Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 5 de 5
Filtrar
1.
Behav Res Methods ; 48(4): 1343-1348, 2016 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26660198

RESUMEN

Change blindness has been a topic of interest in cognitive sciences for decades. Change detection experiments are frequently used for studying various research topics such as attention and perception. However, creating change detection stimuli is tedious and there is no open repository of such stimuli using natural scenes. We introduce the Change Blindness (CB) Database with object changes in 130 colored images of natural indoor scenes. The size and eccentricity are provided for all the changes as well as reaction time data from a baseline experiment. In addition, we have two specialized satellite databases that are subsets of the 130 images. In one set, changes are seen in rooms or in mirrors in those rooms (Mirror Change Database). In the other, changes occur in a room or out a window (Window Change Database). Both the sets have controlled background, change size, and eccentricity. The CB Database is intended to provide researchers with a stimulus set of natural scenes with defined stimulus parameters that can be used for a wide range of experiments. The CB Database can be found at http://search.bwh.harvard.edu/new/CBDatabase.html .


Asunto(s)
Bases de Datos Factuales , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Percepción Visual , Humanos , Tiempo de Reacción
2.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 108(17): 7230-5, 2011 Apr 26.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21482795

RESUMEN

Organisms with complex visual systems rarely respond to just the sum of all visual stimuli impinging on their eyes. Often, they restrict their responses to stimuli in a temporarily selected region of the visual field (selective visual attention). Here, we investigate visual attention in the fly Drosophila during tethered flight at a torque meter. Flies can actively shift their attention; however, their attention can be guided to a certain location by external cues. Using visual cues, we can direct the attention of the fly to one or the other of the two visual half-fields. The cue can precede the test stimulus by several seconds and may also be spatially separated from the test by at least 20° and yet attract attention. This kind of external guidance of attention is found only in the lower visual field.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Conducta Animal/fisiología , Vuelo Animal/fisiología , Animales , Drosophila , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos
3.
Nat Commun ; 12(1): 4131, 2021 07 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34226544

RESUMEN

Feeding decisions are fundamental to survival, and decision making is often disrupted in disease. Here, we show that neural activity in a small population of neurons projecting to the fan-shaped body higher-order central brain region of Drosophila represents food choice during sensory conflict. We found that food deprived flies made tradeoffs between appetitive and aversive values of food. We identified an upstream neuropeptidergic and dopaminergic network that relays internal state and other decision-relevant information to a specific subset of fan-shaped body neurons. These neurons were strongly inhibited by the taste of the rejected food choice, suggesting that they encode behavioral food choice. Our findings reveal that fan-shaped body taste responses to food choices are determined not only by taste quality, but also by previous experience (including choice outcome) and hunger state, which are integrated in the fan-shaped body to encode the decision before relay to downstream motor circuits for behavioral implementation.


Asunto(s)
Proteínas de Drosophila/metabolismo , Drosophila melanogaster/metabolismo , Drosophila/metabolismo , Neuronas/metabolismo , Animales , Encéfalo/metabolismo , Toma de Decisiones , Conducta Alimentaria/fisiología , Preferencias Alimentarias , Hambre/fisiología , Gusto/fisiología , Percepción del Gusto
4.
Nat Commun ; 12(1): 1115, 2021 02 18.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33602917

RESUMEN

Animals form and update learned associations between otherwise neutral sensory cues and aversive outcomes (i.e., punishment) to predict and avoid danger in changing environments. When a cue later occurs without punishment, this unexpected omission of aversive outcome is encoded as reward via activation of reward-encoding dopaminergic neurons. How such activation occurs remains unknown. Using real-time in vivo functional imaging, optogenetics, behavioral analysis and synaptic reconstruction from electron microscopy data, we identify the neural circuit mechanism through which Drosophila reward-encoding dopaminergic neurons are activated when an olfactory cue is unexpectedly no longer paired with electric shock punishment. Reduced activation of punishment-encoding dopaminergic neurons relieves depression of olfactory synaptic inputs to cholinergic neurons. Synaptic excitation by these cholinergic neurons of reward-encoding dopaminergic neurons increases their odor response, thus decreasing aversiveness of the odor. These studies reveal how an excitatory cholinergic relay from punishment- to reward-encoding dopaminergic neurons encodes the absence of punishment as reward, revealing a general circuit motif for updating aversive memories that could be present in mammals.


Asunto(s)
Dopamina/metabolismo , Drosophila melanogaster/fisiología , Castigo , Recompensa , Animales , Reacción de Prevención/fisiología , Condicionamiento Clásico , Neuronas Dopaminérgicas/fisiología , Memoria/fisiología , Aprendizaje Inverso , Olfato/fisiología , Sinapsis/fisiología
5.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 22(4): 980-6, 2015 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25407658

RESUMEN

Is an object reflected in a mirror perceived differently from an object that is seen directly? We asked observers to label "everything" in photographs of real-world scenes. Some scenes contained a mirror in which objects could be seen. Reflected objects received significantly fewer labels than did their nonreflected counterparts. If an object was visible only as a reflection, it was labeled more often than a reflected object that appeared both as a reflection and nonreflected in the room. These unique reflected objects were still not labeled more often than the unique nonreflected objects in the room. In a second experiment, we used a change blindness paradigm in which equivalent object changes occurred in the nonreflected and reflected parts of the scene. Reaction times were longer and accuracy was lower for finding the changes in reflections. These results suggest that reflected information is easily discounted when processing images of natural scenes.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Tiempo de Reacción , Adulto Joven
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA