Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 28
Filtrar
1.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 35(11): 1693-1715, 2023 11 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37677060

RESUMEN

There has been a long-lasting debate about whether salient stimuli, such as uniquely colored objects, have the ability to automatically distract us. To resolve this debate, it has been suggested that salient stimuli do attract attention but that they can be suppressed to prevent distraction. Some research supporting this viewpoint has focused on a newly discovered ERP component called the distractor positivity (PD), which is thought to measure an inhibitory attentional process. This collaborative review summarizes previous research relying on this component with a specific emphasis on how the PD has been used to understand the ability to ignore distracting stimuli. In particular, we outline how the PD component has been used to gain theoretical insights about how search strategy and learning can influence distraction. We also review alternative accounts of the cognitive processes indexed by the PD component. Ultimately, we conclude that the PD component is a useful tool for understanding inhibitory processes related to distraction and may prove to be useful in other areas of study related to cognitive control.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Aprendizaje , Humanos , Atención/fisiología , Inhibición Psicológica , Estimulación Luminosa , Electroencefalografía , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología
2.
Psychol Sci ; 30(12): 1724-1732, 2019 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31693453

RESUMEN

Previous research suggests that observers can suppress salient-but-irrelevant stimuli in a top-down manner. However, one question left unresolved is whether such suppression is, in fact, solely due to distractor-feature suppression or whether it instead also reflects some degree of target-feature enhancement. The present study (N = 60) addressed this issue. On search trials (70% of trials), participants searched for a shape target when an irrelevant color singleton was either present or absent; performance was better when a color singleton was present. On interleaved probe trials (30% of trials), participants searched for a letter target. Responses were faster for the letter on a target-colored item than on a neutral-colored item, whereas responses were slower for the letter on a distractor-colored item than on a neutral-colored item. The results demonstrate that target-feature enhancement and distractor-feature suppression contribute to attentional guidance independently; enhancement and suppression flexibly guide attention as the occasion demands.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Percepción de Color/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa/instrumentación , Color , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto Joven
3.
Psychol Sci ; 27(4): 476-85, 2016 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26893292

RESUMEN

Previous research indicates that prior information about a target feature, such as its color, can speed search. Can search also be speeded by knowing what a target willnotlook like? In the two experiments reported here, participants searched for target letters. Prior to viewing search displays, participants were prompted either with the color in which one or more nontarget letters would appear (ignore trials) or with no information about the search display (neutral trials). Critically, when participants were given one consistent color to ignore for the duration of the experiment, compared with when they were given no information, there was a cost in reaction time (RT) early in the experiment. However, after extended practice, RTs on ignore trials were significantly faster than RTs on neutral trials, which provides a novel demonstration that knowledge about nontargets can improve search performance for targets. When the to-be-ignored color changed from trial to trial, no RT benefit was observed.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Inhibición Psicológica , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Tiempo de Reacción , Color , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
4.
Learn Mem ; 22(11): 563-6, 2015 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26472646

RESUMEN

Previous work suggests that visual long-term memory (VLTM) is highly detailed and has a massive capacity. However, memory performance is subject to the effects of the type of testing procedure used. The current study examines detail memory performance by probing the same memories within the same subjects, but using divergent probing methods. The results reveal that while VLTM representations are typically sufficient to support performance when the procedure probes gist-based information, they are not sufficient in circumstances when the procedure requires more detail. We show that VLTM capacity, albeit large, is heavily reliant on gist as well as detail. Thus, the nature of the mnemonic representations stored in VLTM is important in understanding its capacity limitations.


Asunto(s)
Memoria a Largo Plazo , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Percepción Visual , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
5.
Psychol Sci ; 25(2): 315-24, 2014 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24390823

RESUMEN

Attention can modulate processing of visual input according to task-relevant features, even as early as approximately 100 ms after stimulus presentation. In the present study, event-related potential and behavioral data revealed that inhibition of distractor features, rather than activation of target features, is the primary driver of early feature-based selection in human observers. This discovery of inhibition consistent with task goals during early visual processing suggests that inhibition plays a much larger role at an earlier stage of target selection than previously recognized. It also highlights the importance of understanding the role of inhibition (in addition to activation) in attention.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Percepción de Color/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados Visuales/fisiología , Inhibición Psicológica , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
6.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 50(10): 1033-1047, 2024 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39172365

RESUMEN

Do salient distractors have the power to automatically capture attention? This question has led to a heated debate concerning the role of salience in attentional control. A potential resolution, called the signal suppression hypothesis, has proposed that salient items produce a bottom-up signal that vies for attention, but that salient stimuli can be suppressed via top-down control to prevent the capture of attention. This hypothesis, however, has been criticized on the grounds that the distractors used in initial studies of support were weakly salient. It has been difficult to know how seriously to take this low-salience criticism because assertions about high and low salience were made in the absence of a common (or any) measure of salience. The current study used a recently developed psychophysical technique to compare the salience of distractors from two previous studies at the center of this debate. Surprisingly, we found that the original stimuli criticized as having low salience were, if anything, more salient than stimuli from the later studies that purported to increase salience. Follow-up experiments determined exactly why the original stimuli were more salient and tested whether further improving salience could cause attentional capture as predicted by the low-salience account. Ultimately, these findings challenge purely stimulus-driven accounts of attentional control. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Atención , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Humanos , Atención/fisiología , Adulto , Adulto Joven , Femenino , Masculino , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Adolescente
7.
J Cogn ; 6(1): 39, 2023.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37426056

RESUMEN

There has been a lengthy debate about whether salient stimuli have the power to automatically capture attention, even when entirely task irrelevant. Theeuwes (2022) has suggested that an attentional window account could explain why capture is observed in some studies, but not others. According to this account, when search is difficult, participants narrow their attentional window, and this prevents the salient distractor from generating a saliency signal. In turn, this causes the salient distractor to fail to capture attention. In the present commentary, we describe two major problems with this account. First, the attentional window account proposes that attention must be focused so narrowly that featural information from the salient distractor will be filtered prior to saliency computations. However, many previous studies observing no capture provided evidence that featural processing was sufficiently detailed to guide attention toward the target shape. This indicates that the attentional window was sufficiently broad to allow featural processing. Second, the attentional window account proposes that capture should occur more readily in easy search tasks than difficult search tasks. We review previous studies that violate this basic prediction of the attentional window account. A more parsimonious account of the data is that control over feature processing can be exerted proactively to prevent capture, at least under certain conditions.

8.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 30(6): 2262-2271, 2023 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37231178

RESUMEN

Researchers have long debated whether salient distractors have the power to automatically capture attention. Recent research has suggested a potential resolution, called the signal suppression hypothesis, whereby salient distractors produce a bottom-up salience signal, but can be suppressed to prevent visual distraction. This account, however, has been criticized on the grounds that previous studies may have used distractors that were only weakly salient. This claim has been difficult to empirically test because there are currently no well-established measures of salience. The current study addresses this by introducing a psychophysical technique to measure salience. First, we generated displays that aimed to manipulate the salience of two color singletons via color contrast. We then verified that this manipulation was successful using a psychophysical technique to determine the minimum exposure duration required to detect each color singleton. The key finding was that high-contrast singletons were detected at briefer exposure thresholds than low-contrast singletons, suggesting that high-contrast singletons were more salient. Next, we evaluated the participants' ability to ignore these singletons in a task in which they were task irrelevant. The results showed that, if anything, high-salience singletons were more strongly suppressed than low-salience singletons. These results generally support the signal suppression hypothesis and refute claims that highly salient singletons cannot be ignored.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Humanos , Tiempo de Reacción , Investigadores
9.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 83(1): 260-269, 2021 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33241528

RESUMEN

Although it is often assumed that a physically salient stimulus automatically captures attention even when it is irrelevant to a current task, the signal-suppression hypothesis proposes that observers can actively suppress a salient-but-irrelevant distractor. However, it is still unknown whether suppression alone (i.e., without target enhancement) is potent enough to override attentional capture by a salient singleton in an otherwise-homogeneous background. The current study addressed this issue. On search trials (70% of trials), participants searched for a shape target on trials that either did or did not contain an irrelevant color singleton. The effects of learning to suppress the color of the singleton were examined on interleaved probe trials (30% of trials). On these trials, participants searched for a probe target letter; those letters were presented on four ovals (one colored oval and three gray ovals). Each colored oval was a singleton that was one of three types: the color of the distractor on search trials, the color of the target on search trials, or a neutral color that had not appeared on search trials. Responses were faster for the probe target on a neutral-colored or target-colored item than on a gray-colored item; however, responses were slower for the probe target on a distractor-colored item than on a gray-colored item. The results demonstrate a powerful suppression mechanism overriding attentional capture by a singleton item.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Color , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Atención , Humanos , Aprendizaje , Tiempo de Reacción
10.
Vis cogn ; 29(9): 587-591, 2021.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34707459

RESUMEN

Strong evidence supporting the top-down modulation of attention has come from studies in which participants learned to suppress a singleton in a heterogeneous four-item display. These studies have been criticized on the grounds that the displays are so sparse that the singleton is not actually salient. We argue that similar evidence of suppression has been found with substantially larger displays where salience is not in question. Additionally, we examine the results of applying salience models to four-item displays, and find prominent markers of salience at the location of the singleton. We conclude that small heterogeneous displays do not preclude strong salience signals. Beyond that, we reflect on how further basic research on salience may speed resolution of the attentional capture debate.

11.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 34(2): 268-85, 2008 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18377170

RESUMEN

The authors report a newly identified intertrial priming phenomenon, within-dimension singleton priming, by which search for a target that happens to be a singleton on the current trial is faster when the target on the previous trial had also been a singleton on the same dimension rather than a nonsingleton. This effect was replicated in 6 experiments with different procedures, with singletons on various dimensions, when the featural contrast defining the singleton remained the same or changed within a dimension from one trial to the next, and when the target was a singleton on a target-defining dimension or on an irrelevant dimension. These findings cannot be explained by previously demonstrated intertrial repetition effects such as dimension-specific priming or priming of pop-out. Theoretical implications of the within-dimension singleton priming phenomenon are discussed relative to the dimension-weighting hypothesis, the role of stimulus-driven salience in feature-guided search, and the roles of intertrial priming and goal-directed factors in visual search.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Señales (Psicología) , Humanos , Valores de Referencia
12.
Psychol Res ; 72(1): 106-13, 2008 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16897098

RESUMEN

The biased-competition theory of attention proposes that objects compete for cortical representation in a mutually inhibitory network; competition is biased in favor of the attended item. Here we test two predictions derived from the biased-competition theory. First we assessed whether increasing an object's relative brightness (luminance contrast) biased competition in favor of (i.e., prioritized) the brighter object. Second we assessed whether increasing an object's size biased competition in favor of the larger object. In fulfillment of these aims we used an attentional capture paradigm to test whether a featural singleton (an item unique with respect to a feature such as size or brightness) can impact attentional priority even when those features are irrelevant to finding the target. The results support the prediction that a singleton with respect to luminance contrast receives attentional prioritization and extend the biased-competition account to include size contrast, because a large singleton also receives attentional prioritization.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Luz , Percepción del Tamaño , Percepción Visual , Humanos
13.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 25(2): 586-595, 2018 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29075994

RESUMEN

Inborn preference for palatable energy-dense food is thought to be an evolutionary adaptation. One way this preference manifests itself is through the control of visual attention. In the present study, we investigated how attentional capture is influenced by changes in naturally occurring goal-states, in this case desire for energy-dense foods (typically high fat and/or high sugar). We demonstrate that even when distractors are entirely irrelevant, participants were significantly more distracted by energy-dense foods compared with non-food objects and even low-energy foods. Additionally, we show the lability of these goal-states by having a separate set of participants consume a small amount of calorie-dense food prior to the task. The amount of distraction by the energy-dense food images in this case was significantly reduced and no different than distraction by images of low-energy foods and images of non-food objects. While naturally occurring goal-states can be difficult to ignore, they also are highly flexible.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Grasas de la Dieta , Azúcares de la Dieta , Alimentos , Percepción Visual , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
14.
Curr Biol ; 14(19): R850-2, 2004 Oct 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15458666

RESUMEN

Visual attention is attracted by salient stimuli that 'pop out' from their surroundings. Attention can also be voluntarily directed to objects of current importance to the observer. What happens in the brain when these two processes interact?


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Modelos Psicológicos , Vías Visuales/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Percepción de Color , Humanos , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Factores de Tiempo , Campos Visuales/fisiología
15.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 13(3): 524-9, 2006 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17048741

RESUMEN

The literature contains conflicting results concerning whether an irrelevant featural singleton (an item unique with respect to a feature such as color or brightness) can control attention in a stimulus-driven manner. The present study explores whether target-nontarget similarity influences stimulus-driven shifts of attention to a distractor. An experiment evaluated whether manipulating target-nontarget similarity by varying orientation would modulate distraction by an irrelevant feature (a bright singleton). We found that increasing target-nontarget similarity resulted in a decreased impact of a uniquely bright object on visual search. This method of manipulating the target-nontarget similarity independent of the salience of a distracting feature suggests that the extent to which visual attention is stimulus-driven depends on the target-nontarget similarity.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Exploratoria , Tiempo de Reacción , Percepción Visual , Atención , Humanos
16.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 13(1): 132-8, 2006 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16724780

RESUMEN

Bacon and Egeth (1994) proposed that observed instances of attentional capture by feature singletons (e.g., color) were the result of a salience-based strategy adopted by subjects (singleton detection mode) and, thus, were not automatic. They showed that subjects could override capture by adopting strategies based on searching for specific target features (feature search mode). However, Theeuwes (2004) has recently argued that Bacon and Egeth's results arose from experimental confounds. He elaborated a model in which attentional capture must be expected when salient distractors fall within a spatial window of attention. According to Theeuwes's (2004) model, there exist two essential criteria for examining stimulus-driven capture. First, search latencies cannot increase with display size, since the search must be parallel; second, the salience of the irrelevant distractor must not be compromised by characteristics of the search display. Contrary to the predictions of Theeuwes's (2004) model, we provide evidence that involuntary capture can be overridden when both of these criteria are met. Our results are consistent with Bacon and Egeth's proposal.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Percepción de Color , Aprendizaje Discriminativo , Orientación , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Toma de Decisiones , Humanos , Modelos Psicológicos , Tiempo de Reacción , Disposición en Psicología
17.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 13(2): 287-93, 2006 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16892996

RESUMEN

Recent literature suggests that observers can use advance knowledge of the target feature to guide their search but fail to do so whenever the target is reliably a singleton. Instead, they engage in singleton-detection mode--that is, they search for the most salient object. In the present study, we aimed to test the notion of a default salience-based search mode. Using several measures, we compared search for a known target when it is always a singleton (fixed-singleton search) relative to when it is incidentally a singleton (multiple-target search). We examined the relative contributions of strategic factors (knowledge that the target is a singleton) and intertrial repetition effects (singleton priming, or the advantage of responding to a singleton target if the target on the previous trial had also been a singleton). In two experiments, singleton priming eliminated all the differences in performance between fixed-singleton and multiple-target search, suggesting that search for a known singleton may be feature based rather than salience based.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Exploratoria , Tiempo de Reacción , Percepción Visual , Fijación Ocular , Humanos , Detección de Señal Psicológica
18.
Vis cogn ; 23(9-10): 1098-1123, 2015.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27504073

RESUMEN

When participants search for a shape (e.g., a circle) among a set of homogenous shapes (e.g., triangles) they are subject to distraction by color singletons that are more salient than the target. However, when participants search for a shape among heterogeneous shapes, the presence of a non-target color singleton does not slow responses to the target. Attempts have been made to explain these results from both bottom-up and top-down perspectives. What both accounts have in common is that they do not predict the occurrence of attentional capture on typical feature search displays. Here, we present a case where manipulating selection history, rather than the displays themselves, leads to attentional capture on feature search trials. The ability to map specific colors to the target and distractor appears to be what enables resistance to capture during feature search.

19.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 29(5): 1003-20, 2003 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14585019

RESUMEN

Six experiments were conducted to determine the circumstances under which an irrelevant singleton captures attention. Subjects searched for a target while ignoring a salient distractor that appeared at different stimulus onset asynchronies (SOAs) prior to each search display. Spatial congruency and interference effects were measured. The strategies available to find the target were controlled (only singleton-detection mode, only feature-search mode, or both search strategies available). An irrelevant abrupt onset captured attention in search for a color target, across SOAs, whatever strategies were available. In contrast, in search for a shape target, an irrelevant color singleton captured attention in the singleton-detection condition but delayed response at its location in the feature-search condition, across SOAs. When both strategies were available, capture was short lived (50- to 100-msec SOAs). The theoretical implications of these findings in relation to current views on attentional capture are discussed. ((c) 2003 APA, all rights reserved)


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Área de Dependencia-Independencia , Inhibición Psicológica , Procesos Mentales/fisiología , Enmascaramiento Perceptual/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Color , Señales (Psicología) , Discriminación en Psicología , Humanos , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Estimulación Luminosa , Tiempo de Reacción
20.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 30(6): 1019-31, 2004 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15584812

RESUMEN

Attentional allocation in feature-search mode (W. F. Bacon & H. E. Egeth, 1994) is thought to be solely determined by top-down factors, with no role for stimulus-driven salience. The authors reassessed this conclusion using variants of the spatial cuing and rapid serial visual presentation paradigms developed by C. L. Folk and colleagues (C. L. Folk, R. W. Remington, & J. C. Johnston, 1992; C. L. Folk, A. B. Leber, & H. E. Egeth, 2002). They found that (a) a nonsingleton distractor that possesses the target feature produces attentional capture, (b) such capture is modulated by bottom-up salience, and (c) resistance to capture by irrelevant singletons is mediated by inhibitory processes. These results extend the role of top-down factors in search for a nonsingleton target while arguing against the notion that effects of bottom-up salience and top-down factors on attentional priority are strictly encapsulated within distinct search modes.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Detección de Señal Psicológica , Percepción Visual , Computadores , Fijación Ocular , Humanos , Tiempo de Reacción
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA