Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 109
Filtrar
Más filtros

Bases de datos
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 230: 105629, 2023 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36731280

RESUMEN

The fission and fusion illusions provide measures of multisensory integration. The sound-induced tap fission illusion occurs when a tap is paired with two distractor sounds, resulting in the perception of two taps; the sound-induced tap fusion illusion occurs when two taps are paired with a single sound, resulting in the perception of a single tap. Using these illusions, we measured integration in three groups of children (9-, 11-, and 13-year-olds) and compared them with a group of adults. Based on accuracy, we derived a measure of magnitude of illusion and used a signal detection analysis to estimate perceptual discriminability and decisional criterion. All age groups showed a significant fission illusion, whereas only the three groups of children showed a significant fusion illusion. When compared with adults, the 9-year-olds showed larger fission and fusion illusions (i.e., reduced discriminability and greater bias), whereas the 11-year-olds were adult-like for fission but showed some differences for fusion: significantly worse discriminability and marginally greater magnitude and criterion. The 13-year-olds were adult-like on all measures. Based on the pattern of data, we speculate that the developmental trajectories for fission and fusion differ. We discuss these developmental results in the context of three non-mutually exclusive theoretical frameworks: sensory dominance, maximum likelihood estimation, and causal inference.


Asunto(s)
Ilusiones , Percepción del Tacto , Adulto , Niño , Humanos , Percepción Visual , Estimulación Acústica/métodos , Percepción Auditiva , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos
2.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 117(18): 10089-10096, 2020 05 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32321833

RESUMEN

Synesthesia is a neurologic trait in which specific inducers, such as sounds, automatically elicit additional idiosyncratic percepts, such as color (thus "colored hearing"). One explanation for this trait-and the one tested here-is that synesthesia results from unusually weak pruning of cortical synaptic hyperconnectivity during early perceptual development. We tested the prediction from this hypothesis that synesthetes would be superior at making discriminations from nonnative categories that are normally weakened by experience-dependent pruning during a critical period early in development-namely, discrimination among nonnative phonemes (Hindi retroflex /d̪a/ and dental /ɖa/), among chimpanzee faces, and among inverted human faces. Like the superiority of 6-mo-old infants over older infants, the synesthetic groups were significantly better than control groups at making all the nonnative discriminations across five samples and three testing sites. The consistent superiority of the synesthetic groups in making discriminations that are normally eliminated during infancy suggests that residual cortical connectivity in synesthesia supports changes in perception that extend beyond the specific synesthetic percepts, consistent with the incomplete pruning hypothesis.


Asunto(s)
Cognición/fisiología , Neuroimagen , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Sinestesia/diagnóstico por imagen , Adulto , Cara/diagnóstico por imagen , Cara/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Sinestesia/fisiopatología
3.
J Pediatr ; 241: 212-220.e2, 2022 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34687692

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: To test the association of material deprivation and the utilization of vision care services for young children. STUDY DESIGN: We conducted a population-based, repeated measures cohort study using linked health and administrative datasets. All children born in Ontario in 2010 eligible for provincial health insurance were followed from birth until their seventh birthday. The main exposure was neighborhood-level material deprivation quintile, a proxy for socioeconomic status. The primary outcome was receipt of a comprehensive eye examination (not to include a vision screening) by age 7 years from an eye care professional, or family physician. RESULTS: Of 128 091 children included, female children represented 48.7% of the cohort, 74.4% lived in major urban areas, and 16.2% lived in families receiving income assistance. Only 65% (n = 82 833) had at least 1 comprehensive eye examination, with the lowest uptake (56.9%; n = 31 911) in the most deprived and the highest uptake (70.5%; n =19 860) in the least deprived quintiles. After adjusting for clinical and demographic variables, children living in the least materially deprived quintile had a higher odds of receiving a comprehensive eye examination (aOR 1.43; 95% CI 1.36, 1.51) compared with children in the most materially deprived areas. CONCLUSIONS: Uptake of comprehensive eye examinations is poor, especially for children living in the most materially deprived neighborhoods. Strategies to improve uptake and reduce inequities are warranted.


Asunto(s)
Utilización de Instalaciones y Servicios/economía , Accesibilidad a los Servicios de Salud/economía , Disparidades en Atención de Salud/economía , Clase Social , Trastornos de la Visión/diagnóstico , Pruebas de Visión/economía , Niño , Preescolar , Utilización de Instalaciones y Servicios/estadística & datos numéricos , Femenino , Accesibilidad a los Servicios de Salud/estadística & datos numéricos , Disparidades en Atención de Salud/estadística & datos numéricos , Humanos , Lactante , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Ontario , Pruebas de Visión/estadística & datos numéricos
4.
CMAJ ; 192(29): E822-E831, 2020 07 20.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32690557

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Visual problems can negatively affect visual development and learning but often go undetected. We assessed the feasibility of scaling up a school-based screening program to identify and treat kindergarten children with visual problems. METHODS: We conducted a prospective cohort study offering vision screening to junior (JK) and senior kindergarten (SK) children attending 43 schools in 15 Ontario communities. Screening comprised photoscreeners and tests of visual acuity, stereoacuity and eye alignment. Children who failed any test were referred for a comprehensive eye examination, with treatment as needed (e.g., glasses). RESULTS: Using a passive consent model, 89% of children were screened compared with 62% using an active consent model (p < 0.001). Referral rates to an optometrist varied across schools (mean referral rate for children in JK 53%, range 25%-83%; mean referral rate for children in SK 34%, range 12%-61%). Among 4811 children who were screened, a visual problem was detected in 516 (10.7%), including 164 (3.4%) with amblyopia and 324 (6.7%) with clinically significant refractive errors. For 347 (67.2%) of the children with a visual problem, this was their first eye examination. Rescreening in Year 2 did not lead to detection of additional problems among children who passed screening in Year 1. Regardless of location (child's school or optometrist's office), 1563 (68.9%) of children attended the follow-up optometry examination. Most of the children who were surveyed (291 of 322, 90.4%) indicated that they enjoyed vision screening. INTERPRETATION: Many children in Ontario with a visual problem were not being identified by the status quo in 2015-2017. We found that in-school vision screening with follow-up eye examinations is an effective strategy for identifying at-risk children and placing them in eye care before grade 1.


Asunto(s)
Servicios de Salud Escolar/organización & administración , Trastornos de la Visión/diagnóstico , Selección Visual/organización & administración , Agudeza Visual , Ambliopía/diagnóstico , Niño , Preescolar , Estudios de Factibilidad , Humanos , Masculino , Ontario , Estudios Prospectivos , Errores de Refracción/diagnóstico
5.
Dev Sci ; 23(2): e12890, 2020 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31350857

RESUMEN

Being born at extremely low birth weight (ELBW; ≤1,000 g) is associated with enduring visual impairments. We tested for long-term, higher order visual processing problems in the oldest known prospectively followed cohort of ELBW survivors. Configural processing (spacing among features of an object) was examined in 62 adults born at ELBW (Mage  = 31.9 years) and 82 adults born at normal birth weight (NBW; ≥2,500 g: Mage  = 32.5 years). Pairs of human faces, monkey faces, or houses were presented in a delayed match-to-sample task, where non-matching stimuli differed only in the spacing of their features. Discrimination accuracy for each stimulus type was compared between birth weight groups, adjusting for neurosensory impairment, visual acuity, binocular fusion ability, IQ, and sex. Both groups were better able to discriminate human faces than monkey faces (p < .001). However, the ELBW group discriminated between human faces (p < .001), between monkey faces (p < .001), and to some degree, between houses (p < .06), more poorly than NBW control participants, suggesting a general deficit in perceptual processing. Human face discrimination was related to performance IQ (PIQ) across groups, but especially among ELBW survivors. Coding (a PIQ subtest) also predicted human face discrimination in ELBW survivors, consistent with previously reported links between visuo-perceptive difficulties and regional slowing of cortical activity in individuals born preterm. Correlations with Coding suggested ELBW survivors may have used a feature-matching approach to processing human faces. Future studies could examine brain-based anatomical and functional evidence for altered face processing, as well as the social and memory consequences of face-processing deficits in ELBW survivors.


Asunto(s)
Recien Nacido con Peso al Nacer Extremadamente Bajo/fisiología , Adulto , Cognición , Estudios de Cohortes , Reconocimiento Facial/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Recién Nacido , Masculino , Memoria , Percepción Visual/fisiología
6.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 183: 208-221, 2019 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30913423

RESUMEN

We charted the developmental trajectory of the perception of audiotactile simultaneity by testing three groups of children (aged 7, 9, and 11 years) and one group of adults. A white noise burst and a tap to the index finger were presented at 1 of 13 stimulus onset asynchronies (SOAs), and the participants were asked to report whether the two stimuli were simultaneous. Compared with adults, 7-year-olds made significantly more simultaneous responses at 9 of the 13 SOAs, whereas 9-year-olds differed from adults at only 2 SOAs. The precision of simultaneity perception was lower, and response errors were higher, in younger children than in adults. The 11-year-olds were adult-like on all measures, thereby demonstrating that judgments about simultaneity for audiotactile stimuli are mature by 11 years. This developmental pattern is similar to that for simultaneity perception for visuotactile stimuli but later than that for audiovisual stimuli. The longer developmental trajectories of the perception of simultaneity between touch and vision and between touch and audition may arise from the need to coordinate and recalibrate between different reference frames and different neural transmission times in each sensory system during body growth; in addition, the ubiquity of audiovisual experience in everyday life may accelerate the development of that modality pairing.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/fisiología , Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Percepción del Tacto/fisiología , Adulto , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Juicio/fisiología , Masculino , Tacto/fisiología
7.
Vis Neurosci ; 35: E012, 2018 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29905124

RESUMEN

Amblyopia is a developmental disorder that affects the spatial vision of one or both eyes in the absence of an obvious organic cause; it is associated with a history of abnormal visual experience during childhood. Subtypes have been defined based on the purported etiology, namely, strabismus (misaligned eyes) and/or anisometropia (unequal refractive error). Here we consider the usefulness of these subclassifications.


Asunto(s)
Ambliopía/clasificación , Adulto , Ambliopía/diagnóstico , Ambliopía/etiología , Niño , Preescolar , Variación Genética , Humanos , Lactante , Pruebas de Visión/instrumentación
8.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 173: 304-317, 2018 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29783043

RESUMEN

A simultaneity judgment (SJ) task was used to measure the developmental trajectory of visuotactile simultaneity perception in children (aged 7, 9, 11, and 13 years) and adults. Participants were presented with a visual flash in the center of a computer monitor and a tap on their right index finger (located 20° below the flash) with 13 possible stimulus onset asynchronies (SOAs). Participants reported whether the flash and tap were presented at the same time. Compared with the adult group, children aged 7 and 9 years made more simultaneous responses when the tap led by more than 300 ms and when the flash led by more than 200 ms, whereas they made fewer simultaneous responses at the 0 ms SOA. Model fitting demonstrated that the window of visuotactile simultaneity became narrower with development and reached adult-like levels between 9 and 11 years of age. Response errors decreased continuously until 11 years of age. The point of subjective simultaneity (PSS) was located on the tactile-leading side in all participants tested, indicating that 7-year olds (the youngest age tested) are adult-like on this measure. In summary, the perception of visuotactile simultaneity is not fully mature until 11 years of age. The protracted development of visuotactile simultaneity perception may be related to the need for crossmodal recalibration as the body grows and to the developmental improvements in the ability to optimally integrate visual and tactile signals.


Asunto(s)
Juicio/fisiología , Percepción del Tiempo/fisiología , Percepción del Tacto/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adolescente , Factores de Edad , Niño , Femenino , Dedos , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Adulto Joven
9.
Dev Sci ; 20(3)2017 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26825050

RESUMEN

Faces are adaptively coded relative to visual norms that are updated by experience, and this adaptive coding is linked to face recognition ability. Here we investigated whether adaptive coding of faces is disrupted in individuals (adolescents and adults) who experience face recognition difficulties following visual deprivation from congenital cataracts in infancy. We measured adaptive coding using face identity aftereffects, where smaller aftereffects indicate less adaptive updating of face-coding mechanisms by experience. We also examined whether the aftereffects increase with adaptor identity strength, consistent with norm-based coding of identity, as in typical populations, or whether they show a different pattern indicating some more fundamental disruption of face-coding mechanisms. Cataract-reversal patients showed significantly smaller face identity aftereffects than did controls (Experiments 1 and 2). However, their aftereffects increased significantly with adaptor strength, consistent with norm-based coding (Experiment 2). Thus we found reduced adaptability but no fundamental disruption of norm-based face-coding mechanisms in cataract-reversal patients. Our results suggest that early visual experience is important for the normal development of adaptive face-coding mechanisms.


Asunto(s)
Cara , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Trastornos de la Visión/fisiopatología , Adaptación Fisiológica , Adolescente , Adulto , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Catarata/congénito , Niño , Humanos , Adulto Joven
10.
Dev Sci ; 20(3)2017 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26743221

RESUMEN

We tested the effect of early monocular and binocular deprivation of normal visual input on the development of contour interpolation. Patients deprived from birth by dense central cataracts in one or both eyes, and age-matched controls, discriminated between fat and thin shapes formed by either illusory or luminance-defined contours. Thresholds indicated the minimum amount of curvature (the fatness or thinness) required for discrimination of the illusory shape, providing a measure of the precision of interpolation. The results show that individuals deprived of visual input in one eye, but not those deprived in both eyes, later show deficits in perceptual interpolation. The deficits were shown mostly for weakly supported contours in which interpolation of contours between the inducers was over a large distance relative to the size of the inducers. Deficits shown for the unilateral but not for the bilateral patients point to the detrimental effect of unequal competition between the eyes for cortical connections on the later development of the mechanisms underlying contour interpolation.


Asunto(s)
Desarrollo Infantil/fisiología , Percepción de Forma , Percepción Visual , Adolescente , Adulto , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Niño , Humanos , Trastornos de la Visión/fisiopatología , Visión Binocular , Visión Monocular , Adulto Joven
11.
Dev Psychobiol ; 59(3): 375-389, 2017 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28181225

RESUMEN

We examined the role of early visual input in visual system development by testing adults who had been born with dense bilateral cataracts that blocked all patterned visual input during infancy until the cataractous lenses were removed surgically and the eyes fitted with compensatory contact lenses. Patients viewed checkerboards and textures to explore early processing regions (V1, V2), Glass patterns to examine global form processing (V4), and moving stimuli to explore global motion processing (V5). Patients' ERPs differed from those of controls in that (1) the V1 component was much smaller for all but the simplest stimuli and (2) extrastriate components did not differentiate amongst texture stimuli, Glass patterns, or motion stimuli. The results indicate that early visual deprivation contributes to permanent abnormalities at early and mid levels of visual processing, consistent with enduring behavioral deficits in the ability to process complex textures, global form, and global motion.


Asunto(s)
Potenciales Evocados Visuales/fisiología , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Trastornos de la Percepción/fisiopatología , Privación Sensorial/fisiología , Corteza Visual/fisiopatología , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Trastornos de la Percepción/etiología , Adulto Joven
12.
Dev Psychobiol ; 59(8): 1051-1057, 2017 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29071716

RESUMEN

The current study investigated the impact of birth weight on the ability to recognize facial expressions in adulthood among the longest known prospectively followed cohort of extremely low birth weight survivors (ELBW; <1,000 g). We measured perceptual threshold to detect subtle facial expressions and confusion among different emotion categories in order to disentangle visual perceptual ability from emotional processing. ELBW adults (N = 64, Mage = 31.9 years) were more likely than normal birth weight (NBW) controls (N = 82, Mage = 32.5 years) to see fear in angry faces. This finding was not a result of increased perceptual efficiency in processing fearful expressions in the ELBW adults, since the two groups did not differ on their threshold to detect emotion in low intensity facial expressions. These findings suggest that a processing bias toward fear may reflect long-term developmental effects from being born at ELBW that may portend socioemotional problems that characterize ELBW survivors.


Asunto(s)
Ira/fisiología , Expresión Facial , Reconocimiento Facial/fisiología , Miedo/fisiología , Desarrollo Humano/fisiología , Recien Nacido con Peso al Nacer Extremadamente Bajo/fisiología , Percepción Social , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Recién Nacido , Masculino , Sobrevivientes
13.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 150: 301-313, 2016 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27376924

RESUMEN

The current experiment measured symbolic SNARC (Spatial-Numeric Association of Response Codes) and distance effects in school-aged children and investigated the relation between these measures and visuospatial skills and mathematics ability. In the experiment, 6-, 7-, and 8-year-olds performed a magnitude-relevant SNARC task, in which they indicated whether a target number was less or greater than 5, as well as standardized tests of visuospatial skills (Developmental Test of Visual Perception-Second Edition, DTVP-2) and mathematics ability (Test of Early Mathematics Ability-Third Edition, TEMA-3). Consistent with previous research using numerical SNARC tasks with Western children, all age groups exhibited robust distance effects, and SNARC effects were observed only in 7- and 8-year-olds. Distance effects, but not SNARC effects, were moderately but significantly correlated with a subtest of the DTVP-2 measuring the ability to mentally manipulate objects in space but no other subtest. These data suggest that mental orientation abilities, but perhaps not visuospatial skills involved in visual perception and visuomotor coordination, are related to some aspects of mental number line development. Nevertheless, no relation was observed between SNARC or distance effects and mathematics ability. This result is consistent with previous developmental studies investigating the association between SNARC and math skill. However, these data are inconsistent with most experiments assessing the relationship between distance effect strength and math-a difference that can likely be attributed to the fact that a magnitude-relevant SNARC task was employed as opposed to a traditional SNARC parity task.


Asunto(s)
Matemática , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Aptitud/fisiología , Niño , Cognición/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Orientación/fisiología , Orientación Espacial/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología
14.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 146: 17-33, 2016 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26897264

RESUMEN

We measured the typical developmental trajectory of the window of audiovisual simultaneity by testing four age groups of children (5, 7, 9, and 11 years) and adults. We presented a visual flash and an auditory noise burst at various stimulus onset asynchronies (SOAs) and asked participants to report whether the two stimuli were presented at the same time. Compared with adults, children aged 5 and 7 years made more simultaneous responses when the SOAs were beyond ± 200 ms but made fewer simultaneous responses at the 0 ms SOA. The point of subjective simultaneity was located at the visual-leading side, as in adults, by 5 years of age, the youngest age tested. However, the window of audiovisual simultaneity became narrower and response errors decreased with age, reaching adult levels by 9 years of age. Experiment 2 ruled out the possibility that the adult-like performance of 9-year-old children was caused by the testing of a wide range of SOAs. Together, the results demonstrate that the adult-like precision of perceiving audiovisual simultaneity is developed by 9 years of age, the youngest age that has been reported to date.


Asunto(s)
Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Percepción del Tiempo/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica/métodos , Adolescente , Adulto , Factores de Edad , Niño , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Ruido , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Adulto Joven
15.
Perception ; 45(12): 1399-1411, 2016 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27488568

RESUMEN

Adults who missed early visual input because of congenital cataracts later have deficits in many aspects of face processing. Here we investigated whether they make normal judgments of facial attractiveness. In particular, we studied whether their perceptions are affected normally by a face's proximity to the population mean, as is true of typically developing adults, who find average faces to be more attractive than most other faces. We compared the judgments of facial attractiveness of 12 cataract-reversal patients to norms established from 36 adults with normal vision. Participants viewed pairs of adult male and adult female faces that had been transformed 50% toward and 50% away from their respective group averages, and selected which face was more attractive. Averageness influenced patients' judgments of attractiveness, but to a lesser extent than controls. The results suggest that cataract-reversal patients are able to develop a system for representing faces with a privileged position for an average face, consistent with evidence from identity aftereffects. However, early visual experience is necessary to set up the neural architecture necessary for averageness to influence perceptions of attractiveness with its normal potency.

16.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 120: 1-16, 2014 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24326246

RESUMEN

We examined how recent biased face experience affects the influence of averageness on judgments of facial attractiveness among 8- and 9-year-old children attending a girls' school, a boys' school, and a mixed-sex school. We presented pairs of individual faces in which one face was transformed 50% toward its group average, whereas the other face was transformed 50% away from that average. Across blocks, the faces varied in age (adult, 9-year-old, or 5-year-old) and sex (male or female). We expected that averageness might influence attractiveness judgments more strongly for same-age faces and, for children attending single-sex schools, same-sex faces of that age because their prototype(s) should be best tuned to the faces they see most frequently. Averageness influenced children's judgments of attractiveness, but the strength of the influence was not modulated by the age of the face, nor did the effects of sex of face differ across schools. Recent biased experience might not have affected the results because of similarities between the average faces of different ages and sexes and/or because a minimum level of experience with a particular group of faces may be adequate for the formation of a veridical prototype and its influence on judgments of attractiveness. The results suggest that averageness affects children's judgments of the attractiveness of the faces they encounter in everyday life regardless of age or sex of face.


Asunto(s)
Belleza , Conducta Infantil/psicología , Conducta de Elección/fisiología , Juicio/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Estudiantes/psicología , Niño , Cara , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Instituciones Académicas , Factores Sexuales , Estudiantes/estadística & datos numéricos
17.
Dev Psychobiol ; 56(2): 154-78, 2014 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24519366

RESUMEN

In this article, we begin with a summary of the evidence for perceptual narrowing for various aspects of language (e.g., vowel and consonant contrasts, tone languages, visual language, sign language) and of faces (e.g., own species, own race). We then consider possible reasons for the apparent differences in the timing of narrowing (e.g., apparently earlier for own race than for own species). Throughout we consider whether the evidence fits a model of maintenance/loss or is better characterized as enhancement/attunement to exposed categories. Finally, we consider evidence on the malleability of the timing and its implications for the role of endogenous factors versus learning in controlling when narrowing occurs. Overall, the comparison across domains revealed many similarities but also striking differences which lead to suggestions for future research.


Asunto(s)
Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Desarrollo Infantil/fisiología , Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Lenguaje , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Cara , Humanos , Lactante , Aprendizaje , Reconocimiento en Psicología
18.
Dev Psychobiol ; 56(1): 96-108, 2014 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23192566

RESUMEN

Patients treated for bilateral congenital cataract are later impaired on several hallmarks of adults' expertise with upright faces but report no problem with remembering faces. Here, we provide the first formal data on their face memory. We compared 12 adults with a history of visual deprivation from bilateral congenital cataracts to 24 age-matched controls with normal vision on their ability to recognize famous and recently learned faces, and on their subjective impression of their face memory. Bilateral congenital cataract patients demonstrated a prosopagnosic-like deficit, being slower and less accurate in recognizing both famous faces and recently learned faces, despite not differing on most questions about their impression of their face memory. Patients' results on three perceptual tasks (the composite face effect, the Benton test of recognizing faces through a change in point of view, and the Jane test of sensitivity to feature spacing) were also not correlated with their face memory deficits. These results suggest that early visual input is necessary not only for perceptual expertise in differentiating among unfamiliar upright faces, but also for normal accuracy in remembering the identity of individual faces.


Asunto(s)
Catarata/complicaciones , Trastornos de la Memoria/complicaciones , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Catarata/psicología , Cara , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Trastornos de la Memoria/psicología , Personas con Daño Visual
19.
Can J Public Health ; 2024 Apr 30.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38691337

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: To assess the effectiveness of a kindergarten vision screening program by randomly assigning schools to receive or not receive vision screening, then following up 1.5 years later. METHODS: Fifty high-needs elementary schools were randomly assigned to participate or not in a vision screening program for children in senior kindergarten (SK; age 5‒6 years). When the children were in Grade 2 (age 6‒7 years), vision screening was conducted at all 50 schools. RESULTS: Contrary to expectations, screened and non-screened schools did not differ in the prevalence of suspected amblyopia in Grade 2 (8.6% vs. 7.5%, p = 0.10), nor prevalence of other visual problems such as astigmatism (45.1% vs. 47.1%, p = 0.51). There was also no difference between screened and non-screened schools in academic outcomes such as the proportion of children below grade level in reading (33% vs. 29%) or math (44% vs. 38%) (p = 0.86). However, more children were wearing glasses in screened than in non-screened schools (10.2% vs. 7.8%, p = 0.05), and more children reported their glasses as missing or broken (8.3% vs. 4.7%, p = 0.01), suggesting that SK screening had identified successfully those in need of glasses. Examination of individual results revealed that 72% of children diagnosed and treated for amblyopia in SK no longer had amblyopia in Grade 2. CONCLUSION: The prevalence of amblyopia and other visual problems was not reduced in Grade 2 by our SK vision screening program, perhaps because of poor treatment compliance and high attrition. The results suggest that a single screening intervention is insufficient to reduce visual problems among young children. However, the data from individuals with amblyopia suggest that continuing vision care and access to glasses benefits children, especially children from lower socioeconomic class.


RéSUMé: OBJECTIF: Évaluer l'efficacité d'un programme de dépistage visuel à l'école maternelle (EM) en assignant aléatoirement des écoles à participer ou non à un tel programme, puis en faisant un suivi un an et demi après. MéTHODES: Cinquante écoles primaires pour étudiants et étudiantes ayant des besoins importants ont été assignées aléatoirement à participer ou non à un programme de dépistage visuel auprès des enfants fréquentant la maternelle (EM; 5‒6 ans). Lorsque ces enfants étaient en 2e année (6‒7 ans), un dépistage visuel a été effectué dans les 50 écoles. RéSULTATS: Contre toute attente, il n'y a pas eu de différence entre les écoles ayant participé ou non au dépistage dans la prévalence de l'amblyopie présumée en 2e année (8,6 % contre 7,5 %, p = 0,10), ni dans la prévalence d'autres problèmes de vision comme l'astigmatisme (45,1 % contre 47,1 %, p = 0,51). Il n'y a pas eu non plus de différence dans les résultats scolaires des deux groupes d'écoles, comme la proportion d'enfants dont le niveau en lecture (33 % contre 29 %) ou en mathématiques (44 % contre 38 %), p = 0,86, ne correspondait pas à leur année d'étude. Cependant, le nombre d'enfants portant des lunettes était plus élevé dans les écoles ayant participé au dépistage que dans les autres écoles (10,2 % contre 7,8 %, p = 0,05), ainsi que le nombre d'enfants disant avoir perdu ou brisé leurs lunettes (8,3 % contre 4,7 %, p = 0,01), ce qui indique que le dépistage en maternelle a identifié avec succès les enfants ayant besoin de lunettes. L'examen des résultats individuels a révélé que 72 % des enfants diagnostiqués et traités pour l'amblyopie en maternelle ne présentaient plus d'amblyopie en 2e année. CONCLUSION: Notre programme de dépistage visuel à l'école maternelle n'a pas réduit la prévalence de l'amblyopie et d'autres problèmes de vision en 2e année, peut-être en raison du manque d'assiduité au traitement et d'une attrition importante. Les résultats indiquent qu'une seule intervention de dépistage ne suffit pas à réduire les problèmes de vision chez les jeunes enfants. Cependant, les données individuelles des sujets présentant une amblyopie indiquent qu'il est avantageux pour les enfants, et surtout ceux de la classe socioéconomique inférieure, de continuer de recevoir des soins de la vue et d'avoir accès à des lunettes.

20.
Dev Sci ; 16(5): 728-42, 2013 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24033578

RESUMEN

The expertise of adults in face perception is facilitated by their ability to rapidly detect that a stimulus is a face. In two experiments, we examined the role of early visual input in the development of face detection by testing patients who had been treated as infants for bilateral congenital cataract. Experiment 1 indicated that, at age 9 to 20, patients' accuracy and response times on a Mooney face detection task were normal. Experiment 2 revealed that the neural mechanisms underlying face detection in a similar group of adult patients are abnormal: the amplitude of both the P100 and N170 event-related potential were larger in patients than in visually normal controls, and the extent of augmentation was related to the duration of deprivation. Thus, early visual experience is necessary for the establishment of normal neural networks for face detection; abnormalities at these early processing stages may contribute to the deficits we previously reported in configural face processing for this patient cohort.


Asunto(s)
Cara , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Privación Sensorial/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Factores de Edad , Análisis de Varianza , Catarata/congénito , Catarata/fisiopatología , Niño , Electroencefalografía , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Adulto Joven
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA