Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 53
Filtrar
Más filtros

Bases de datos
País/Región como asunto
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Brain Inj ; 38(5): 361-367, 2024 Apr 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38329033

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: Metacognition and quality of life (QoL) are both adversely affected by traumatic brain injury (TBI), but the relation between them is not fully understood. As such, the purpose of this study was to determine the degree to which metacognitive accuracy predicts QoL in individuals with TBI. METHODS: Eighteen participants with moderate-to-severe TBI completed a stimulus-response task requiring the discrimination of emotions depicted in pictures of faces and then provided a retrospective confidence judgment after each response. Metacognitive accuracy was calculated using participants' response accuracy and confidence judgment accuracy. Participants also completed the Quality of Life After Brain Injury (QOLIBRI) questionnaire to assess QoL in various areas of functioning. RESULTS: Performance of a linear regression analysis revealed that higher metacognitive accuracy significantly predicted lower overall QoL. Additionally, higher metacognitive accuracy significantly predicted lower QoL related to cognition and physical limitations. CONCLUSION: The study results provide evidence of an inverse relation between metacognitive performance and QoL following TBI. Metacognitive changes associated with TBI and their relation to QoL have several clinical implications for TBI rehabilitation.


Asunto(s)
Lesiones Traumáticas del Encéfalo , Metacognición , Humanos , Calidad de Vida/psicología , Autoinforme , Estudios Retrospectivos , Lesiones Traumáticas del Encéfalo/psicología
2.
Psychol Res ; 86(5): 1683-1697, 2022 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34480245

RESUMEN

In the present article, we examine a novel illusion of motion-the Z-Box illusion-in which the presence of a bounding object influences the perception of motion of an ambiguous stimulus that appears within. Specifically, the stimuli are a structure-from-motion (SFM) particle orb and a wireframe cube. The orb could be perceived as rotating clockwise or counterclockwise while the cube could only be perceived as moving in one direction. Both stimuli were presented on a two-dimensional (2D) display with inferred three-dimensional (3D) properties. In a single experiment, we examine motion perception of a particle orb, both in isolation and when it appears within a rotating cube. Participants indicated the orb's direction of motion and whether the direction changed at any point during the trial. Accuracy was the critical measure while motion direction, the number of particles in the orb and presence of the wireframe cube were all manipulated. The results suggest that participants could perceive the orb's true rotation in the absence of the cube so long as it was made up of at least ten particles. The presence of the cube dominated perception as participants consistently perceived congruent motion of the orb and cube, even when they moved in objectively different directions. These findings are considered as they relate to prior research on motion perception, computational modelling of motion perception, structure from motion and 3D object perception.


Asunto(s)
Ilusiones , Percepción de Movimiento , Humanos , Movimiento (Física) , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Rotación
3.
J Vis ; 21(7): 9, 2021 07 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34264288

RESUMEN

Previous attempts to classify task from eye movement data have relied on model architectures designed to emulate theoretically defined cognitive processes and/or data that have been processed into aggregate (e.g., fixations, saccades) or statistical (e.g., fixation density) features. Black box convolutional neural networks (CNNs) are capable of identifying relevant features in raw and minimally processed data and images, but difficulty interpreting these model architectures has contributed to challenges in generalizing lab-trained CNNs to applied contexts. In the current study, a CNN classifier was used to classify task from two eye movement datasets (Exploratory and Confirmatory) in which participants searched, memorized, or rated indoor and outdoor scene images. The Exploratory dataset was used to tune the hyperparameters of the model, and the resulting model architecture was retrained, validated, and tested on the Confirmatory dataset. The data were formatted into timelines (i.e., x-coordinate, y-coordinate, pupil size) and minimally processed images. To further understand the informational value of each component of the eye movement data, the timeline and image datasets were broken down into subsets with one or more components systematically removed. Classification of the timeline data consistently outperformed the image data. The Memorize condition was most often confused with Search and Rate. Pupil size was the least uniquely informative component when compared with the x- and y-coordinates. The general pattern of results for the Exploratory dataset was replicated in the Confirmatory dataset. Overall, the present study provides a practical and reliable black box solution to classifying task from eye movement data.


Asunto(s)
Movimientos Oculares , Redes Neurales de la Computación , Humanos , Movimientos Sacádicos
4.
J Vis ; 20(6): 16, 2020 06 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32579673

RESUMEN

Object-based warping is a powerful visual illusion wherein space between features within figural regions is regularly overestimated compared with those within ground regions. Originally, the effect was only examined in displays of two-dimensional (2D) stimuli. The present study sought to examine whether object-based warping persists in more naturalistic viewing conditions, where additional contextual cues are present. Stimuli were presented with either three-dimensional (3D) printed objects (Experiment 1) or 3D objects in virtual reality (Experiments 2-4). The testing metric was actual distance of features (dots) compared with estimated distances made by participants. Responses for the 3D printed stimuli were measured with replica dots on a slide ruler device. The virtual reality experiments collected responses either with a computer mouse or motion-tracked controller and included manipulations of object type, spatial separation, viewing distance of stimuli, and head motion. A standard warping effect in 3D was observed in all experiments, although the effect was not present in one condition that elicits warping in 2D (Occluded Rectangle). The final experiment resolves this discrepancy by reducing the multicomponent object (Occluded Rectangle) to a single component figure, while demonstrating the influence of depth cues on the warping effect under occlusion. Collectively, these experiments reveal that object-based warping is a powerful effect, even in naturalistic settings.


Asunto(s)
Ilusiones/fisiología , Imagenología Tridimensional , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Atención/fisiología , Señales (Psicología) , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Realidad Virtual , Vías Visuales/fisiología
5.
J Vis ; 19(1): 7, 2019 01 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30650435

RESUMEN

The motion-induced contour (MIC) was first described by Victor Klymenko and Naomi Weisstein in a series of papers in the 1980s. The effect is created by rotating the outline of a tilted cube in depth. When one of the vertical edges is removed, an illusory contour can be seen in its place. In four experiments, we explored which stimulus features influence perceived illusory contour strength. Participants provided subjective ratings of illusory contour strength as a function of orientation of the stimulus, separation between inducing edges, and the length of inducing edges. We found that the angle of tilt of the object in depth had the largest impact on perceived illusory contour strength with tilt angles of 20° and 30° producing the strongest percepts. Tilt angle is an unexplored feature of structure-from-motion displays. In addition, we found that once the depth structure of the object was extracted, other features of the display, such as the distance spanned by the illusory contour, could also influence its strength, similar to the notion of support ratio for 2-D illusory contours. Illusory contour strength was better predicted by the length of the contour in 3-D rather than in 2-D, suggesting that MICs are constructed by a 3-D process that takes as input initially recovered contour orientation and position information in depth and only then forms interpolations between them.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Forma/fisiología , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Ilusiones Ópticas/fisiología , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Percepción de Profundidad/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Orientación , Adulto Joven
6.
J Neurophysiol ; 119(5): 1629-1635, 2018 05 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29364069

RESUMEN

Neuroimaging-based investigations of change blindness, a phenomenon in which seemingly obvious changes in visual scenes fail to be detected, have significantly advanced our understanding of visual awareness. The vast majority of prior investigations, however, utilize paradigms involving visual disruptions (e.g., intervening blank screens, saccadic movements, "mudsplashes"), making it difficult to isolate neural responses toward visual changes cleanly. To address this issue in this present study, high-density EEG data (256 channel) were collected from 25 participants using a paradigm in which visual changes were progressively introduced into detailed real-world scenes without the use of visual disruption. Oscillatory activity associated with undetected changes was contrasted with activity linked to their absence using standardized low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (sLORETA). Although an insufficient number of detections were present to allow for analysis of actual change detection, increased beta-2 activity in the right inferior parietal lobule (rIPL), a region repeatedly associated with change blindness in disruption paradigms, followed by increased theta activity in the right superior temporal gyrus (rSTG) was noted in undetected visual change responses relative to the absence of change. We propose the rIPL beta-2 activity to be associated with orienting attention toward visual changes, with the subsequent rise in rSTG theta activity being potentially linked with updating preconscious perceptual memory representations. NEW & NOTEWORTHY This study represents the first neuroimaging-based investigation of gradual change blindness, a visual phenomenon that has significant potential to shed light on the processes underlying visual detection and conscious perception. The use of gradual change materials is reflective of real-world visual phenomena and allows for cleaner isolation of signals associated with the neural registration of change relative to the use of abrupt change transients.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Concienciación/fisiología , Ondas Encefálicas/fisiología , Electroencefalografía/métodos , Neuroimagen/métodos , Lóbulo Parietal/fisiopatología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Lóbulo Temporal/fisiopatología , Tomografía/métodos , Trastornos de la Visión/fisiopatología , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Lóbulo Parietal/diagnóstico por imagen , Lóbulo Temporal/diagnóstico por imagen , Trastornos de la Visión/diagnóstico por imagen , Adulto Joven
7.
Cogn Emot ; 31(4): 772-780, 2017 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26963703

RESUMEN

Surprised expressions are interpreted as negative by some people, and as positive by others. When compared to fearful expressions, which are consistently rated as negative, surprise and fear share similar morphological structures (e.g. widened eyes), but these similarities are primarily in the upper part of the face (eyes). We hypothesised, then, that individuals would be more likely to interpret surprise positively when fixating faster to the lower part of the face (mouth). Participants rated surprised and fearful faces as either positive or negative while eye movements were recorded. Positive ratings of surprise were associated with longer fixation on the mouth than negative ratings. There were also individual differences in fixation patterns, with individuals who fixated the mouth earlier exhibiting increased positive ratings. These findings suggest that there are meaningful individual differences in how people process faces.


Asunto(s)
Emociones/fisiología , Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Individualidad , Sesgo , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Adulto Joven
8.
Exp Brain Res ; 234(9): 2653-65, 2016 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27161551

RESUMEN

The serotonin system is heavily involved in cognitive and emotional control processes. Previous work has typically investigated this system's role in control processes separately for cognitive and emotional domains, yet it has become clear the two are linked. The present study, therefore, examined whether variation in a serotonin receptor gene (HTR2A, rs6313) moderated effects of emotion on inhibitory control. An emotional antisaccade task was used in which participants looked toward (prosaccade) or away (antisaccade) from a target presented to the left or right of a happy, angry, or neutral face. Overall, antisaccade latencies were slower for rs6313 C allele homozygotes than T allele carriers, with no effect of genotype on prosaccade latencies. Thus, C allele homozygotes showed relatively weak inhibitory control but intact reflexive control. Importantly, the emotional stimulus was either present during target presentation (overlap trials) or absent (gap trials). The gap effect (slowed latency in overlap versus gap trials) in antisaccade trials was larger with angry versus neutral faces in C allele homozygotes. This impairing effect of negative valence on inhibitory control was larger in C allele homozygotes than T allele carriers, suggesting that angry faces disrupted/competed with the control processes needed to generate an antisaccade to a greater degree in these individuals. The genotype difference in the negative valence effect on antisaccade latency was attenuated when trial N-1 was an antisaccade, indicating top-down regulation of emotional influence. This effect was reduced in C/C versus T/_ individuals, suggesting a weaker capacity to downregulate emotional processing of task-irrelevant stimuli.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Emociones/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Receptor de Serotonina 5-HT2A/genética , Movimientos Sacádicos/genética , Adolescente , Adulto , Cara , Expresión Facial , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Adulto Joven
9.
Psychol Res ; 79(2): 183-93, 2015 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24558017

RESUMEN

Individuals are highly sensitive to statistical regularities in their visual environment, even when these patterns do not reach conscious awareness. Here, we examine whether oculomotor behavior is systematically altered when distractor/target configurations rarely repeat, but target location on an initial trial predicts the location of a target on the subsequent trial. The purpose of the current study was to explore whether this temporal-spatial contextual cueing in a conjunction search task influences both reaction time to the target and participant's search strategy. Participants searched for a target through a gaze-contingent window in a display consisting of a large number of distractors, providing a target-present/absent response. Participants were faster to respond to the target on the predicted trial relative to the predictor trial in an implicit contextual cueing task but were no more likely to fixate first to the target quadrant on the predicted trial (Experiment 1). Furthermore, implicit learning was interrupted when instructing participants to vary their searching strategy across trials to eliminate visual scan similarity (Experiment 2). In Experiment 3, when participants were explicitly informed that a pattern was present at the start of the experiment, explicit learning was observed in both reaction time and eye movements. The present experiments provide evidence that implicit learning of sequential regularities regarding target locations is not based on learning more efficient scan paths, but is due to some other mechanism.


Asunto(s)
Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Adulto , Señales (Psicología) , Humanos , Adulto Joven
10.
Cogn Behav Ther ; 44(5): 377-88, 2015.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25767901

RESUMEN

A growing theoretical and research literature suggests that trait and state social anxiety can predict attentional patterns in the presence of emotional stimuli. The current study adds to this literature by examining the effects of state anxiety on visual attention and testing the vigilance-avoidance hypothesis, using a method of continuous visual attentional assessment. Participants were 91 undergraduate college students with high or low trait fear of negative evaluation (FNE), a core aspect of social anxiety, who were randomly assigned to either a high or low state anxiety condition. Participants engaged in a free view task in which pairs of emotional facial stimuli were presented and eye movements were continuously monitored. Overall, participants with high FNE avoided angry stimuli and participants with high state anxiety attended to positive stimuli. Participants with high state anxiety and high FNE were avoidant of angry faces, whereas participants with low state and low FNE exhibited a bias toward angry faces. The study provided partial support for the vigilance-avoidance hypothesis. The findings add to the mixed results in the literature that suggest that both positive and negative emotional stimuli may be important in understanding the complex attention patterns associated with social anxiety. Clinical implications and suggestions for future research are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Ansiedad , Atención , Emociones , Expresión Facial , Trastornos Fóbicos , Adolescente , Reacción de Prevención , Medidas del Movimiento Ocular , Cara , Femenino , Fijación Ocular , Humanos , Masculino , Teoría Psicológica , Percepción Visual , Adulto Joven
11.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 85(2): 494-504, 2023 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35708846

RESUMEN

Previous research has demonstrated that individuals exhibit a tendency to overestimate the variability of both low-level features (e.g., color, orientation) and mid-level features (e.g., size) when items are presented dynamically in a sequential order, a finding we will refer to as the variability overestimation effect. Because previous research on this bias used sequential displays, an open question is whether the effect is due to a memory-related bias or a vision-related bias. To assess whether the bias would also be apparent with static, simultaneous displays, and to examine whether the bias generalizes to spatial properties, we tested participants' perception of the variability of a cluster of dots. Results showed a consistent overestimation bias: Participants judged the dots as being more spread than they actually were. The variability overestimation effect was observed when there were 10 or 20 dots but not when there were 50 dots. Taken together, the results of the current study contribute to the ensemble perception literature by providing evidence that simultaneously presented stimuli are also susceptible to the variability overestimation effect. The use of static displays further demonstrates that this bias is present in both dynamic and static contexts, suggesting an inherent bias existent in the human visual system. A potential theoretical account-boundary effect-is discussed as a potential underlying mechanism. Moreover, the present study has implications for common visual tasks carried out in real-world scenarios, such as a radiologist making judgments about distribution of calcification in breast cancer diagnoses.


Asunto(s)
Juicio , Humanos , Sesgo
12.
Neuropsychologia ; 177: 108428, 2022 12 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36414100

RESUMEN

Cognitive control allows individuals to flexibly and efficiently perform tasks by attending to relevant stimuli while inhibiting distraction from irrelevant stimuli. The antisaccade task assesses cognitive control by requiring participants to inhibit a prepotent glance towards a peripheral stimulus and generate an eye movement to the mirror image location. This task can be administered with various contextual manipulations to investigate how factors such as trial timing or emotional content interact with cognitive control. In the current study, 26 healthy adults completed a mixed antisaccade and prosaccade fMRI task that included task irrelevant emotional faces and gap/overlap timing. The results showed typical antisaccade and gap behavioral effects with greater BOLD activation in frontal and parietal brain regions for antisaccade and overlap trials. Conversely, there were no differences in behavior based on the emotion of the task irrelevant face, but trials with neutral faces had greater activation in widespread visual regions than trials with angry faces, particularly for prosaccade and overlap trials. Together, these effects suggest that a high level of cognitive control and inhibition was required throughout the task, minimizing the impact of the face presentation on saccade behavior, but leading to increased attention to the neutral faces on overlap prosaccade trials when both the task cue (look towards) and emotion stimulus (neutral, non-threatening) facilitated disinhibition of visual processing.


Asunto(s)
Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Movimientos Sacádicos , Adulto , Humanos , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Emociones
13.
J Med Imaging (Bellingham) ; 9(3): 035503, 2022 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35692281

RESUMEN

Purpose: Diagnosing breast cancer based on the distribution of calcifications is a visual task and thus prone to visual biases. We tested whether a recently discovered visual bias that has implications for breast cancer diagnosis would be present in expert radiologists, thereby validating the concern of this bias for accurate diagnoses. Approach: We ran a vision experiment with expert radiologists and untrained observers to test the presence of visual bias when judging the spread of dots that resembled calcifications and when judging the spread of line orientations. We calculated visual bias scores for both groups for both tasks. Results: Participants overestimated the spread of the dots and the spread of the line orientations. This bias, referred to as the variability overestimation effect, was of similar magnitudes in both expert radiologists and untrained observers. Even though the radiologists were better at both tasks, they were similarly biased compared with the untrained observers. Conclusions: The results justify the concern of the variability overestimation effect for accurate diagnoses based on breast calcifications. Specifically, the bias is likely to lead to an increased number of false-negative results, thereby leading to delayed treatments.

14.
Psychol Res ; 75(1): 2-9, 2011 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20379741

RESUMEN

Three experiments are reported examining whether the presentation of irrelevant negative numbers at central fixation interacts with attentional orienting beyond fixation. It has been previously shown that number perception influences spatial attention, with the presentation of spatially nonpredictive numbers resulting in the allocation of attention to the left when the number is low (e.g., 1 or 2) and to the right when the number is high (e.g., 8 or 9). In the present experiment, it is examined whether this attentional spatial numerical association of response codes (SNARC) effect is influenced by the presentation of negative numbers, which should have spatial properties that are in direct opposition to their positive counterparts (e.g., -1 or -2 would be considered high numbers relative to -8 or -9, which would be considered low numbers). Though the presentation of negative numbers does not lead to a reversal of the attentional SNARC effect, it does lead to an elimination of the effect, providing insight into how the attentional SNARC effect develops.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Pesos y Medidas , Análisis de Varianza , Señales (Psicología) , Humanos , Nebraska , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Detección de Señal Psicológica , Estudiantes/psicología
15.
J Vis ; 11(8): 17, 2011 Jul 28.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21799023

RESUMEN

The purpose of the present study was to examine the influence of task set on the spatial and temporal characteristics of eye movements during scene perception. In previous work, when strong control was exerted over the viewing task via specification of a target object (as in visual search), task set biased spatial, rather than temporal, parameters of eye movements. Here, we find that more participant-directed tasks (in which the task establishes general goals of viewing rather than specific objects to fixate) affect not only spatial (e.g., saccade amplitude) but also temporal parameters (e.g., fixation duration). Further, task set influenced the rate of change in fixation duration over the course of viewing but not saccade amplitude, suggesting independent mechanisms for control of these parameters.


Asunto(s)
Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas , Adulto Joven
16.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 82(7): 3329-3339, 2020 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32617862

RESUMEN

It has been repeatedly demonstrated that when performing a visual search task, items can pop out of a display such that they are identified rapidly, independent of the number of distractors present. It has been less clear whether this type of pop-out is limited to static displays (e.g., images) or whether it can also occur in scenes containing movement, more akin to how we experience the real world. Recently, Jardine and Moore (Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42, 617-630, 2016) examined whether pop-out also occurs in displays consisting of dynamic motion - wherein items in the display rotated continuously until a critical frame that would elicit pop-out under static presentation conditions - and found that search was greatly impaired. It remains unclear, however, whether such impairment is exerted equivalently across all types of dynamic motions or if it is specific to orientation. In the present study, we replicate the original Jardine and Moore (Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42, 617-630, 2016) finding and extend this examination to another dimension - color change. We also explore whether search efficiency can be improved with dynamic context if aspects of the display become predictable. The results suggest that not all types of dynamic change impair search performance. Specifically, oddball color targets continue to pop out even when the items in the display are dynamic. Interestingly, adding predictable context did not aid search accuracy as expected, rather resulting in poorer performance. Taken together, the findings suggest that the influence of dynamic context on search performance is not absolute.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Percepción de Color , Humanos , Orientación , Tiempo de Reacción
17.
Psychol Sci ; 20(3): 333-9, 2009 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19222812

RESUMEN

We report a study that examined whether inhibition of return (IOR) is specific to visual search or a general characteristic of visual behavior. Participants were shown a series of scenes and were asked to (a) search each scene for a target, (b) memorize each scene, (c) rate how pleasant each scene was, or (d) view each scene freely. An examination of saccadic reaction times to probes provided evidence of IOR during search: Participants were slower to look at probes at previously fixated locations than to look at probes at novel locations. For the other three conditions, however, the opposite pattern of results was observed: Participants were faster to look at probes at previously fixated locations than to look at probes at novel locations, a facilitation-of-return effect that has not been reported previously. These results demonstrate that IOR is a search-specific strategy and not a general characteristic of visual attention.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Exploratoria , Inhibición Psicológica , Percepción Visual , Humanos , Tiempo de Reacción , Movimientos Sacádicos
18.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 35(1): 133-45, 2009 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19170476

RESUMEN

The authors describe a new visual illusion first discovered in a natural setting. A cyclist riding beside a pair of sagging chains that connect fence posts appears to move up and down with the chains. In this illusion, a static shape (the chains) affects the perception of a moving shape (the bicycle), and this influence involves assimilation (averaging) rather than opposition (differentiation). These features distinguish the illusion from illusions of motion capture and induced motion. The authors take this bicycle illusion into the laboratory and report 4 findings: Naïve viewers experience the illusion when discriminating horizontal from sinusoidal motion of a disc in the context of stationary curved lines; the illusion shifts from motion assimilation to motion opposition as the visual size of the display is increased; the assimilation and opposition illusions are dissociated by variations in luminance contrast of the stationary lines and the moving disc; and the illusion does not occur when simply comparing two stationary objects at different locations along the curved lines. The bicycle illusion provides a unique opportunity for studying the interactions between shape and motion perception.


Asunto(s)
Ciclismo , Percepción de Forma , Percepción de Movimiento , Ilusiones Ópticas , Humanos
19.
Vision Res ; 157: 202-212, 2019 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30243641

RESUMEN

Recent reports have shown that individuals from small hometowns show relatively poor face recognition ability as measured by the Cambridge Face Memory Test or CFMT (Balas & Saville, 2015, 2017), suggesting that the number of faces present in an individual's visual environment relates to that individual's face recognition ability. We replicate this finding in a sample from a different region (Nebraska) and with more variable age distribution. We extend the study by using another test of face recognition ability that does not require learning over trials, and with non-face object recognition tests that share the learning format with the CFMT. We find no hometown effect in these other tests, although more power would be required to show the CFMT effect is significantly larger. We use the same dataset to explore whether experience with more faces and cars in larger hometowns leads to specialization of these abilities. We find strong and substantial support for the hypothesis that the recognition abilities for faces and for cars are more independent from general object recognition in people from larger hometowns. This suggests that experience may be critical to the specialization of these abilities.


Asunto(s)
Automóviles , Reconocimiento Facial/fisiología , Percepción de Forma/fisiología , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Características de la Residencia , Adulto Joven
20.
Vision Res ; 163: 14-23, 2019 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31472340

RESUMEN

Several studies have found that individuals from small hometowns show diminished face recognition ability as compared with individuals from larger hometowns. We further this line of research by relating six measures of face recognition ability to hometown density. We predicted that the three face recognition ability measures which included a learning component would relate to hometown density whereas the three measures which did not include such a learning component would not. Instead, we found that none of the six measures related to hometown density. Interestingly, we found interactions between gender and hometown population density on many of these measures and on a general index of face recognition, with females from small hometowns outperforming males from small hometowns but no such differences in the large hometown group. In a follow-up re-analysis of a previous study, we found a similar interaction in one of two face recognition ability measures. Together, these results reveal a pattern of gender differences modulated by hometown population density. If indeed experience with faces in one's hometown influences face recognition ability, understanding these effects may require more than a quantification of the environment. Men and women growing up in the same environment likely have different experiences, which likely modulates effects on visual abilities.


Asunto(s)
Reconocimiento Facial/fisiología , Densidad de Población , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Aprendizaje , Masculino , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Factores Sexuales , Adulto Joven
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA