Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 49
Filtrar
Más filtros

Bases de datos
País/Región como asunto
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Nature ; 565(7738): 202-205, 2019 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30626942

RESUMEN

White dwarfs are stellar embers depleted of nuclear energy sources that cool over billions of years1. These stars, which are supported by electron degeneracy pressure, reach densities of 107 grams per cubic centimetre in their cores2. It has been predicted that a first-order phase transition occurs during white-dwarf cooling, leading to the crystallization of the non-degenerate carbon and oxygen ions in the core, which releases a considerable amount of latent heat and delays the cooling process by about one billion years3. However, no direct observational evidence of this effect has been reported so far. Here we report the presence of a pile-up in the cooling sequence of evolving white dwarfs within 100 parsecs of the Sun, determined using photometry and parallax data from the Gaia satellite4. Using modelling, we infer that this pile-up arises from the release of latent heat as the cores of the white dwarfs crystallize. In addition to the release of latent heat, we find strong evidence that cooling is further slowed by the liberation of gravitational energy from element sedimentation in the crystallizing cores5-7. Our results describe the energy released by crystallization in strongly coupled Coulomb plasmas8,9, and the measured cooling delays could help to improve the accuracy of methods used to determine the age of stellar populations from white dwarfs10.

2.
Sensors (Basel) ; 23(5)2023 Feb 22.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36904633

RESUMEN

Background: Toe clearance on stairs is typically measured using optoelectronic systems, though these are often constrained to the laboratory, due to their complex setups. Here we measured stair toe clearance through a novel prototype photogate setup and compared this to optoelectronic measurements. Methods: Twelve participants (age 22 ± 3 years) completed 25 stair ascent trials, each on a seven-step staircase. Toe clearance over the fifth step edge was measured using Vicon and the photogates. Twenty-two photogates were created in rows through laser diodes and phototransistors. The height of the lowest photogate broken at step-edge crossing was used to determine photogate toe clearance. A limits of agreement analysis and Pearson's correlation coefficient compared the accuracy, precision and relationship between systems. Results: We found a mean difference of -1.5 mm (accuracy) between the two measurement systems, with upper and lower limits (precision) of 10.7 mm and -13.8 mm, respectively. A strong positive correlation was also found (r = 70, n = 12, p = 0.009) between the systems. Discussion: The results suggest that photogates could be an option for measuring real-world stair toe clearances, where optoelectronic systems are not routinely used. Improvements to the design and measurement factors may help to improve the precision of the photogates.

3.
J Appl Biomech ; 38(5): 301-311, 2022 Oct 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35977716

RESUMEN

A limitation of the ability to rotate the head with respect to the upper body has been associated with turning problems; however, the extent of head constraints on whole-body coordination has not been fully determined. The aim of this study was to limit head on body rotation and observe the effects on whole-body coordination during standing turns at various speeds. Twelve participants completed standing turns at 180°. A Vicon motion system and a BlueGain Electrooculography system were used to record movement kinematics and measure horizontal eye movements, respectively. All participants were tested at 3 randomized speeds, and under 2 conditions with or without their head constrained using a head, neck, and chest brace which restricted neck movement. A repeated-measures analysis of variance found a significant main effect of turning speed on the onset latency of all segments, peak head-thorax angular separation, and step characteristics. Constraining the head rotation had multiple significant effects including delayed onset latency and decreased intersegmental coordination defined as peak head segmental angular separations, increased total step and step duration, and decreased step size. This indicates the contribution of speed, head, and neck constraints, which have been associated with falls during turning and whole-body coordination.


Asunto(s)
Movimientos Oculares , Orientación , Fenómenos Biomecánicos , Movimientos de la Cabeza , Humanos , Movimiento , Posición de Pie
4.
Sensors (Basel) ; 21(8)2021 Apr 16.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33923838

RESUMEN

Difficulty in turning is prevalent in older adults and results in postural instability and risk of falling. Despite this, the mechanisms of turning problems have yet to be fully determined, and it is unclear if different speeds directly result in altered posture and turning characteristics. The aim of this study was to identify the effects of turning speeds on whole-body coordination and to explore if these can be used to help inform fall prevention programs in older adults. Forty-two participants (21 healthy older adults and 21 younger adults) completed standing turns on level ground. Inertial Measurement Units (XSENS) were used to measure turning kinematics and stepping characteristics. Participants were randomly tasked to turn 180° at one of three speeds; fast, moderate, or slow to the left and right. Two factors mixed model analysis of variance (MM ANOVA) with post hoc pairwise comparisons were performed to assess the two groups and three turning speeds. Significant interaction effects (p < 0.05) were seen in; reorientation onset latency of head, pelvis, and feet, peak segmental angular separation, and stepping characteristics (step frequency and step size), which all changed with increasing turn speed. Repeated measures ANOVA revealed the main effects of speeds within the older adults group on those variables as well as the younger adults group. Our results suggest that turning speeds result in altered whole-body coordination and stepping behavior in older adults, which use the same temporospatial sequence as younger adults. However, some characteristics differ significantly, e.g., onset latency of segments, peak head velocity, step frequency, and step size. Therefore, the assessment of turning speeds elucidates the exact temporospatial differences between older and younger healthy adults and may help to determine some of the issues that the older population face during turning, and ultimately the altered whole-body coordination, which lead to falls.


Asunto(s)
Accidentes por Caídas , Postura , Accidentes por Caídas/prevención & control , Anciano , Fenómenos Biomecánicos , Cabeza , Humanos , Pelvis , Equilibrio Postural , Caminata
5.
Sensors (Basel) ; 20(9)2020 Apr 29.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32365573

RESUMEN

Camera-based 3D motion analysis systems are considered to be the gold standard for movement analysis. However, using such equipment in a clinical setting is prohibitive due to the expense and time-consuming nature of data collection and analysis. Therefore, Inertial Measurement Units (IMUs) have been suggested as an alternative to measure movement in clinical settings. One area which is both important and challenging is the assessment of turning kinematics in individuals with movement disorders. This study aimed to validate the use of IMUs in the measurement of turning kinematics in healthy adults compared to a camera-based 3D motion analysis system. Data were collected from twelve participants using a Vicon motion analysis system which were compared with data from four IMUs placed on the forehead, middle thorax, and feet in order to determine accuracy and reliability. The results demonstrated that the IMU sensors produced reliable kinematic measures and showed excellent reliability (ICCs 0.80-0.98) and no significant differences were seen in paired t-tests in all parameters when comparing the two systems. This suggests that the IMU sensors provide a viable alternative to camera-based motion capture that could be used in isolation to gather data from individuals with movement disorders in clinical settings and real-life situations.


Asunto(s)
Movimiento (Física) , Acelerometría , Algoritmos , Fenómenos Biomecánicos , Pie , Movimiento , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados
6.
J Neurophysiol ; 120(4): 2066-2082, 2018 10 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30020836

RESUMEN

If a whole body reaching task is produced when standing or adopting challenging postures, it is unclear whether changes in attentional demands or the sensorimotor integration necessary for balance control influence the interaction between visuomotor and postural components of the movement. Is gaze control prioritized by the central nervous system (CNS) to produce coordinated eye movements with the head and whole body regardless of movement context? Considering the coupled nature of visuomotor and whole body postural control during action, this study aimed to understand how changing equilibrium constraints (in the form of different postural configurations) influenced the initiation of eye, head, and arm movements. We quantified the eye-head metrics and segmental kinematics as participants executed either isolated gaze shifts or whole body reaching movements to visual targets. In total, four postural configurations were compared: seated, natural stance, with the feet together (narrow stance), or while balancing on a wooden beam. Contrary to our initial predictions, the lack of distinct changes in eye-head metrics; timing of eye, head, and arm movement initiation; and gaze accuracy, in spite of kinematic differences, suggests that the CNS integrates postural constraints into the control necessary to initiate gaze shifts. This may be achieved by adopting a whole body gaze strategy that allows for the successful completion of both gaze and reaching goals. NEW & NOTEWORTHY Differences in sequence of movement among the eye, head, and arm have been shown across various paradigms during reaching. Here we show that distinct changes in eye characteristics and movement sequence, coupled with stereotyped profiles of head and gaze movement, are not observed when adopting postures requiring changes to balance constraints. This suggests that a whole body gaze strategy is prioritized by the central nervous system with postural control subservient to gaze stability requirements.


Asunto(s)
Brazo/fisiología , Movimientos Oculares , Movimientos de la Cabeza , Postura , Desempeño Psicomotor , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
7.
Exp Brain Res ; 236(4): 1077-1089, 2018 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29435606

RESUMEN

Older adults with falls risk tend to look away prematurely from targets for safe foot placement to view future hazards; behaviour associated with increased anxiety and stepping inaccuracies. We aimed to determine the effectiveness of route previewing in reducing anxiety and optimizing gaze behaviour and stepping performance of younger and older adults. Nine younger and nine older adults completed six walks with three task complexities over two sessions. Each trial used either an isolated stepping target, or a target followed by either one or two obstacles. Participants with eyes closed, on hearing a signal, opened their eyes and initiated walking (go trials) or stood previewing the route for 10 s before starting (preview trials). Kinematic data were collected using a Vicon motion analysis system. Gaze behaviour was recorded using a Dikablis eye tracker. On average, both older and younger adults fixated the target for significantly longer during walking when they had previewed the route than when they had not. Self-confidence scores were also significantly higher following 'preview trials' than 'go trials'. Stepping performance significantly improved following route previewing (reduced Medial lateral foot placement variability for both groups and reduced anterior/posterior foot placement error in older adults only). These findings implicate route previewing as a potential intervention to increase self-confidence and reduce the risk of tripping in older adults.


Asunto(s)
Accidentes por Caídas/prevención & control , Adaptación Fisiológica/fisiología , Ansiedad/psicología , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Miedo/psicología , Autoimagen , Caminata/fisiología , Adulto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Fenómenos Biomecánicos/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Caminata/psicología , Adulto Joven
8.
Exp Brain Res ; 235(12): 3593-3603, 2017 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28884336

RESUMEN

Turning the body towards a new direction is normally achieved via a top-down synergy whereby gaze (eye direction in space) leads the upper body segments, which in turn lead the feet. These anticipatory eye movements are observable even in darkness and constraining the initial eye movements modifies the stereotyped top-down reorientation sequence. Our aim was to elucidate the relative contributions of vision and eye movements to whole-body coordination during large standing turns by observing the effects of separately removing visual information or suppressing eye movements throughout the turn. We predicted that constraining eye movements would modify the steering synergy, whereas removing vision would have little effect. We found that preventing eye movements modified both timing and spatial characteristics of axial segment and feet rotation. When gaze was fixed, gait initiation, but not axial segment rotation, was delayed in comparison to both full vision and no vision turns. When eye movements were prevented, the predictable relationship between the extent head rotation led the body and peak head angular velocity was abolished suggesting that anticipatory head movements normally subserve gaze behaviour. In addition, stepping frequency significantly reduced during the gaze fixation condition but not during the no-vision condition, suggesting that oculomotor control is linked to stepping behaviour.


Asunto(s)
Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Orientación/fisiología , Postura/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Visión Ocular/fisiología , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Fenómenos Biomecánicos , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Adulto Joven
9.
Exp Brain Res ; 235(2): 573-583, 2017 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27817106

RESUMEN

Falls on stairs are a major hazard for older adults. Visual decline in normal ageing can affect step-climbing ability, altering gait and reducing toe clearance. Here we show that a loss of fine-grained visual information associated with age can affect the perception of surface undulations in patterned surfaces. We go on to show that such cues affect the limb trajectories of young adults, but due to their lack of sensitivity, not that of older adults. Interestingly neither the perceived height of a step nor conscious awareness is altered by our visual manipulation, but stepping behaviour is, suggesting that the influence of shape perception on stepping behaviour is via the unconscious, action-centred, dorsal visual pathway.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/fisiología , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Juicio/fisiología , Estereognosis/fisiología , Adulto , Anciano , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Estimulación Luminosa , Propiedades de Superficie , Adulto Joven
10.
Exp Brain Res ; 234(6): 1599-609, 2016 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26838356

RESUMEN

When a single light cue is given in the visual field, our eyes orient towards it with an average latency of 200 ms. If a second cue is presented at or around the time of the response to the first, a secondary eye movement occurs that represents a reorientation to the new target. While studies have shown that eye movement latencies to 'single-step' targets may or may not be lengthened with age, secondary eye movements (during 'double-step' displacements) are significantly delayed with increasing age. The aim of this study was to investigate whether the postural challenge posed simply by standing (as opposed to sitting) results in significantly longer eye movement latencies in older adults compared to the young. Ten young (<35 years) and 10 older healthy adults (>65 years) participated in the study. They were required to fixate upon a central target and move their eyes in response to 2 types of stimuli: (1) a single-step perturbation of target position either 15° to the right or left and (2) a double-step target displacement incorporating an initial target jump to the right or left by 15°, followed after 200 ms, by a shift of target position to the opposite side (e.g. +15° then -15°). All target displacement conditions were executed in sit and stand positions with the participant at the same distance from the targets. Eye movements were recorded using electro-oculography. Older adults did not show significantly longer eye movement latencies than the younger adults for single-step target displacements, and postural configuration (stand compared to sit) had no effect upon latencies for either group. We categorised double-step trials into those during which the second light changed after or before the onset of the eye shift to the first light. For the former category, young participants showed faster secondary eye shifts to the second light in the standing position, while the older adults did not. For the latter category of double-step trial, young participants showed no significant difference between sit and stand secondary eye movement latencies, but older adults were significantly longer standing compared to sitting. The older adults were significantly longer than the younger adults across both postural conditions, regardless of when the second light change occurred during the eye shift to the first light. We suggest that older adults require greater time and perhaps attentional processes to execute eye movements to unexpected changes in target position when faced with the need to maintain standing balance.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/fisiología , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Postura/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Factores de Edad , Anciano , Electrooculografía , Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Humanos , Adulto Joven
11.
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt ; 34(4): 459-69, 2014 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24836288

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: The current study aimed to quantitatively assess differences in gaze behaviour between participants grouped on the basis of their age and measures of functional mobility during a virtual walking paradigm. METHODS: The gaze behaviour of nine young adults, seven older adults with a relatively low risk of falling and seven older adults with a relatively higher risk of falling was measured while they watched five first-person perspective movies representing the viewpoint of a pedestrian walking through various environments. Participants also completed a number of cognitive tests: Stroop task, visual search, trail making task, Mini Mental Status Examination, and reaction time, visual tests (visual acuity and contrast sensitivity) and assessments of balance (Activities Balance Confidence Scale and Berg Balance Scale) to aid in the interpretation of differences in gaze behaviour. RESULTS: The high risk older adult group spent significantly more time fixating aspects of the travel path than the low risk and young adult groups. High risk older adults were also significantly slower in performing a number of the cognitive tasks than young adults. Correlations were conducted to compare the extent to which travel path fixation durations co-varied with scores on the tests of visual search, motor, and cognitive function. A positive significant correlation was found between the speed of response to the incongruent Stroop task and travel path fixation duration r21  = 0.44, p < 0.05. CONCLUSIONS: The results indicate that our movie-viewing paradigm can identify differences in gaze behaviour between participants grouped on the basis of their age and measures of functional mobility and that these differences are associated with cognitive decline.


Asunto(s)
Accidentes por Caídas , Atención/fisiología , Caminata/fisiología , Adulto , Factores de Edad , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Análisis de Varianza , Cognición/fisiología , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Femenino , Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Equilibrio Postural/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción , Grabación en Video , Agudeza Visual/fisiología , Adulto Joven
12.
Disabil Rehabil ; 44(3): 420-427, 2022 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32475182

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: To compare the immediate effects of voluntary-induced stepping response training (VSR) and DynSTABLE perturbation training (DST) on protective stepping in patients with stroke. METHODS: A randomized controlled trial (registration number: TCTR20170827001) was conducted in 34 patients with chronic stroke who were randomly allocated to the VSR (n = 17) or DST (n = 17) group. The VSR group was instructed to lean forward to induce protective stepping, while the DST group experienced support surface translation. All participants received one session of training (3 set, 10 min for each set with 10-minute rest in between). Step length, step width, number of steps and center of mass (CoM) position during protective stepping were assessed using a computer-assisted rehabilitation environment (CAREN) system prior to and immediately after training. Two-way ANOVA was used to compare between groups and times. RESULTS: Both types of training resulted in an increase in step width, but step length increased and there was a more positive COM position exhibited following DST (p < .05) than following VSR. Single-step incidence increased, whereas multiple-step incidence decreased significantly in both groups. Only participants in the VSR group generated protective stepping with the affected leg in a larger percentage of trials (27%) after training than before training. CONCLUSION: Both DST and VSR led to changes in protective stepping parameters after a single session of training. VSR may be a feasible alternative to equipment-based training but requires further study.Implication for RehabilitationVSR and DST trainings improved protective stepping in stroke.Step length and CoM control at foot touchdown increased after DST training.VSR training for 50 minutes led to increase affected stepping and reduce grasping.Step width, affected step length, and single step increased after both trainings.Without instrument, VSR increased steps execution and performance similar to DST.


Asunto(s)
Equilibrio Postural , Accidente Cerebrovascular , Accidentes por Caídas , Humanos , Equilibrio Postural/fisiología
13.
Physiother Res Int ; 27(4): e1963, 2022 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35717662

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION: En bloc turning highlights a lack of rotational intersegmental coordination, which commonly impacts turning ability in people with Parkinson's disease (PD). Whilst this turning deficit responds fairly well to medical treatment, it may be further mitigated by performing specific exercise training. Thus, the present study aimed to examine the effects of a 4-week exercise program, which focused on task-specific movements (TSM program) on turning ability and clinical outcomes in people with PD. METHODS: Twenty-two adults (67 ± 6 years) with early-to-mid-stage idiopathic PD were randomly assigned to an experimental group (EG; n = 11) or a control group (CG; n = 11). The exercise group (EG) group received a 60-min per session TSM program for 4 weeks (a total of 15 sessions), while the CG group performed their routine rehabilitation program (a total of 12 sessions). Inertial measurement units were used to measure turning kinematics including; onset latency of body segments and stepping characteristics. Clinical outcomes included the Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS), functional reach test (FRT), and fall efficacy scale international (FES-I). Assessments were conducted at baseline and after 4 weeks. RESULTS: In the EG, turning kinematics, UPDRS scores, FRT, and FES-I scale, were improved at the end of the 4-week program compared with the CG (all p < 0.05). IMPACT STATEMENT: A 4-week TSM program could be a promising alternative rehabilitation program for improving "en bloc" turns and clinical outcomes in PD patients.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedad de Parkinson , Adulto , Fenómenos Biomecánicos , Terapia por Ejercicio , Humanos , Movimiento , Modalidades de Fisioterapia
14.
Appl Ergon ; 101: 103678, 2022 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35151119

RESUMEN

Serious falls occur frequently on stairs with inconsistent dimensions. Inconsistent smaller goings are thought to reduce user's foot clearances and foot contact lengths since individuals do not detect and alter their behaviour prior to the inconsistency, increasing the risk of a trip, heel-catch or over-step and potential slip on the stairs. So far, these mechanisms for a stair fall remain theoretical only. The aim of this paper was to identify the underlying mechanisms by which steps with inconsistent going size increase the risk of falls. For this study twenty-seven younger adults (24 ± 3 y, 1.74 ± 0.09 m, 71.41 ± 11.04 kg) and thirty-three older adults (70 ± 4 y, 1.68 ± 0.08 m, 67.90 ± 14.10 kg) ascended and descended a seven-step instrumented staircase in two conditions: 1) consistent dimensions with 200 mm risers and 250 mm goings and 2) inconsistent going dimensions where the going of the third step was reduced by 10 mm, and consequently the going of the second step was larger by 10 mm. Five repeated trials on the inconsistent stairs were performed to assess if there was an adaptation effect after first exposure. In descent in the first inconsistent trial, foot contact lengths were not significantly different between conditions for the younger and older adults on the inconsistently shorter step (∼1%, p = .121). Foot trajectories were pulled further back in the last 22% of swing before contact (p = .025), contradicting previous expectations. Younger adults then had reduced clearances over the next step (∼5 mm, p = .027), which was inconsistently longer, increasing the risk of a heel-catch, whereas foot clearances for older adults were not different. With repeated inconsistent trials the foot contact length of older adults reduced on the shorter step (p = .024). In ascent, in the first inconsistent trial, interaction effects were detected between groups and conditions on three steps: the inconsistently longer step (p = .003), the shorter step (p = .004), the next step (p = .006), as well as on the walkway (p = .048). Older adults positioned themselves further away from the stairs on the walkway compared to younger adults and then had a reduced foot contact length on the inconsistently shorter step (∼2.8%, p = .026), increasing the chances of under-stepping and slipping off the shorter step. Whereas younger adults were positioned closer to the stairs on the walkway, had increased foot contact lengths on the inconsistently longer step and contact lengths that were not different on the inconsistently shorter step. With repeated inconsistent trials, foot contact lengths were reduced on the longer step (p = .006) and then on the shorter step (p = .018). These findings contradict previous assumptions that individuals do not adapt to inconsistent goings on stairs. In descent on the first trial, both groups adjusted their stepping behaviour late in the swing prior to contact with the first inconsistent step. In ascent younger adults made changes to their position and stepping behaviour before stepping on the stairs. These behaviours to mitigate the risk of the inconsistent step, did not persist in the repeat trials. Future investigations should establish the magnitude at which inconsistencies are detectable and can be acted upon and should include a wider range of individuals. This type of research could help inform future initiatives to prevent serious stair falls.


Asunto(s)
Pie , Marcha , Accidentes por Caídas/prevención & control , Adaptación Fisiológica , Anciano , Fenómenos Biomecánicos , Humanos , Caminata
15.
Neuroscience ; 455: 223-239, 2021 02 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33246066

RESUMEN

Postural and movement components must be coordinated without significant disturbance to balance when reaching from a standing position. Traditional theories propose that muscle activity prior to movement onset create the mechanics to counteract the internal torques generated by the future limb movement, reducing possible instability via centre of mass (CoM) displacement. However, during goal-directed reach movements executed on a fixed base of support (BoS), preparatory postural adjustments (or pPAs) promote movement of the CoM within the BoS. Considering this dichotomy, the current study investigated if pPAs constitute part of a whole-body strategy that is tied to the efficient execution of movement, rather than the constraints of balance. We reasoned that if pPAs were tied primarily to balance control, they would modulate as a function of perceived instability. Alternatively, if tied to dynamics necessary for movement initiation, they would remain unchanged, with feedback-based changes being sufficient to retain balance following volitional arm movement. Participants executed beyond-arm reaching movements in four different postural configurations that altered the quality of the BoS. Quantification of these changes to stability did not drastically alter the tuning or timing of preparatory muscle activity despite modifications to arm and CoM trajectories necessary to complete the reaching movement. In contrast to traditional views, preparatory postural muscle activity is not always tuned for balance maintenance or even as a calculation of upcoming instability but may reflect a requirement of voluntary movement towards a pre-defined location.


Asunto(s)
Movimiento , Postura , Desempeño Psicomotor , Retroalimentación , Humanos , Equilibrio Postural , Torque
16.
Exp Gerontol ; 149: 111309, 2021 07 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33716111

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION: Tripping on stairs results from insufficient foot to step edge clearance and can often lead to a fall in older adults. A stair horizontal-vertical illusion is suggested to increase the perceived riser height of a step and increase foot clearance when stepping up. However, this perception-action link has not been empirically determined in older adults. Previous findings suggesting a perception-action effect have also been limited to a single step or a three-step staircase. On larger staircases, somatosensory learning of step heights may be greater which could override the illusory effect on the top step. Furthermore, the striped nature of the existing stair horizontal-vertical illusion is associated with visual stress and may not be aesthetically suitable for use on public stairs. These issues need resolving before potential future implementation on public stairs. METHODS: Experiment 1. A series of four computer-based perception tests were conducted in older (N = 14: 70 ± 6 years) and young adults (N = 42: 24 ± 3 years) to test the influence of different illusion designs on stair riser height estimation. Participants compared images of stairs, with horizontal-vertical illusions or arbitrary designs on the bottom step, to a plain stair with different bottom step riser heights and selected the stair they perceived to have the tallest bottom riser. Horizontal-vertical illusions included a previously developed design and versions with modified spatial frequencies and mark space ratios. Perceived riser height differences were assessed between designs and between age groups. Experiment 2. To assess the perception-action link, sixteen older (70 ± 7 years) and fifteen young (24 ± 3 years) adults ascended a seven-step staircase with and without horizontal-vertical illusions tested in experiment 1 placed onto steps one and seven. Foot clearances were measured over each step. To determine whether changes in perception were linked to changes in foot clearance, perceived riser heights for each horizontal-vertical illusion were assessed using the perception test from experiment 1 before and after stair ascent. Additional measures to characterise stair safety included vertical foot clearance, margins of stability, foot overhang, stair speed, and gaze duration, which were assessed over all seven steps. RESULTS: Experiment 1. All horizontal-vertical illusion designs led to significant increases in the perceived riser height in both young and older adults (12-19% increase) with no differences between age groups. Experiment 2. On step 7, each horizontal-vertical illusion led to an increase in vertical foot clearance for young (up to 0.8 cm) and older adults (up to 2.1 cm). On step 1 significant increases in vertical foot clearance were found for a single horizontal-vertical illusion when compared to plain (1.19 cm increase). The horizontal-vertical illusions caused significant increases in the perceived riser height (young; 13% increase, older; 11% increase) with no differences between illusion design, group or before and after stair ascent. No further differences were found for the remaining variables and steps. CONCLUSION: Results indicate a perception-action link between perceived riser height and vertical foot clearance in response to modified versions of the horizontal-vertical illusion in both young and older adults. This was shown with no detriment to additional stair safety measures. Further evaluating these illusions on private/public stairs, especially those with inconsistently taller steps, may be beneficial to help improve stair safety for older adults.


Asunto(s)
Ilusiones , Accidentes por Caídas , Anciano , Fenómenos Biomecánicos , Pie , Marcha , Humanos
17.
J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci ; 76(4): 638-646, 2021 03 31.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32453832

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Stair falls are a major health problem for older people, but presently, there are no specific screening tools for stair fall prediction. The purpose of the present study was to investigate whether stair fallers could be differentiated from nonfallers by biomechanical risk factors or physical/psychological parameters and to establish the biomechanical stepping profile posing the greatest risk for a stair fall. METHODS: Eighty-seven older adults (age: 72.1 ± 5.2 years) negotiated an instrumented seven-step staircase and performed a range of physical/psychological tasks. k-Means clustering was used to profile the overall stair negotiation behavior with biomechanical parameters indicative of fall risk as input. Falls and events of balance perturbation (combined "hazardous events") were then monitored during a 12-month follow-up. Cox-regression analysis was performed to examine whether physical/psychological parameters or biomechanical outcome measures could predict future hazardous events. Kaplan-Meier survival curves were obtained to identify the stepping strategy posing a risk for a hazardous event. RESULTS: Physical/psychological parameters did not predict hazardous events and the commonly used Fall Risk Assessment Tool classified only 1/17 stair fallers at risk for a fall. Single biomechanical risk factors could not predict hazardous events on stairs either. On the contrary, two particular clusters identified by the stepping profiling method in stair ascent were linked with hazardous events. CONCLUSION: This highlights the potential of the stepping profiling method to predict stair fall risk in older adults against the limited predictability of single-parameter approaches currently used as screening tools.


Asunto(s)
Accidentes por Caídas , Equilibrio Postural , Medición de Riesgo/métodos , Conducta de Reducción del Riesgo , Subida de Escaleras/fisiología , Accidentes por Caídas/prevención & control , Accidentes por Caídas/estadística & datos numéricos , Anciano , Fenómenos Biomecánicos/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Tamizaje Masivo/métodos , Evaluación de Resultado en la Atención de Salud/métodos , Valor Predictivo de las Pruebas , Pruebas Psicológicas , Factores de Riesgo , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas
18.
PLoS One ; 16(9): e0257159, 2021.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34520496

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION: Stair falls can be caused by inconsistent stair dimensions. During ascent, inconsistently taller stair risers lead to reduced foot clearances as the inconsistency goes unnoticed. A stair horizontal-vertical illusion increases perceived riser heights and foot clearance and could offset reduced foot clearances over inconsistently taller risers, though this might impact other stair safety measures. METHOD: Twelve participants (age: 22 (3) years) ascended a seven-step staircase under three conditions: i) all steps consistent in riser height (consistent), ii) a 1cm increase in step 5 riser height (inconsistent) and iii) a 1cm increase in step 5 riser height, superimposed with a stair horizontal-vertical illusion (illusion). Vertical foot clearance, foot overhang, and margins of stability were assessed over step 4, 5 and 6. Perceived riser height due to the illusion was determined through a computer perception test. A One-Way Repeated Measures ANOVA compared biomechanical variables between conditions. A One Sample t test compared perceived riser height to the true height. RESULTS: Over the inconsistent step 5, foot clearance reduced by 0.8cm compared to consistent. Illusion increased foot clearance by 1.1cm and decreased foot overhang by 4% compared to inconsistent. On step 4 the illusion led to more anterior instability compared to inconsistent. Illusion and inconsistent led to more mediolateral stability compared to consistent. The illusion increased perceived riser height by 12%. DISCUSSION: Foot clearance reductions over inconsistently taller risers can be offset by a stair horizontal-vertical illusion. Additional benefits included a safer foot overhang and unaffected stability over the inconsistent riser. Changes to step 4 stability might have resulted from leaning forward to look at the step 5 illusion. The stair horizontal-vertical illusion could be a practical solution for inconsistently taller stair risers, where a rebuild is usually the only solution.


Asunto(s)
Pie/fisiología , Ilusiones , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Movimiento/fisiología , Evaluación de Resultado en la Atención de Salud , Adulto Joven
19.
Hum Mov Sci ; 77: 102774, 2021 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33676032

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Step-surface visual properties are often associated with stair falls. However, evidence for decorating stairs typically concerns the application of step-edge highlighters rather than the entire step-surface. Here we examine the influence of step-surface visual properties on stair descent safety, with a view to generating preliminary evidence for safe stair décor. METHODS: Fourteen young (YA: 23.1 ± 3.7 years), 13 higher (HAOA: 67 ± 3.5) and 14 lower (LAOA: 73.4 ± 5.7) ability older adults descended a seven-step staircase. Older adults were stratified based on physiological/cognitive function. Step-surface décor patterns assessed were: Black and white (Busy); fine grey (Plain); and striped multicolour (Striped); each implemented with/without black edge-highlighters (5.5 cm width) totalling six conditions. Participants descended three times per condition. Confidence was assessed prior to, and anxiety following, the first descent in each condition. 3D kinematics (Vicon) quantified descent speed, margin of stability, and foot clearances with respect to step-edges. Eye tracking (Pupil-labs) recorded gaze. Data from three phases of descent (entry, middle, exit) were analysed. Linear mixed-effects models assessed within-subject effects of décor (×3) and edge highlighters (×2), between-subject effects of age (×3), and interactions between terms (α = p < .05). RESULTS: Décor: Plain décor reduced anxiety in all ages and abilities (p = .032, effect size: gav = 0.3), and increased foot clearances in YA and HAOA in the middle phase (p < .001, gav = 0.53), thus improving safety. In contrast, LAOA exhibited no change in foot clearance with Plain décor. Patterned décor slowed descent (Busy: p < .001, gav = 0.2), increased margins of stability (Busy: p < .001, gav = 0.41; Striped: p < .001, gav = 0.25) and reduced steps looked ahead (Busy: p = .053, gav = 0.25; Striped: p = .039, gav = 0.28) in all ages and abilities. This reflects cautious descent, likely due to more challenging conditions for visually extracting information about the spatial characteristics of the steps useful to guide descent. Edge highlighters: Step-edge highlighters increased confidence (p < .001, gav = 0.53) and reduced anxiety (p < .001, gav = 0.45) in all ages and abilities and for all décor, whilst removing them slowed descent in HAOA (p = .01, gav = 0.26) and LAOA (p = .003, gav = 0.25). Step-edge highlighters also increased foot clearance in YA and HAOA (p = .003, gav = 0.14), whilst LAOA older adults showed no adaptation. No change in foot clearances with décor or step-edge highlighters in LAOA suggests an inability to adapt to step-surface visual properties. CONCLUSION: Patterned step surfaces can lead to more cautious and demanding stair negotiation from the perspective of visually extracting spatial information about the steps. In contrast, plain décor with step edge highlighters improves safety. We therefore suggest plain décor with edge highlighters is preferable for use on stairs.


Asunto(s)
Ansiedad/fisiopatología , Fijación Ocular , Marcha , Autoimagen , Accidentes por Caídas/prevención & control , Adolescente , Adulto , Factores de Edad , Anciano , Trastornos de Ansiedad , Fenómenos Biomecánicos , Femenino , Pie , Humanos , Masculino , Procesos Mentales , Persona de Mediana Edad , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Propiedades de Superficie , Adulto Joven
20.
Exp Brain Res ; 201(3): 467-78, 2010 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19882147

RESUMEN

This study investigates if there are age- and falls-risk related differences in the length of time individuals need following fixation of a stepping target in order to step accurately onto it. This aim was achieved by manipulating the timing and location of stepping target presentation and comparing the effects on stepping performance between young adults, older adults characterised as having a low risk of falling and older adults characterised as having increased risk of falling (N = 10 in each group). Eye and lower limb kinematics were recorded using an eye tracker interfaced with a 3D motion analysis system. Temporal and spatial characteristics of eye and stepping movements were analysed and compared between groups and conditions in which participants had either <1, 2 or 3 s, following target fixation, in order to view and respond to target presentations. Comparisons were made between steps to centrally or laterally positioned targets (125% of individual participant's normal step width). The results showed that high-risk older adults required significantly more time than low-risk older and younger adults in order to plan and execute medio-lateral stepping adjustments. A reduced ability to make rapid sideways stepping adjustments to avoid obstacles or step on safe areas may contribute towards trips and falls in these individuals. Possible neural mechanisms underlying this group-related decline in performance are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Accidentes por Caídas/prevención & control , Adaptación Fisiológica/fisiología , Envejecimiento/fisiología , Locomoción/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Envejecimiento/psicología , Fenómenos Biomecánicos/fisiología , Femenino , Trastornos Neurológicos de la Marcha/diagnóstico , Trastornos Neurológicos de la Marcha/fisiopatología , Humanos , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador , Imagenología Tridimensional/métodos , Juicio/fisiología , Pierna/fisiología , Masculino , Destreza Motora/fisiología , Factores de Riesgo , Grabación en Video , Adulto Joven
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA