Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 73
Filtrar
Más filtros

Bases de datos
País/Región como asunto
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 119(28): e2118295119, 2022 07 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35787056

RESUMEN

The biological foundation for the language-ready brain in the human lineage remains a debated subject. In humans, the arcuate fasciculus (AF) white matter and the posterior portions of the middle temporal gyrus are crucial for language. Compared with other primates, the human AF has been shown to dramatically extend into the posterior temporal lobe, which forms the basis of a number of models of the structural connectivity basis of language. Recent advances in both language research and comparative neuroimaging invite a reassessment of the anatomical differences in language streams between humans and our closest relatives. Here, we show that posterior temporal connectivity via the AF in humans compared with chimpanzees is expanded in terms of its connectivity not just to the ventral frontal cortex but also to the parietal cortex. At the same time, posterior temporal regions connect more strongly to the ventral white matter in chimpanzees as opposed to humans. This pattern is present in both brain hemispheres. Additionally, we show that the anterior temporal lobe harbors a combination of connections present in both species through the inferior fronto-occipital fascicle and human-unique expansions through the uncinate and middle and inferior longitudinal fascicles. These findings elucidate structural changes that are unique to humans and may underlie the anatomical foundations for full-fledged language capacity.


Asunto(s)
Sustancia Blanca , Animales , Mapeo Encefálico/métodos , Humanos , Lenguaje , Vías Nerviosas/anatomía & histología , Neuroanatomía , Pan troglodytes/anatomía & histología , Lóbulo Temporal/anatomía & histología , Lóbulo Temporal/diagnóstico por imagen , Sustancia Blanca/anatomía & histología , Sustancia Blanca/diagnóstico por imagen
2.
Eur J Neurosci ; 2024 Jul 23.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39039939

RESUMEN

Language control in bilingual speakers is thought to be implicated in effectively switching between languages, inhibiting the non-intended language, and continuously monitoring what to say and what has been said. It has been a matter of controversy concerning whether language control operates in a comparable manner to cognitive control processes in non-linguistic domains (domain-general) or if it is exclusive to language processing (domain-specific). As midfrontal theta oscillations have been considered as an index of cognitive control, examining whether a midfrontal theta effect is evident in tasks requiring bilingual control could bring new insights to the ongoing debate. To this end, we reanalysed the EEG data from two previous bilingual production studies where Dutch-English bilinguals named pictures based on colour cues. Specifically, we focused on three fundamental control processes in bilingual production: switching between languages, inhibition of the nontarget language, and monitoring of speech errors. Theta power increase was observed in switch trials compared to repeat trials, with a midfrontal scalp distribution. However, no theta power difference was observed in switch trials following a shorter sequence of same-language trials compared to a longer sequence, suggesting a missing modulation of inhibitory control. Similarly, increased midfrontal theta power was observed when participants failed to switch to the intended language compared to correct responses. Altogether, these findings tentatively support the involvement of domain-general cognitive control mechanisms in bilingual switching.

3.
Psychophysiology ; : e14624, 2024 Jun 14.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38873838

RESUMEN

Previous studies have found electroencephalogram (EEG) amplitude and scalp topography differences between neurotypical and neurological/neurosurgical groups, being interpreted at the cognitive level. However, these comparisons are invariably accompanied by anatomical changes. Critical to EEG are the so-called volume currents, which are affected by the spatial distribution of the different tissues in the head. We investigated the effect of cerebrospinal fluid (CSF)-filled cavities on simulated EEG scalp data. We simulated EEG scalp potentials for known sources using different volume conduction models: a reference model (i.e., unlesioned brain) and models with realistic CSF-filled cavities gradually increasing in size. We used this approach for a single source close or far from the CSF-lesion cavity, and for a scenario with a distributed configuration of sources (i.e., a "cognitive event-related potential effect"). The magnitude and topography errors between the reference and lesion models were quantified. For the single-source simulation close to the lesion, the CSF-filled lesion modulated signal amplitude with more than 17% magnitude error and topography with more than 9% topographical error. Negligible modulation was found for the single source far from the lesion. For the multisource simulations of the cognitive effect, the CSF-filled lesion modulated signal amplitude with more than 6% magnitude error and topography with more than 16% topography error in a nonmonotonic fashion. In conclusion, the impact of a CSF-filled cavity cannot be neglected for scalp-level EEG data. Especially when group-level comparisons are made, any scalp-level attenuated, aberrant, or absent effects are difficult to interpret without considering the confounding effect of CSF.

4.
Cereb Cortex ; 33(12): 7816-7829, 2023 06 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37143175

RESUMEN

In the present study, we used chronometric TMS to probe the time-course of 3 brain regions during a picture naming task. The left inferior frontal gyrus, left posterior middle temporal gyrus, and left posterior superior temporal gyrus were all separately stimulated in 1 of 5 time-windows (225, 300, 375, 450, and 525 ms) from picture onset. We found posterior temporal areas to be causally involved in picture naming in earlier time-windows, whereas all 3 regions appear to be involved in the later time-windows. However, chronometric TMS produces nonspecific effects that may impact behavior, and furthermore, the time-course of any given process is a product of both the involved processing stages along with individual variation in the duration of each stage. We therefore extend previous work in the field by accounting for both individual variations in naming latencies and directly testing for nonspecific effects of TMS. Our findings reveal that both factors influence behavioral outcomes at the group level, underlining the importance of accounting for individual variations in naming latencies, especially for late processing stages closer to articulation, and recognizing the presence of nonspecific effects of TMS. The paper advances key considerations and avenues for future work using chronometric TMS to study overt production.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico , Neocórtex , Lóbulo Temporal , Corteza Prefrontal , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador
5.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc ; 29(2): 113-125, 2023 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35225209

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: While declarative learning is dependent on the hippocampus, procedural learning and repetition priming can operate independently from the hippocampus, making them potential targets for behavioral interventions that utilize non-declarative memory systems to compensate for the declarative learning deficits associated with hippocampal insult. Few studies have assessed procedural learning and repetition priming in individuals with amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI). METHOD: This study offers an overview across declarative, conceptual repetition priming, and procedural learning tasks by providing between-group effect sizes and Bayes Factors (BFs) comparing individuals with aMCI and controls. Seventy-six individuals with aMCI and 83 cognitively unimpaired controls were assessed. We hypothesized to see the largest differences between individuals with aMCI and controls on declarative learning, followed by conceptual repetition priming, with the smallest differences on procedural learning. RESULTS: Consistent with our hypotheses, we found large differences between groups with supporting BFs on declarative learning. For conceptual repetition priming, we found a small-to-moderate between-group effect size and a non-conclusive BF somewhat in favor of a difference between groups. We found more variable but overall trivial differences on procedural learning tasks, with inconclusive BFs, in line with expectations. CONCLUSIONS: The current results suggest that conceptual repetition priming does not remain intact in individuals with aMCI while procedural learning may remain intact. While additional studies are needed, our results contribute to the evidence-base that suggests that procedural learning may remain spared in aMCI and helps inform behavioral interventions that aim to utilize procedural learning in this population.


Asunto(s)
Disfunción Cognitiva , Aprendizaje , Humanos , Anciano , Teorema de Bayes , Disfunción Cognitiva/psicología , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas
6.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 34(2): 209-223, 2022 01 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34813643

RESUMEN

Idioms can have both a literal interpretation and a figurative interpretation (e.g., to "kick the bucket"). Which interpretation should be activated can be disambiguated by a preceding context (e.g., "The old man was sick. He kicked the bucket."). We investigated whether the idiomatic and literal uses of idioms have different predictive properties when the idiom has been biased toward a literal or figurative sentence interpretation. EEG was recorded as participants performed a lexical decision task on idiom-final words in biased idioms and literal (compositional) sentences. Targets in idioms were identified faster in both figuratively and literally used idioms than in compositional sentences. Time-frequency analysis of a prestimulus interval revealed relatively more alpha-beta power decreases in literally than figuratively used idiomatic sequences and compositional sentences. We argue that lexico-semantic retrieval plays a larger role in literally than figuratively biased idioms, as retrieval of the word meaning is less relevant in the latter and the word form has to be matched to a template. The results are interpreted in terms of context integration and word retrieval and have implications for models of language processing and predictive processing in general.


Asunto(s)
Psicolingüística , Lectura , Comprensión , Humanos , Lenguaje , Masculino , Semántica
7.
Neuroimage ; 260: 119422, 2022 10 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35781078

RESUMEN

Source reconstruction of magnetoencephalography (MEG) has been used to assess brain reorganization after brain damage, such as stroke. Lesions result in parts of the brain having an electrical conductivity that differs from the normal values. The effect this has on the forward solutions (i.e., the propagation of electric currents and magnetic fields generated by cortical activity) is well predictable. However, their influence on source localization results is not well characterized and understood. This is specifically a concern for patient studies with asymmetric (i.e., within one hemisphere) lesions focusing on asymmetric and lateralized brain activity, such as language. In particular, it is good practice to consider the level of geometrical detail that is necessary to compute and interpret reliable source reconstruction results. To understand the effect of lesions on source estimates and propose recommendations to researchers working with clinical data, in this study we consider the trade off between improved accuracy and the additional effort to compute more realistic head models, with the aim to answer the question whether the additional effort is worth it. We simulated and analyzed the effects of a stroke lesion (i.e., an asymmetrically distributed CSF-filled cavity) in the head model with three different sizes and locations when performing MEG source reconstruction using a finite element method (FEM). We compared the effect of the lesion with a homogeneous head model that neglects the lesion. We computed displacement and attenuation/amplification maps to quantify the localization errors and signal magnitude modulation. We conclude that brain lesions leading to asymmetrically distributed CSF-filled cavities should be modeled when performing MEG source reconstruction, especially when investigating deep sources or post-stroke hemispheric lateralization of functions. The strongest effects are not only visible in perilesional areas, but can extend up to 20 mm from the lesion. Bigger lesions lead to stronger effects impacting larger areas, independently from the lesion location. Lastly, we conclude that more priority should be given to usability and accessibility of the required computational tools, to allow researchers with less technical expertise to use the improved methods that are available but currently not widely adopted yet.


Asunto(s)
Electroencefalografía , Magnetoencefalografía , Encéfalo , Mapeo Encefálico/métodos , Electroencefalografía/métodos , Cabeza , Humanos , Magnetoencefalografía/métodos
8.
Neuroimage ; 260: 119438, 2022 10 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35792291

RESUMEN

Since the second-half of the twentieth century, intracranial electroencephalography (iEEG), including both electrocorticography (ECoG) and stereo-electroencephalography (sEEG), has provided an intimate view into the human brain. At the interface between fundamental research and the clinic, iEEG provides both high temporal resolution and high spatial specificity but comes with constraints, such as the individual's tailored sparsity of electrode sampling. Over the years, researchers in neuroscience developed their practices to make the most of the iEEG approach. Here we offer a critical review of iEEG research practices in a didactic framework for newcomers, as well addressing issues encountered by proficient researchers. The scope is threefold: (i) review common practices in iEEG research, (ii) suggest potential guidelines for working with iEEG data and answer frequently asked questions based on the most widespread practices, and (iii) based on current neurophysiological knowledge and methodologies, pave the way to good practice standards in iEEG research. The organization of this paper follows the steps of iEEG data processing. The first section contextualizes iEEG data collection. The second section focuses on localization of intracranial electrodes. The third section highlights the main pre-processing steps. The fourth section presents iEEG signal analysis methods. The fifth section discusses statistical approaches. The sixth section draws some unique perspectives on iEEG research. Finally, to ensure a consistent nomenclature throughout the manuscript and to align with other guidelines, e.g., Brain Imaging Data Structure (BIDS) and the OHBM Committee on Best Practices in Data Analysis and Sharing (COBIDAS), we provide a glossary to disambiguate terms related to iEEG research.


Asunto(s)
Electrocorticografía , Electroencefalografía , Encéfalo/fisiología , Mapeo Encefálico/métodos , Electrocorticografía/métodos , Electrodos , Electroencefalografía/métodos , Humanos
9.
Eur J Neurosci ; 56(8): 5235-5259, 2022 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36028218

RESUMEN

Our understanding of post-stroke language function is largely based on older age groups, who show increasing age-related brain pathology and neural reorganisation. To illustrate language outcomes in the young-adult brain, we present the case of J., a 23-year-old woman with chronic aphasia from a left-hemisphere stroke affecting the temporal lobe. Diffusion MRI-based tractography indicated that J.'s language-relevant white-matter structures were severely damaged. Employing magnetoencephalography (MEG), we explored J.'s conceptual preparation and word planning abilities using context-driven and bare picture-naming tasks. These revealed naming deficits, manifesting as word-finding difficulties and semantic paraphasias about half of the time. Naming was however facilitated by semantically constraining lead-in sentences. Altogether, this pattern indicates disrupted lexical-semantic and phonological retrieval abilities. MEG revealed that J.'s conceptual and naming-related neural responses were supported by the right hemisphere, compared to the typical left-lateralised brain response of a matched control. Differential recruitment of right-hemisphere structures (330-440 ms post-picture onset) was found concurrently during successful naming (right mid-to-posterior temporal lobe) and word-finding attempts (right inferior frontal gyrus). Disconnection of the temporal lobes via corpus callosum was not critical for recruitment of the right hemisphere in visually guided naming, possibly due to neural activity right lateralising from the outset. Although J.'s right hemisphere responded in a timely manner during word planning, its lexical and phonological retrieval abilities remained modest.


Asunto(s)
Afasia , Accidente Cerebrovascular , Adulto , Anciano , Afasia/patología , Mapeo Encefálico , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Semántica , Accidente Cerebrovascular/complicaciones , Accidente Cerebrovascular/diagnóstico por imagen , Lóbulo Temporal/diagnóstico por imagen , Adulto Joven
10.
Neuropsychol Rev ; 2022 Dec 24.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36564612

RESUMEN

Clinical populations with basal ganglia pathologies may present with language production impairments, which are often described in combination with comprehension measures or attributed to motor, memory, or processing-speed problems. In this systematic review and meta-analysis, we studied word production in four (vascular and non-vascular) pathologies of the basal ganglia: stroke affecting the basal ganglia, small vessel disease, Parkinson's disease, and Huntington's disease. We compared scores of these clinical populations with those of matched cognitively unimpaired adults on four well-established production tasks, namely picture naming, category fluency, letter fluency, and past-tense verb inflection. We conducted a systematic search in PubMed and PsycINFO with terms for basal ganglia structures, basal ganglia disorders and language production tasks. A total of 114 studies were included, containing results for one or more of the tasks of interest. For each pathology and task combination, effect sizes (Hedges' g) were extracted comparing patient versus control groups. For all four populations, performance was consistently worse than that of cognitively unimpaired adults across the four language production tasks (p-values < 0.010). Given that performance in picture naming and verb inflection across all pathologies was quantified in terms of accuracy, our results suggest that production impairments cannot be fully explained by motor or processing-speed deficits. Our review shows that while language production difficulties in these clinical populations are not negligible, more evidence is necessary to determine the exact mechanism that leads to these deficits and whether this mechanism is the same across different pathologies.

11.
Neuropsychol Rev ; 32(2): 228-246, 2022 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33895980

RESUMEN

The literature on repetition priming in Alzheimer's disease (AD) is inconsistent, with some findings supporting spared priming while others do not. Several factors may explain these inconsistencies, including AD severity (e.g., dementia vs. Mild Cognitive Impairment; MCI) and priming paradigm-related characteristics. This systematic review and meta-analysis provides a quantitative summary of repetition priming in AD. We examined the between-group standard mean difference comparing repetition priming in AD dementia or amnestic MCI (aMCI; presumably due to AD) to controls. Thirty-two studies were selected, including 590 individuals with AD dementia, 267 individuals with amnestic MCI, and 703 controls. Our results indicated that both individuals with aMCI and AD dementia perform worse on repetition priming tasks than cognitively older adults. Paradigm-related moderators suggested that the effect size between studies comparing the combined aMCI or AD dementia group to cognitively healthy older adults was the highest for paradigms that required participants to produce, rather than identify, primes during the test phase. Our results further suggested that priming in AD is impaired for both conceptual and perceptual priming tasks. Lastly, while our results suggested that priming in AD is impaired for priming tasks that require deep processing, we were unable to draw firm conclusions about whether priming is less impaired in aMCI or AD dementia for paradigms that require shallow processing.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedad de Alzheimer , Disfunción Cognitiva , Anciano , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/complicaciones , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/psicología , Disfunción Cognitiva/complicaciones , Disfunción Cognitiva/psicología , Humanos , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Memoria Implícita , Índice de Severidad de la Enfermedad
12.
BMC Cancer ; 22(1): 244, 2022 Mar 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35248013

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: With a growing, younger population of head and neck cancer survivors, attention to long-term side-effects of prior, often radiotherapeutic, treatment is warranted. Therefore, we studied the long-term cognitive effects in young adult patients irradiated for head and neck neoplasms (HNN). METHODS: Young to middle-aged adults with HNN (aged 18-40 years) and treated with unilateral neck irradiation ≥ 5 years before inclusion underwent cardiovascular risk and neuropsychological assessments and answered validated questionnaires regarding subjective cognitive complaints, fatigue, depression, quality of life, and cancer-specific distress. Additionally, magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) of the brain was performed to assess white matter hyperintensities (WMH), infarctions, and atrophy. RESULTS: Twenty-nine patients (aged 24-61, 13 men) median 9.2 [7.3-12.9] years post-treatment were included. HNN patients performed worse in episodic memory (Z-score = -1.16 [-1.58-0.34], p < 0.001) and reported more fatigue symptoms (Z-score = 1.75 [1.21-2.00], p < 0.001) compared to normative data. Furthermore, patients had a high level of fear of tumor recurrence (13 patients [44.8%]) and a heightened speech handicap index (13 patients [44.8%]). Only a small number of neurovascular lesions were found (3 infarctions in 2 patients and 0.11 [0.00-0.40] mL WMH), unrelated to the irradiated side. Cognitive impairment was not associated with WMH, brain atrophy, fatigue, or subjective speech problems. CONCLUSIONS: HNN patients showed impairments in episodic memory and an increased level of fatigue ≥ 5 years after radiotherapy compared to normative data. Cognitive impairments could not be explained by WMH or brain atrophy on brain MRI or psychological factors. TRIAL REGISTRATION: Clinicaltrials.gov ( https://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT04257968 ).


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/patología , Supervivientes de Cáncer/psicología , Neoplasias de Cabeza y Cuello/psicología , Traumatismos por Radiación/psicología , Sustancia Blanca/patología , Adulto , Atrofia , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Disfunción Cognitiva/etiología , Depresión/etiología , Fatiga/etiología , Femenino , Neoplasias de Cabeza y Cuello/radioterapia , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Neuroimagen , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Tamaño de los Órganos , Distrés Psicológico , Calidad de Vida , Sustancia Blanca/diagnóstico por imagen , Adulto Joven
13.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 42(4): 993-1002, 2021 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33231360

RESUMEN

The presence of white matter lesions in patients with cerebral small vessel disease (SVD) is among the main causes of cognitive decline. We investigated the relation between white matter hyperintensity (WMH) locations and executive and language abilities in 442 SVD patients without dementia with varying burden of WMH. We used Stroop Word Reading, Stroop Color Naming, Stroop Color-Word Naming, and Category Fluency as language measures with varying degrees of executive demands. The Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT) was used as a control task, as it measures processing speed without requiring language use or verbal output. A voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping (VLSM) approach was used, corrected for age, sex, education, and lesion volume. VLSM analyses revealed statistically significant clusters for tests requiring language use, but not for SDMT. Worse scores on all tests were associated with WMH in forceps minor, thalamic radiations and caudate nuclei. In conclusion, an association was found between WMH in a core frontostriatal network and executive-verbal abilities in SVD, independent of lesion volume and processing speed. This circuitry underlying executive-language functioning might be of potential clinical importance for elderly with SVD. More detailed language testing is required in future research to elucidate the nature of language production difficulties in SVD.


Asunto(s)
Núcleo Caudado , Enfermedades de los Pequeños Vasos Cerebrales , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Red Nerviosa , Corteza Prefrontal , Psicolingüística , Sustancia Blanca , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Núcleo Caudado/diagnóstico por imagen , Núcleo Caudado/patología , Núcleo Caudado/fisiopatología , Enfermedades de los Pequeños Vasos Cerebrales/diagnóstico por imagen , Enfermedades de los Pequeños Vasos Cerebrales/patología , Enfermedades de los Pequeños Vasos Cerebrales/fisiopatología , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Red Nerviosa/diagnóstico por imagen , Red Nerviosa/patología , Red Nerviosa/fisiopatología , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Corteza Prefrontal/diagnóstico por imagen , Corteza Prefrontal/patología , Corteza Prefrontal/fisiopatología , Sustancia Blanca/diagnóstico por imagen , Sustancia Blanca/patología , Sustancia Blanca/fisiopatología
14.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 32(8): 1497-1507, 2020 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32286133

RESUMEN

Little is known about language impairment in brain tumor patients, especially in the presurgical phase. Impairment in this population may be missed because standardized tests fail to capture mild deficits. Additionally, neuroplasticity may also contribute to minimizing language impairments. We examined 14 presurgical patients with brain tumors in the language-dominant hemisphere using magnetoencephalography (MEG) while they performed a demanding picture-word interference task, that is, participants name pictures while ignoring distractor words. Brain tumor patients had behavioral picture-naming effects typically observed in healthy controls. The MEG responses also showed the expected pattern in its timing and amplitude modulation typical of controls, but with an altered spatial distribution of right hemisphere sources, in contrast to the classic left hemisphere source found in healthy individuals. This finding supports tumor-induced neural reorganization of language before surgery. Crucially, the use of electrophysiology allowed us to show the "same" neuronal response in terms of its timing and amplitude modulation in the right hemisphere, supporting the hypothesis that the processes performed by the right hemisphere following reorganization are similar in nature to those (previously) performed by the left hemisphere. We also identified one participant with a fast-growing tumor affecting large parts of critical language areas and underlying ventral and dorsal white matter tracts who showed a deviant pattern in behavior and in the MEG event-related responses. In conclusion, our results attest to the validity of using a demanding picture-naming task in presurgical patients and provide evidence for neuroplasticity, with the right hemisphere performing similar computations as the left hemisphere typically performs.


Asunto(s)
Neoplasias Encefálicas , Magnetoencefalografía , Mapeo Encefálico , Humanos , Lenguaje , Plasticidad Neuronal
15.
Eur J Neurosci ; 52(5): 3457-3469, 2020 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32432366

RESUMEN

Changes in brain organization following damage are commonly observed, but they remain poorly understood. These changes are often studied with imaging techniques that overlook the temporal granularity at which language processes occur. By contrast, electrophysiological measures provide excellent temporal resolution. To test the suitability of magnetoencephalography (MEG) to track language-related neuroplasticity, the present study aimed at establishing the spectro-temporo-spatial across-session consistency of context-driven picture naming in healthy individuals, using MEG in two test-retest sessions. Spectro-temporo-spatial test-retest consistency in a healthy population is a prerequisite for studying neuronal changes in clinical populations over time. For this purpose, 15 healthy speakers were tested with MEG while performing a context-driven picture-naming task at two time points. Participants read a sentence missing the final word and named a picture completing the sentence. Sentences were constrained or unconstrained toward the picture, such that participants could either retrieve the picture name through sentence context (constrained sentences), or could only name it after the picture appeared (unconstrained sentences). The context effect (constrained versus unconstrained) in picture-naming times had a strong effect size and high across-session consistency. The context MEG results revealed alpha-beta power decreases (10-20 Hz) in the left temporal and inferior parietal lobule that were consistent across both sessions. As robust spectro-temporo-spatial findings in a healthy population are required for working toward longitudinal patient studies, we conclude that using context-driven language production and MEG is a suitable way to examine language-related neuroplasticity after brain damage.


Asunto(s)
Lenguaje , Magnetoencefalografía , Encéfalo , Mapeo Encefálico , Humanos , Lectura
16.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 41(4): 1061-1071, 2020 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31705740

RESUMEN

While much progress has been made in how brain organization supports language function, the language network's ability to adapt to immediate disturbances by means of reorganization remains unclear. The aim of this study was to examine acute reorganizational changes in brain activity related to conceptual and lexical retrieval in unimpaired language production following transient disruption of the left middle temporal gyrus (MTG). In a randomized single-blind within-subject experiment, we recorded the electroencephalogram from 16 healthy participants during a context-driven picture-naming task. Prior to the task, the left MTG was perturbed with real continuous theta-burst stimulation (cTBS) or sham stimulation. During the task, participants read lead-in sentences creating a constraining (e.g., "The farmer milks the") or nonconstraining context (e.g., "The farmer buys the"). The last word was shown as a picture that participants had to name (e.g., "cow"). Replicating behavioral studies, participants were overall faster in naming pictures following a constraining relative to a nonconstraining context, but this effect did not differ between real and sham cTBS. In contrast, real cTBS increased overall error rates compared to sham cTBS. In line with previous studies, we observed a decrease in alpha-beta (8-24 Hz) oscillatory power for constraining relative to nonconstraining contexts over left temporal-parietal cortex after participants received sham cTBS. However, following real cTBS, this decrease extended toward left prefrontal regions associated with both domain-general and domain-specific control mechanisms. Our findings provide evidence that immediately after perturbing the left MTG, the lexical-semantic network is able to quickly reconfigure, also recruiting domain-general regions.


Asunto(s)
Ondas Encefálicas/fisiología , Red Nerviosa/fisiología , Plasticidad Neuronal/fisiología , Corteza Prefrontal/fisiología , Psicolingüística , Lóbulo Temporal/fisiología , Estimulación Magnética Transcraneal , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Lectura , Método Simple Ciego , Habla/fisiología , Lóbulo Temporal/diagnóstico por imagen , Adulto Joven
17.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 113(40): 11366-11371, 2016 10 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27647880

RESUMEN

Language is classically thought to be supported by perisylvian cortical regions. Here we provide intracranial evidence linking the hippocampal complex to linguistic processing. We used direct recordings from the hippocampal structures to investigate whether theta oscillations, pivotal in memory function, track the amount of contextual linguistic information provided in sentences. Twelve participants heard sentences that were either constrained ("She locked the door with the") or unconstrained ("She walked in here with the") before presentation of the final word ("key"), shown as a picture that participants had to name. Hippocampal theta power increased for constrained relative to unconstrained contexts during sentence processing, preceding picture presentation. Our study implicates hippocampal theta oscillations in a language task using natural language associations that do not require memorization. These findings reveal that the hippocampal complex contributes to language in an active fashion, relating incoming words to stored semantic knowledge, a necessary process in the generation of sentence meaning.


Asunto(s)
Hipocampo/fisiología , Lenguaje , Ritmo Teta/fisiología , Adulto , Anciano , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Femenino , Hipocampo/anatomía & histología , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Semántica , Factores de Tiempo , Vocabulario
18.
Eur J Neurosci ; 48(7): 2622-2629, 2018 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28887896

RESUMEN

Different frequency bands in the electroencephalogram are postulated to support distinct language functions. Studies have suggested that alpha-beta power decreases may index word-retrieval processes. In context-driven word retrieval, participants hear lead-in sentences that either constrain the final word ('He locked the door with the') or not ('She walked in here with the'). The last word is shown as a picture to be named. Previous studies have consistently found alpha-beta power decreases prior to picture onset for constrained relative to unconstrained sentences, localised to the left lateral-temporal and lateral-frontal lobes. However, the relative contribution of temporal versus frontal areas to alpha-beta power decreases is unknown. We recorded the electroencephalogram from patients with stroke lesions encompassing the left lateral-temporal and inferior-parietal regions or left-lateral frontal lobe and from matched controls. Individual participant analyses revealed a behavioural sentence context facilitation effect in all participants, except for in the two patients with extensive lesions to temporal and inferior parietal lobes. We replicated the alpha-beta power decreases prior to picture onset in all participants, except for in the two same patients with extensive posterior lesions. Thus, whereas posterior lesions eliminated the behavioural and oscillatory context effect, frontal lesions did not. Hierarchical clustering analyses of all patients' lesion profiles, and behavioural and electrophysiological effects identified those two patients as having a unique combination of lesion distribution and context effects. These results indicate a critical role for the left lateral-temporal and inferior parietal lobes, but not frontal cortex, in generating the alpha-beta power decreases underlying context-driven word production.


Asunto(s)
Electroencefalografía , Lóbulo Frontal/fisiología , Lenguaje , Lóbulo Temporal/fisiología , Anciano , Conducta/fisiología , Electroencefalografía/métodos , Femenino , Humanos , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador/métodos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Lóbulo Parietal/fisiología , Accidente Cerebrovascular/fisiopatología
19.
Hum Genet ; 137(5): 389-400, 2018 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29754270

RESUMEN

Unraveling the causes and pathomechanisms of progressive disorders is essential for the development of therapeutic strategies. Here, we identified heterozygous pathogenic missense variants of LMX1A in two families of Dutch origin with progressive nonsyndromic hearing impairment (HI), using whole exome sequencing. One variant, c.721G > C (p.Val241Leu), occurred de novo and is predicted to affect the homeodomain of LMX1A, which is essential for DNA binding. The second variant, c.290G > C (p.Cys97Ser), predicted to affect a zinc-binding residue of the second LIM domain that is involved in protein-protein interactions. Bi-allelic deleterious variants of Lmx1a are associated with a complex phenotype in mice, including deafness and vestibular defects, due to arrest of inner ear development. Although Lmx1a mouse mutants demonstrate neurological, skeletal, pigmentation and reproductive system abnormalities, no syndromic features were present in the participating subjects of either family. LMX1A has previously been suggested as a candidate gene for intellectual disability, but our data do not support this, as affected subjects displayed normal cognition. Large variability was observed in the age of onset (a)symmetry, severity and progression rate of HI. About half of the affected individuals displayed vestibular dysfunction and experienced symptoms thereof. The late-onset progressive phenotype and the absence of cochleovestibular malformations on computed tomography scans indicate that heterozygous defects of LMX1A do not result in severe developmental abnormalities in humans. We propose that a single LMX1A wild-type copy is sufficient for normal development but insufficient for maintenance of cochleovestibular function. Alternatively, minor cochleovestibular developmental abnormalities could eventually lead to the progressive phenotype seen in the families.


Asunto(s)
Pérdida Auditiva/genética , Heterocigoto , Proteínas con Homeodominio LIM/genética , Mutación Missense , Factores de Transcripción/genética , Enfermedades Vestibulares/genética , Adulto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Sustitución de Aminoácidos , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad
20.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 38(6): 3151-3162, 2017 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28345282

RESUMEN

The understanding of neuroplasticity following stroke is predominantly based on neuroimaging measures that cannot address the subsecond neurodynamics of impaired language processing. We combined behavioral and electrophysiological measures and structural-connectivity estimates to characterize neuroplasticity underlying successful compensation of language abilities after left-hemispheric stroke. We recorded the electroencephalogram from patients with stroke lesions to the left temporal lobe and from matched controls during context-driven word retrieval. Participants heard lead-in sentences that either constrained the final word ("He locked the door with the") or not ("She walked in here with the"). The last word was shown as a picture to be named. Individual-participant analyses were conducted, focusing on oscillatory power as a subsecond indicator of a brain region's functional neurophysiological computations. All participants named pictures faster following constrained than unconstrained sentences, except for two patients, who had extensive damage to the left temporal lobe. Left-lateralized alpha-beta oscillatory power decreased in controls pre-picture presentation for constrained relative to unconstrained contexts. In patients, the alpha-beta power decreases were observed with the same time course as in controls but were lateralized to the intact right hemisphere. The right lateralization depended on the probability of white-matter connections between the bilateral temporal lobes. The two patients who performed poorly behaviorally showed no alpha-beta power decreases. Our findings suggest that incorporating direct measures of neural activity into investigations of neuroplasticity can provide important neural markers to help predict language recovery, assess the progress of neurorehabilitation, and delineate targets for therapeutic neuromodulation. Hum Brain Mapp 38:3151-3162, 2017. © 2017 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.


Asunto(s)
Ondas Encefálicas/fisiología , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Trastornos del Lenguaje/etiología , Trastornos del Lenguaje/patología , Accidente Cerebrovascular/complicaciones , Lóbulo Temporal/fisiopatología , Anciano , Mapeo Encefálico , Comprensión/fisiología , Electroencefalografía , Femenino , Humanos , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador , Trastornos del Lenguaje/diagnóstico por imagen , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Recuerdo Mental , Persona de Mediana Edad , Plasticidad Neuronal/fisiología , Estadísticas no Paramétricas , Accidente Cerebrovascular/diagnóstico por imagen , Lóbulo Temporal/diagnóstico por imagen , Aprendizaje Verbal
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA