition of social-evaluative threat. Findings showed that, on average, this task elicits a moderate stress response in a community sample of 83 12- to 15-year-old adolescents. During the speech, participants reported feeling more nervous and having higher heart rate and sweatiness of the hands than at baseline or recovery. Likewise, physiological (heart rate and skin conductance) and neuroendocrine (cortisol) activity were higher during the speech than at baseline or recovery. Additionally, an anticipation effect was observed: baseline levels were higher than recovery levels for most variables. Taking the anticipation and speech response together, a substantial cortisol response was observed for 55% of participants. The findings indicate that the Leiden Public Speaking Task might be particularly suited to investigate individual differences in sensitivity to social-evaluative situations.


Asunto(s)
Hidrocortisona/metabolismo , Medio Social , Habla , Estrés Psicológico/metabolismo , Estrés Psicológico/fisiopatología , Conducta Verbal/fisiología , Adolescente , Análisis de Varianza , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Dimensión del Dolor , Saliva/metabolismo , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas
7.
Child Dev ; 74(6): 1769-82, 2003.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14669895

RESUMEN

In a sample of 157 monozygotic and dizygotic twins, genetic and environmental influences on infant attachment and temperament were quantified. Only unique environmental or error components could explain the variance in disorganized versus organized attachment as assessed in the Ainsworth Strange Situation Procedure. For secure versus nonsecure attachment, 52% of the variance in attachment security was explained by shared environment, and 48% of the variance was explained by unique environmental factors and measurement error. The role of genetic factors in attachment disorganization and attachment security was negligible. Genetic factors explained 77% of the variance in temperamental reactivity, and unique environmental factors and measurement error explained 23%. Differences in temperamental reactivity were not associated with attachment concordance.


Asunto(s)
Genética Conductual , Relaciones Madre-Hijo , Apego a Objetos , Medio Social , Gemelos/psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Individualidad , Lactante , Londres , Masculino , Países Bajos , Responsabilidad Parental/psicología , Determinación de la Personalidad/estadística & datos numéricos , Psicometría , Factores de Riesgo , Temperamento , Gemelos/genética
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA