Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 25
Filtrar
Más filtros

Bases de datos
País/Región como asunto
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Psychol Res ; 84(6): 1766-1776, 2020 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30980237

RESUMEN

Growing evidence suggests that the reliance on cognitive control processes during normal walking increases as the locomotor task gets more complex and challenging. The aims of the present study were to explore the (negative) effects of smartphone gaming on gait performance in healthy young adults, and to identify cognitive resources that might help to maintain high gait performance during dual-task walking. Gait speed and gait variability during walking at a self-selected comfortable speed were assessed in 40 healthy, young adults, and compared between single-task and dual-task walking (i.e., concurrent smartphone gaming) in undisturbed, simple and more challenging walking environments (i.e., stepping over an obstacle while walking). Based on single-task performance, dual-tasking costs were computed and linked to higher-level cognitive control processes, which were assessed for each individual. Cognitive function testing encompassed tests on the mental representation of the gait, working memory capacity, inhibitory control and cognitive flexibility. Our data revealed that gaming on a smartphone while walking strongly affected participants' gait performance (i.e., up to 26.8% lower gait speed and 60.2% higher gait variability), and decrements in gait performance were related to higher cognitive control processes. Cognitive resources that were associated with performance decrements in dual-task walking include response inhibition, cognitive flexibility, working memory, and a well-structured mental representation of the gait. From that, it appears that even in healthy young adults better cognitive resources may help to maintain high gait performance in situations, in which we have to deal with dual- or multi-task demands (e.g., using a smartphone) while walking.


Asunto(s)
Cognición/fisiología , Marcha/fisiología , Voluntarios Sanos/psicología , Caminata/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Inhibición Psicológica , Masculino , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Teléfono Inteligente , Juegos de Video/efectos adversos , Adulto Joven
2.
Clin Rehabil ; 33(2): 335-344, 2019 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30322264

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE:: To explore differences in gait-specific long-term memory structures and actual gait performance between patients with hip osteoarthrosis, patients seen six months after total hip arthroplasty and healthy controls to gain insights into the role of the gait-specific mental representation for rehabilitation. DESIGN:: Cross-sectional study. SUBJECTS:: Twenty hip osteoarthrosis patients, 20 patients seen six months after total hip arthroplasty and 20 healthy controls. METHODS:: Spatio-temporal (gait speed, step length) and temporophasic (stance time, swing time, single support time, total double support time) gait parameters, and gait variability were measured with an electronic walkway (OptoGait). The gait-specific mental representation was assessed using the structural dimensional analysis of mental representations (SDA-M). RESULTS:: Hip osteoarthrosis patients showed significantly longer stance and total double support times, shorter swing and single support times, and a decreased gait speed as compared with healthy controls (all P < 0.01). The differences in double support times were still evident in patients seen six months after total hip arthroplasty ( P < 0.01). The gait-specific mental representation differed between hip osteoarthrosis patients and healthy controls with regard to mid-stance and mid-swing phases; the mid-stance phase was still affected six months after total hip arthroplasty (both P < 0.05). CONCLUSION:: Our data indicated that actual gait performance and gait-specific long-term memory structures differ between hip osteoarthrosis patients and healthy controls. Important, some of these disease-related changes were still evident in patients seen six months after total hip arthroplasty.


Asunto(s)
Artroplastia de Reemplazo de Cadera , Marcha/fisiología , Memoria , Osteoartritis de la Cadera/psicología , Osteoartritis de la Cadera/cirugía , Anciano , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Estudios Transversales , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Osteoartritis de la Cadera/fisiopatología
3.
Clin Rehabil ; 32(1): 103-115, 2018 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28719981

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: Objectives were (1) to explore differences in gait-specific long-term memory structures and gait performance between knee osteoarthrosis patients and healthy subjects and (2) to identify the extent to which the gait-specific mental representation is associated with gait performance. DESIGN: Cross-sectional study. SUBJECTS: In total, 18 knee osteoarthrosis patients and 18 control subjects. METHODS: Spatio-temporal (gait speed, step length) and temporophasic (stance time, swing time, single support time, total double support time) gait parameters and gait variability were measured with an electronic walkway (OptoGait). The mental representation was assessed using the structural dimensional analysis of mental representations (SDA-M). RESULTS: (1) Patients showed significantly longer stance times ( P < 0.002) and total double support times, shorter swing times and single support times, a decreased gait speed ( P-values < 0.001) and structural differences in the gait-specific mental representation as compared with the healthy controls. (2) Correlation analyses revealed the mental representation of the human gait to be associated with actual gait performance in osteoarthrosis patients. Double support times were positively associated with the structural quality of the mental representation and step length variability was positively associated with the number of sequencing errors in the representation. CONCLUSION: The gait-specific mental representation and actual gait performance differ between patients with severe knee osteoarthrosis and healthy controls, and both are linked to one another. This finding suggests that musculoskeletal disorders can lead to changes in the mental representation of the gait, and as such the SDA-M could provide useful information to improve the rehabilitation following osteoarthrosis.


Asunto(s)
Marcha/fisiología , Memoria , Osteoartritis de la Rodilla/fisiopatología , Osteoartritis de la Rodilla/psicología , Anciano , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Estudios Transversales , Miedo , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Dolor
4.
J Neurophysiol ; 116(2): 575-86, 2016 08 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27169508

RESUMEN

Performance benefits conferred in the untrained limb after unilateral motor practice are termed cross-limb transfer. Although the effect is robust, the neural mechanisms remain incompletely understood. In this study we used noninvasive brain stimulation to reveal that the neural adaptations that mediate motor learning in the trained limb are distinct from those that underlie cross-limb transfer to the opposite limb. Thirty-six participants practiced a ballistic motor task with their right index finger (150 trials), followed by intermittent theta-burst stimulation (iTBS) applied to the trained (contralateral) primary motor cortex (cM1 group), the untrained (ipsilateral) M1 (iM1 group), or the vertex (sham group). After stimulation, another 150 training trials were undertaken. Motor performance and corticospinal excitability were assessed before motor training, pre- and post-iTBS, and after the second training bout. For all groups, training significantly increased performance and excitability of the trained hand, and performance, but not excitability, of the untrained hand, indicating transfer at the level of task performance. The typical facilitatory effect of iTBS on MEPs was reversed for cM1, suggesting homeostatic metaplasticity, and prior performance gains in the trained hand were degraded, suggesting that iTBS interfered with learning. In stark contrast, iM1 iTBS facilitated both performance and excitability for the untrained hand. Importantly, the effects of cM1 and iM1 iTBS on behavior were exclusive to the hand contralateral to stimulation, suggesting that adaptations within the untrained M1 contribute to cross-limb transfer. However, the neural processes that mediate learning in the trained hemisphere vs. transfer in the untrained hemisphere appear distinct.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Fisiológica/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados Motores/fisiología , Extremidades/fisiología , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Corteza Motora/fisiología , Transferencia de Experiencia en Psicología/fisiología , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Electromiografía , Femenino , Lateralidad Funcional , Humanos , Masculino , Desempeño Psicomotor , Análisis de Regresión , Estimulación Magnética Transcraneal , Adulto Joven
5.
Psychol Res ; 80(4): 543-54, 2016 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25820330

RESUMEN

Specific relations between executive functions (working memory capacity, planning and problem-solving, inhibitory control) and motor skill performance (anticipatory motor planning, manual dexterity) were examined in 5- to 6-year-old children (N = 40). Results showed that the two motor skill components were not correlated. Additionally, it was found that response planning performance was a significant predictor of anticipatory motor planning performance, whereas inhibitory control and working memory capacity measures were significant predictors of manual dexterity scores. Taken together, these results suggest that cognitive and motor skills are linked, but that manual dexterity and anticipatory motor planning involve different specialized skills. The current study provides support for specific relations between cognitive and motor performance, which has implications for early childhood cognitive-motor training and intervention programs.


Asunto(s)
Función Ejecutiva , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Destreza Motora/fisiología , Solución de Problemas/fisiología , Niño , Preescolar , Cognición , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología
6.
Neural Plast ; 2015: 578620, 2015.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26167305

RESUMEN

Intermittent theta burst stimulation (iTBS) has the potential to enhance corticospinal excitability (CSE) and subsequent motor learning. However, the effects of iTBS following motor learning are unknown. The purpose of the present study was to explore the effect of iTBS on CSE and performance following motor learning. Therefore twenty-four healthy participants practiced a ballistic motor task for a total of 150 movements. iTBS was subsequently applied to the trained motor cortex (STIM group) or the vertex (SHAM group). Performance and CSE were assessed before motor learning and before and after iTBS. Training significantly increased performance and CSE in both groups. In STIM group participants, subsequent iTBS significantly reduced motor performance with smaller reductions in CSE. CSE changes as a result of motor learning were negatively correlated with both the CSE changes and performance changes as a result of iTBS. No significant effects of iTBS were found for SHAM group participants. We conclude that iTBS has the potential to degrade prior motor learning as a function of training-induced CSE changes. That means the expected LTP-like effects of iTBS are reversed following motor learning.


Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje/fisiología , Actividad Motora , Corteza Motora/fisiología , Tractos Piramidales/fisiología , Adulto , Potenciales Evocados Motores , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Estimulación Magnética Transcraneal/métodos , Adulto Joven
7.
J Sports Sci ; 31(7): 750-8, 2013.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23215863

RESUMEN

We examined the influence of visual context information on skilled motor behaviour and motor adaptation in basketball. The rules of basketball in Europe have recently changed, such that that the distance for three-point shots increased from 6.25 m to 6.75 m. As such, we tested the extent to which basketball experts can adapt to the longer distance when a) only the unfamiliar, new three-point line was provided as floor markings (NL group), or b) the familiar, old three-point line was provided in addition to the new floor markings (OL group). In the present study 20 expert basketball players performed 40 three-point shots from 6.25 m and 40 shots from 6.75 m. We assessed the percentage of hits and analysed the landing position of the ball. Results showed better adaptation of throwing performance to the longer distance when the old three-point line was provided as a visual landmark, compared to when only the new three-point line was provided. We hypothesise that the three-point line delivered relevant information needed to successfully adapt to the greater distance in the OL group, whereas it disturbed performance and ability to adapt in the NL group. The importance of visual landmarks on motor adaptation in basketball throwing is discussed relative to the influence of other information sources (i.e. angle of elevation relative to the basket) and sport practice.


Asunto(s)
Rendimiento Atlético/psicología , Baloncesto/psicología , Movimiento , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas , Visión Ocular , Percepción Visual , Adaptación Fisiológica , Adulto , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
8.
J Sport Exerc Psychol ; 35(5): 536-41, 2013 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24197721

RESUMEN

We examined whether basketball throwing performance in general and motor skill specificity from the free throw distance in particular are influenced by visual contextual information. Experienced basketball players (N = 36) performed basketball set shots at five distances from the basket. Of particular interest was the performance from the free throw distance (4.23 m), at which experienced basketball players are expected to show superior performance compared with nearby locations as a result of massive amounts of practice. Whereas a control group performed the shots on a regular basketball court, the distance between the rim and the free throw line was either increased or decreased by 30 cm in two experimental groups. Findings showed that only the control group had a superior performance from the free throw distance, and the experimental groups did not. Moreover, all groups performed more accurately from the perceived free throw line (independent of its location) compared with nearby locations. The findings suggest that visual context information influences the presence of specificity effects in experienced performers. The findings have theoretical implications for explaining the memory representation underlying the especial skill effect in basketball.


Asunto(s)
Rendimiento Atlético/fisiología , Baloncesto/fisiología , Destreza Motora/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Rendimiento Atlético/psicología , Baloncesto/psicología , Señales (Psicología) , Alemania , Humanos , Masculino , Memoria/fisiología , Práctica Psicológica
9.
Psychol Res ; 76(6): 768-76, 2012 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22075763

RESUMEN

In this study, we investigated anticipatory motor planning and the development of cognitive representation of grasp postures in children aged 7, 8, and 9 years. Overall, 9-year-old children were more likely to plan their movements to end in comfortable postures, and have distinct representational structures of certain grasp postures, compared to the 7- and 8-year old children. Additionally, the sensitivity toward comfortable end-states (end-state comfort) was related to the mental representation of certain grasp postures. Children with grasp comfort related and functionally well-structured representations were more likely to have satisfied end-state comfort in both the simple and the advanced planning condition. In contrast, end-state comfort satisfaction for the advanced planning condition was much lower for children whose cognitive representations were not structured by grasp comfort. The results of the present study support the notion that cognitive action representation plays an important role in the planning and control of grasp postures.


Asunto(s)
Desarrollo Infantil , Actividad Motora/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Factores de Edad , Niño , Femenino , Fuerza de la Mano , Humanos , Masculino , Postura
10.
J Sports Sci ; 30(10): 1037-45, 2012.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22574753

RESUMEN

Athletes frequently have to adapt their skills to fast changes of play, often requiring the flexible execution of a particular movement skill with either hand. To assess the influence of sport-specific expertise and extensive sport training on human laterality, a video analysis of regular basketball games was performed for professional, semi-professional, and amateur players to investigate how non-dominant hand use and proficiency change with increasing expertise. Our results showed that the right-hand (i.e. dominant hand) bias in basketball players is reduced with increasing expertise (i.e., competitive level). Accordingly, we found that professional players use their non-dominant hand more often and with greater success than semi-professional and amateur players. This was true for most of the basketball-specific skills. Based on these results, we assume that increasing amounts of bilateral practice can lead to a shift in task-specific manual preference towards a higher use of both hands in competition, as well as to a higher proficiency for non-dominant hand actions in particular. From an applied perspective, the more frequent use and higher proficiency of the non-dominant hand in professional basketball players, compared with amateurs, suggests that the context-specific and skilled use of the non-dominant hand is crucial for successful play at higher competitive levels in the sport of basketball.


Asunto(s)
Rendimiento Atlético/fisiología , Baloncesto/fisiología , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Humanos , Masculino , Grabación en Video , Adulto Joven
11.
Laterality ; 17(1): 18-37, 2012.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21500083

RESUMEN

Findings from neurosciences indicate that the two brain hemispheres are specialised for the processing of distinct movement features. How this knowledge can be useful in motor learning remains unclear. Two experiments were conducted to investigate the influence of initial practice with the dominant vs non-dominant hand on the acquisition of novel throwing skills. Within a transfer design two groups practised a novel motor task with the same amount of practice on each hand, but in opposite hand-order. In Experiment 1, participants acquired the position throw in basketball, which places high demands on throwing accuracy. Participants practising this task with their non-dominant hand first, before changing to the dominant hand, showed better skill acquisition than participants practising in opposite order. In Experiment 2 participants learned the overarm throw in team handball, which requires great throwing strength. Participants initially practising with their dominant hand benefited more from practice than participants beginning with their non-dominant hand. These results indicate that spatial accuracy tasks are learned better after initial practice with the non-dominant hand, whereas initial practice with the dominant hand is more efficient for maximum force production tasks. The effects are discussed in terms of brain lateralisation and bilateral practice schedules.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Lateralidad Funcional , Mano , Aprendizaje , Destreza Motora , Adolescente , Rendimiento Atlético , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Práctica Psicológica , Deportes , Transferencia de Experiencia en Psicología
12.
Brain Cogn ; 77(2): 271-9, 2011 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21889250

RESUMEN

Interlimb transfer of motor learning, indicating an improvement in performance with one limb following training with the other, often occurs asymmetrically (i.e., from non-dominant to dominant limb or vice versa, but not both). In the present study, we examined whether interlimb transfer of the same motor task could occur asymmetrically and in opposite directions (i.e., from right to left leg vs. left to right leg) depending on individuals' conception of the task. Two experimental conditions were tested: In a dynamic control condition, the process of learning was facilitated by providing the subjects with a type of information that forced them to focus on dynamic features of a given task (force impulse); and in a spatial control condition, it was done with another type of information that forced them to focus on visuomotor features of the same task (distance). Both conditions employed the same leg extension task. In addition, a fully-crossed transfer paradigm was used in which one group of subjects initially practiced with the right leg and were tested with the left leg for a transfer test, while the other group used the two legs in the opposite order. The results showed that the direction of interlimb transfer varied depending on the condition, such that the right and the left leg benefited from initial training with the opposite leg only in the spatial and the dynamic condition, respectively. Our finding suggests that manipulating the conception of a leg extension task has a substantial influence on the pattern of interlimb transfer in such a way that the direction of transfer can even be opposite depending on whether the task is conceived as a dynamic or spatial control task.


Asunto(s)
Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Extremidad Inferior/fisiología , Práctica Psicológica , Transferencia de Experiencia en Psicología/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Movimiento/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología
13.
Front Psychol ; 12: 651065, 2021.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33796059

RESUMEN

School physical education (PE) has the potential to contribute to public-health promotion and well-being, but oftentimes students' lack of motivation toward PE or physical activity in general, especially during adolescence, diminishes, or eradicates the positive effects associated with PE. Therefore, practical approaches are required that help teachers to increase or awake students intrinsic motivation toward PE, for which self-determination theory may provide the conceptual framework. In that regard, the purpose of the present study was to examine whether the use of real-time, heart rate feedback (as a method to support students' need for autonomy and competence) during regular PE lessons has the potential to increase students' autonomous motivation and physical effort. To achieve this, we had forty healthy adolescents between 16 and 17 years of age run for 30 min either with (experimental group, EG) or without (control group, CG) real-time, individualized heart rate feedback during a regular PE class and compared physical and perceived exertion as well as joy of running between the two groups. Participants were randomly assigned to the groups. Our data revealed that participants in the EG enjoyed running more than participants in the CG (joy of running was 3.20 in the EG vs. 2.63 in the CG, p = 0.03) despite a higher physical (163 to 178 in EG vs. 141 to 156 beats per minute in the CG, p < 0.001) and perceived exertion (rating of perceived exertion of 13.22 in the EG vs. 10.59 in the CG, p = 0.02). That means, running with real-time, individualized heart rate feedback apparently increased participants' motivation to run and to enjoy running at higher levels of exertion. In that regard, real-time, individualized activity feedback should be implemented in regular PE classes systematically and repeatedly to create a controllable and attainable situation that allows students to actively adjust their own behavior to achieve appealing and realistic goals.

14.
Front Hum Neurosci ; 15: 714828, 2021.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34456700

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: There is evidence that sensorimotor and executive functions are inherently intertwined, but that the relationship between these functions differ depending on an individual's stage in development (e.g., childhood, adolescence, adulthood). OBJECTIVE: In this study, sensorimotor and executive function performance was examined in a group of children (n = 40; 8-12 years), adolescents (n = 39; 13-17 years), and young adults (n = 83; 18-24 years) to investigate maturation of these functions, and how the relationships between these functions differ between groups. RESULTS: Adults and adolescents outperformed children on all sensorimotor and executive functions. Adults and adolescents exhibited similar levels of executive functioning, but adults outperformed adolescents on two sensorimotor functioning measures (eye-hand coordination spatial precision and proprioceptive variability). Regression analysis demonstrated that executive functions contribute to children's sensorimotor performance, but do not contribute to adolescent's sensorimotor performance. CONCLUSION: These findings highlight the key role that developmental stage plays in the relationship between sensorimotor and executive functions. Specifically, executive functions appear to contribute to more successful sensorimotor function performance in childhood, but not during adolescence. It is likely that sensorimotor functions begin to develop independently from executive functions during adolescence, and therefore do not contribute to successful sensorimotor performance. The change in the relationship between sensorimotor and executive functions is important to take into consideration when developing sensorimotor and executive function interventions.

15.
Front Med (Lausanne) ; 8: 628021, 2021.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34239883

RESUMEN

Background: Continuous passive motion (CPM) is frequently used during rehabilitation following total knee arthroplasty (TKA). Low-load resistance training (LLRT) using continuous active motion (CAM) devices is a promising alternative. We investigated the effectiveness of CPM compared to LLRT using the affected leg (CAMuni) and both legs (CAMbi) in the early post-operative rehabilitation. HYPOTHESES: (I) LLRT (CAMuni and CAMbi) is superior to CPM, (II) additional training of the unaffected leg (CAMbi) is more effective than unilateral training (CAMuni). Materials and Methods: Eighty-five TKA patients were randomly assigned to three groups, respectively: (i) unilateral CPM of the operated leg; (ii) unilateral CAM of the operated leg (CAMuni); (iii) bilateral alternating CAM (CAMbi). Patients were assessed 1 day before TKA (pre-test), 1 day before discharge (post-test), and 3 months post-operatively (follow-up). PRIMARY OUTCOME: active knee flexion range of motion (ROMFlex). SECONDARY OUTCOMES: active knee extension ROM (ROMExt), swelling, pain, C-reactive protein, quality of life (Qol), physical activity, timed-up-and-go performance, stair-climbing performance, quadriceps muscle strength. Analyses of covariances were performed (modified intention-to-treat and per-protocol). Results: Hypothesis I: Primary outcome: CAMbi resulted in a higher ROMFlex of 9.0° (95%CI -18.03-0.04°, d = 0.76) and 6.3° (95%CI -14.31-0.99°, d = 0.61) compared to CPM at post-test and follow-up, respectively. SECONDARY OUTCOMES: At post-test, C-reactive protein was lower in both CAM groups compared with CPM. Knee pain was lower in CAMuni compared to CPM. Improved ROMExt, reduced swelling, better stair-climbing and timed-up-and-go performance were observed for CAMbi compared to CPM. At follow-up, both CAM groups reported higher Qol and CAMbi showed a better timed-up-and-go performance. Hypothesis II: Primary outcome: CAMbi resulted in a higher knee ROMFlex of 6.5° (95%CI -2.16-15.21°, d = 0.56) compared to CAMuni at post-test. SECONDARY OUTCOMES: At post-test, improved ROMExt, reduced swelling, and better timed-up-and-go performance were observed in CAMbi compared to CAMuni. Conclusions: Additional LLRT of the unaffected leg (CAMbi) seems to be more effective for recovery of function than training of the affected leg only (CAMuni), which may be mediated by positive transfer effects from the unaffected to the affected limb (cross education) and/or preserved neuromuscular function of the trained, unaffected leg. Trial Registration: ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT02062138.

16.
Front Psychol ; 11: 1552, 2020.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32774313

RESUMEN

Evidence suggests that executive and motor functions are functionally intertwined, with the interrelation between the two processes influenced by the developmental stage of the individual. This study examined executive and motor functions in preschool children (n = 41; 65-83 months), and investigated if, and how, preschoolers cognitive-motor functioning differs from that of young adults (n = 40; 18-31 years), who served as a control group reflecting the upper bound of cognitive-motor development. As expected, performance of young adults was significantly better than that of preschool children for all cognitive and motor domains tested. The results further showed differential associations among, and between, cognitive and motor functions in preschool children when compared to young adults. While similar correlations among motor variables are found in both groups, correlations among executive functions and between executive and motor variables are only found in preschool children. It thus appears that executive functions (especially working memory) contribute more to successful motor performance in preschool years than in young adulthood. The findings highlight the importance of considering the developmental stage and/or the proficiency level of the individual when examining cognitive-motor interactions or when drawing implications for childhood cognitive-motor training and interventions.

17.
Sci Rep ; 8(1): 10811, 2018 Jul 17.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30018399

RESUMEN

It has long been postulated that cognitive and motor functions are functionally intertwined. While the idea received convincing support from neuroimaging studies providing evidence that motor and cognitive processes draw on common neural mechanisms and resources, findings from behavioral studies are rather inconsistent. The purpose of the present study was to identify and verify key factors that act on the link between cognitive and motor functions. Specifically we investigated whether it is possible to predict motor skills from cognitive functions. While our results support the idea that motor and cognitive functions are functionally intertwined and different motor skills entail distinct cognitive functions, our data also strongly suggest that the impact of cognitive control processes on motor skill proficiency depends on performance variability, i.e. on how challenging a motor task is. Based on these findings, we presume that motor skills activate specific cognitive control processes on two levels: basic processes that are solely related to the type of the motor task, and variability-driven processes that come into play when performance variability is high. For practitioners, these findings call for specific and challenging motor training interventions to directly tap into the to-be-improved cognitive skills and to involve a maximum of cognitive processes.


Asunto(s)
Cognición/fisiología , Destreza Motora/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Humanos , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Equilibrio Postural , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas , Adulto Joven
18.
Cortex ; 99: 13-29, 2018 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29121485

RESUMEN

When selecting actions based on visual warning stimuli (WS), corticospinal excitability (CSE) is initially suppressed, consistent with a neural mechanism to prevent premature release of the competing responses. Despite being implicated in between-hand movement selection and preparation, the role that interhemispheric inhibition (IHI) may play in this 'impulse control' mechanism is not known. Participants performed a warned, between-hand, choice reaction time (RT) task in which the informativeness of the WS (with regards to which hand would be required to respond) was manipulated. Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) assessed CSE of the right primary motor cortex (M1) and IHI from left to right M1 with 10 (IHI10) and 40 (IHI40) msec interstimulus intervals during movement selection and preparation. Consistent with impulse control, CSE was initially suppressed prior to both left and right hand actions, irrespective of WS informativeness. Subsequent CSE increases occurred in the responding hand which were larger, and occurred earlier, following an informative WS. Importantly, these increases strongly predicted response times. In contrast to the generic CSE suppression, an informative WS permitted a hand-specific release of IHI10 in the responding hand, whereas IHI40 was released in both hands. As releases of IHI cannot explain a simultaneous suppression of CSE, this suggests several distinct movement preparation mechanisms are at play with IHI modulation occurring independently from impulse control. Notably, the findings support the notion that IHI10 and IHI40 between contralateral motor regions are mediated by discrete transcallosal pathways, and are differently modulated by specific motor and cognitive attributes of a rapid choice task.


Asunto(s)
Conducta de Elección , Cognición/fisiología , Corteza Motora/fisiología , Movimiento/fisiología , Inhibición Neural/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Tiempo de Reacción , Estimulación Magnética Transcraneal , Adulto Joven
19.
J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci ; 72(3): 373-382, 2017 May 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26471220

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVES: The end-state comfort (ESC) effect represents an efficiency constraint in anticipatory motor planning. Although young adults usually avoid uncomfortable postures at the end of goal-directed movements, newer studies revealed that children's sensitivity for ESC is not fully in place before the age of 10 years. In this matter, it is surprising that nothing is known about the development of the ESC effect at older ages. Therefore, the aim of the present study was to examine the development of anticipatory motor planning in older adults. METHOD: In 2 experiments, a total of 119 older adults (from 60 to 80 years old) performed in an unimanual (Experiment 1) and a bimanual version (Experiment 2) of the bar-transport-task. RESULTS: Across both experiments, the propensity of the ESC effect was significantly lower in the old-old (71-80 years old) as compared with the young-old (60-70 years old) participants. DISCUSSION: Although the performance of the young-old participants in the unimanual and bimanual task was comparable to what has been reported for young adults, the performance of the old-old participants was rather similar to the behavior of children younger than 10 years. Thus, for the first time, evidence is provided for the decrease of the ESC effect in older adults.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/fisiología , Anticipación Psicológica/fisiología , Actividad Motora/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad
20.
Front Aging Neurosci ; 9: 283, 2017.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28928653

RESUMEN

Anticipatory motor planning abilities mature as children grow older, develop throughout childhood and are likely to be stable till the late sixties. In the seventh decade of life, motor planning performance dramatically declines, with anticipatory motor planning abilities falling to levels of those exhibited by children. At present, the processes enabling successful anticipatory motor planning in general, as do the cognitive processes mediating these age-related changes, remain elusive. Thus, the aim of the present study was (a) to identify cognitive and motor functions that are most affected by normal aging and (b) to elucidate key (cognitive and motor) factors that are critical for successful motor planning performance in young (n = 40, mean age = 23.1 ± 2.6 years) and older adults (n = 37, mean age = 73.5 ± 7.1 years). Results indicate that normal aging is associated with a marked decline in all aspects of cognitive and motor functioning tested. However, age-related declines were more apparent for fine motor dexterity, processing speed and cognitive flexibility. Furthermore, up to 64% of the variance in motor planning performance across age groups could be explained by the cognitive functions processing speed, response planning and cognitive flexibility. It can be postulated that anticipatory motor planning abilities are strongly influenced by cognitive control processes, which seem to be key mechanisms to compensate for age-related decline. These findings support the general therapeutic and preventive value of cognitive-motor training programs to reduce adverse effects associated with high age.

SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA