Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 45
Filtrar
1.
Cogn Emot ; 37(7): 1167-1184, 2023.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37796001

RESUMO

A theory is proposed that views emotional feelings as pivotal for action control. Feelings of emotions are valued interoceptive signals from the body that become multimodally integrated with perceptual contents from registered and mentally simulated events. During the simulation of a perceptual change from one event to the next, a conative feeling signal is created that codes for the wanting of a specific perceptual change. A wanted perceptual change is weighted more strongly than alternatives, increasing its activation level on the cognitive level and that of associated motor structures that produced this perceptual change in the past. As a consequence, a tendency for action is generated that is directed at the production of the wanted perception.


Assuntos
Emoções , Modelos Psicológicos , Humanos , Emoções/fisiologia
2.
Psychol Res ; 86(8): 2341-2351, 2022 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34951661

RESUMO

Hundred years ago, Kurt Lewin published a series of articles in which he vehemently argued against the idea that associations between stimuli and responses motivate behavior. This article reviews his empirical work and theory and the cogency of Lewin's conclusion according to modern standards. We conclude that Lewin's criticism of the contiguity principle of associationism is still valid, and is now supported by a broad range of theories on learning, motivation, and action control. Implications for modern dual-system theory and modern theories on motivated action and (instructed) task sets are discussed.


Assuntos
Comportamento , Motivação , Humanos , Masculino
3.
Cogn Emot ; 36(8): 1509-1521, 2022 12.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36181455

RESUMO

The adaptation-by-binding account and the arousal-biased competition model suggest that emotional arousal increases binding effects for transient links between stimuli and responses. Two highly-powered, pre-registered experiments tested whether transient stimulus-response bindings are stronger for high versus low arousing stimuli. Emotional words were presented in a sequential prime-probe design in which stimulus relation, response relation, and stimulus arousal were orthogonally manipulated. In Experiment 1 (N = 101), words with high and low arousal levels were presented individually in prime and probe displays. In Experiment 2 (N = 170), a high arousing affective word was presented simultaneously with a neutral word during the prime display; in the subsequent probe display, either the arousing or the neutral word repeated or a different high versus low arousal word was shown. Data from both experiments did not demonstrate a modulation of SRBR effects by stimulus arousal and SRBR effects were of equal magnitude for word stimuli of high and low arousal levels. These null results are not in line with binding accounts that hypothesise a modulatory influence of emotional arousal on perception-action binding.


Assuntos
Nível de Alerta , Emoções , Humanos , Emoções/fisiologia , Nível de Alerta/fisiologia
4.
Psychol Sci ; 32(10): 1566-1581, 2021 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34520296

RESUMO

We conducted a preregistered multilaboratory project (k = 36; N = 3,531) to assess the size and robustness of ego-depletion effects using a novel replication method, termed the paradigmatic replication approach. Each laboratory implemented one of two procedures that was intended to manipulate self-control and tested performance on a subsequent measure of self-control. Confirmatory tests found a nonsignificant result (d = 0.06). Confirmatory Bayesian meta-analyses using an informed-prior hypothesis (δ = 0.30, SD = 0.15) found that the data were 4 times more likely under the null than the alternative hypothesis. Hence, preregistered analyses did not find evidence for a depletion effect. Exploratory analyses on the full sample (i.e., ignoring exclusion criteria) found a statistically significant effect (d = 0.08); Bayesian analyses showed that the data were about equally likely under the null and informed-prior hypotheses. Exploratory moderator tests suggested that the depletion effect was larger for participants who reported more fatigue but was not moderated by trait self-control, willpower beliefs, or action orientation.


Assuntos
Ego , Autocontrole , Teorema de Bayes , Humanos , Projetos de Pesquisa
5.
Psychol Res ; 84(8): 2361-2374, 2020 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31327048

RESUMO

The present research shows effects of observed vertical head orientation of another person on numerical cognition in the observer. Participants saw portrait-like photographs of persons from a frontal view with gaze being directed at the camera and the head being tilted up or down (vs. not tilted). The photograph appeared immediately before each trial in different numerical cognition tasks. In Experiment 1, participants produced smaller numbers in a random number generation task after having viewed persons with a down-tilted head orientation relative to up-tilted and non-tilted head orientations. In Experiment 2, numerical estimates in an anchoring-like trivia question task were smaller following presentations of persons with a down-tilted head orientation relative to a non-tilted head orientation. In Experiment 3, a response key that was associated with larger numbers in a numerical magnitude task was pressed less frequently in a randomly intermixed free choice task when the photograph showed a person with a down-tilted relative to an up-tilted head orientation. These findings consistently show that social displays can influence numerical cognition across a variety of task settings.


Assuntos
Cognição/fisiologia , Movimentos Oculares/fisiologia , Movimentos da Cabeça/fisiologia , Orientação Espacial/fisiologia , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Rotação
6.
Aggress Behav ; 46(4): 305-316, 2020 07.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32232867

RESUMO

What reaction stops revenge taking? Four experiments (total N = 191) examined this question where the victim of an interpersonal transgression could observe the offender's reaction (anger, sadness, pain, or calm) to a retributive noise punishment. We compared the punishment intensity selected by the participant before and after seeing the offender's reaction. Seeing the opponent in pain reduced subsequent punishment most strongly, while displays of sadness and verbal indications of suffering had no appeasing effect. Expression of anger about a retributive punishment did not increase revenge seeking relative to a calm reaction, even when the anger response was disambiguated as being angry with the punisher. It is concluded that the expression of pain is the most effective emotional display for the reduction of retaliatory aggression. The findings are discussed in light of recent research on reactive aggression and retributive justice.


Assuntos
Agressão , Ira , Criminosos , Emoções , Punição , Humanos
7.
Cogn Emot ; 33(7): 1410-1423, 2019 11.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30663944

RESUMO

Approach-avoidance training (AAT) has been shown to be effective in both clinical and laboratory research. However, some studies have failed to show the effects of AAT. Therefore, finding moderators of the AAT effect is a priority for further research. We investigate the moderating effect of pre-training evaluative responses towards familiar AAT targets. In particular, we test predictions: (a) that congruent responses (i.e. approach to positive targets and avoidance of negative targets) increase liking, whereas incongruent responses decrease liking; (b) that training is more effective when it can strengthen existing positivity or negativity; and (c) that ambivalence increases AAT effects. Two experiments (total N = 132) implemented an AAT with local soft-drink brands after measuring initial positive/negative explicit evaluative components and implicit liking towards the brands. Results show no reliable evidence for training effects on consumption or rating of drinks, but participants showed more implicit liking of approached drinks than avoided drinks. Furthermore, the magnitude of implicit liking measured pre-training was positively related to the size of the training effect. Ambivalence had no direct effect on the training outcomes. These results partially support the congruency prediction and underline the importance of implicit liking prior to AAT as a moderator for AAT effects.


Assuntos
Afeto/fisiologia , Atitude Frente a Saúde , Aprendizagem da Esquiva/fisiologia , Comportamento de Escolha/fisiologia , Condicionamento Operante/fisiologia , Adulto , Emoções/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
8.
Appetite ; 130: 209-218, 2018 11 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30121309

RESUMO

Four experiments (n = 300) examined motivational effects of approach-avoiding training (AAT) procedures on consumption of sugary soft drinks, implicit preferences and explicit preferences. Experiments varied in the number of training trials, the implementation of approach-avoidance goals during the training, and the frequency and timing of the consumption measure. AAT had no effects on any measure, and Bayesian analyses provided substantial evidence for a null model of AAT effects. A manipulation check showed that AAT affected behavioral tendencies towards the drinks in line with the training procedure (Experiment 3). It is concluded that explicit training of approach and avoidance reactions to soft drinks is not an effective procedure to modify immediate consumption of that drinks. Possible reasons and differences to previous AAT studies are discussed.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem da Esquiva , Bebidas Gaseificadas , Comportamento de Escolha , Comportamento Alimentar , Adolescente , Adulto , Teorema de Bayes , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Motivação , Adulto Jovem
9.
Psychol Res ; 81(2): 355-365, 2017 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26847335

RESUMO

According to ideomotor theory, people use bidirectional associations between movements and their effects for action selection and initiation. Our experiments examined how verbal instructions of action effects influence response selection without prior experience of action effects in a separate acquisition phase. Instructions for different groups of participants specified whether they should ignore, attend, learn, or intentionally produce acoustic effects produced by button presses. Results showed that explicit instructions of action-effect relations trigger effect-congruent action tendencies in the first trials following the instruction; in contrast, no evidence for effect-based action control was observed in these trials when instructions were to ignore or to attend to the action effects. These findings show that action-effect knowledge acquired through verbal instruction and direct experience is similarly effective for effect-based action control as long as the relation between the movement and the effect is clearly spelled out in the instruction.


Assuntos
Estimulação Acústica/métodos , Cognição/fisiologia , Atividade Motora/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Adulto , Comportamento de Escolha/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
10.
Cogn Emot ; 31(6): 1211-1224, 2017 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27414187

RESUMO

Upcoming responses in the second of two subsequently performed tasks can speed up compatible responses in the temporally preceding first task. Two experiments extend previous demonstration of such backward compatibility to affective features: responses to affective stimuli were faster in Task 1 when an affectively compatible response effect was anticipated for Task 2. This emotional backward-compatibility effect demonstrates that representations of the affective consequences of the Task 2 response were activated before the selection of a response in Task 1 was completed. This finding is problematic for the assumption of a serial stimulus-response translation stage. It also shows that the affective consequence of a response is anticipated during, and has an impact on stimulus-response translation, which implies that action planning considers codes representing and predicting the emotional consequences of actions. Implications for the control of emotional actions are discussed.


Assuntos
Afeto , Antecipação Psicológica , Desempenho Psicomotor , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Estimulação Luminosa , Tempo de Reação , Adulto Jovem
11.
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci ; 15(4): 822-36, 2015 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25931151

RESUMO

According to a recent extension of the conflict-monitoring theory, conflict between two competing response tendencies is registered as an aversive event and triggers a motivation to avoid the source of conflict. In the present study, we tested this assumption. Over five experiments, we examined whether conflict is associated with an avoidance motivation and whether stimulus conflict or response conflict triggers an avoidance tendency. Participants first performed a color Stroop task. In a subsequent motivation test, participants responded to Stroop stimuli with approach- and avoidance-related lever movements. These results showed that Stroop-conflict stimuli increased the frequency of avoidance responses in a free-choice motivation test, and also increased the speed of avoidance relative to approach responses in a forced-choice test. High and low proportions of response conflict in the Stroop task had no effect on avoidance in the motivation test. Avoidance of conflict was, however, obtained even with new conflict stimuli that had not been presented before in a Stroop task, and when the Stroop task was replaced with an unrelated filler task. Taken together, these results suggest that stimulus conflict is sufficient to trigger avoidance.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem da Esquiva , Conflito Psicológico , Adolescente , Adulto , Comportamento de Escolha , Feminino , Humanos , Individualidade , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Modelos Psicológicos , Motivação , Tempo de Reação , Teste de Stroop , Adulto Jovem
12.
Psychol Res ; 79(4): 630-49, 2015 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24962237

RESUMO

Five experiments examined whether affective consequences become associated with the responses producing them and whether anticipations of positive and negative action outcomes influence action control differently. In a learning phase, one response produced pleasant and another response unpleasant visual effects. In a subsequent test phase, the same actions were carried out in response to a neutral feature of affective stimuli. Results showed that responses were faster when the irrelevant valence of the response cue matched the valence of the response outcome, but only when the responses still produced outcomes. These results suggest that affective action consequences have a directive function in that they facilitate the selection of the associated response over other responses, even when the response outcome is unpleasant (Experiment 4A). Results of another experiment showed that affective action consequences can also have an incentive function in that responses with pleasant outcomes are generally facilitated relative to responses with unpleasant outcomes. However, this motivational effect was seen only in a free-choice test (Experiment 5). The results suggest that behavioral impulses induced by ideomotor processes are constrained by the motivational evaluation of the anticipated action outcome. A model that integrates motivational factors into ideomotor theory is presented.


Assuntos
Afeto/fisiologia , Antecipação Psicológica/fisiologia , Comportamento de Escolha/fisiologia , Motivação/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
13.
Exp Brain Res ; 231(1): 75-83, 2013 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24042215

RESUMO

Previous studies showed that initial comfort of a posture is traded for a better control at the end position, a phenomenon which has been termed the end-state comfort effect. When participants recall a recently performed motor plan, the end-state comfort effect is reduced. Two experiments investigated whether observing the grasp of another person is sufficient for later recall. Participants moved an object from a home location to different target positions. Results replicated an end-state comfort effect, revealing an inverse relation of grasp height to target height for the first movement. When participants later returned the object back to the home position, recall of the previously self-performed action dominated, replicating the reduction in end-state comfort due to recall processes. Notably, the end-state comfort effect was also reduced in conditions in which a model performed the first movement and in which the participant performed only the second movement (Experiment 1). Model actions were also recalled in situations in which the observed action was incongruent with a comfortable end position of the participant (Experiment 2). These results suggest that observed actions of others can serve as templates for movement planning in social situations.


Assuntos
Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Autoimagem , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Feminino , Força da Mão/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Psicológicos , Movimento/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa , Desempenho Psicomotor , Inquéritos e Questionários , Gravação em Vídeo , Adulto Jovem
14.
Sci Rep ; 13(1): 11161, 2023 07 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37429867

RESUMO

Definitions of obedience require the experience of conflict in response to an authority's demands. Nevertheless, we know little about this conflict and its resolution. Two experiments tested the suitability of the 'object-destruction paradigm' for the study of conflict in obedience. An experimenter instructed participants to shred bugs (among other objects) in a manipulated coffee grinder. In contrast to the demand condition, participants in the control condition were reminded of their free choice. Both received several prods if they defied the experimenter. Results show that participants were more willing to kill bugs in the demand condition. Self-reported negative affect was increased after instructions to destroy bugs relative to other objects (Experiments 1 and 2). In Experiment 2, compliant participants additionally showed an increase in tonic skin conductance and, crucially, self-reported more agency and responsibility after alleged bug-destruction. These findings elucidate the conflict experience and resolution underlying obedience. Implications for prominent explanations (agentic shift, engaged followership) are discussed.


Assuntos
Cafeína , Processos Grupais , Humanos , Café , Aplicação da Lei , Niacinamida
15.
Psychol Res ; 76(1): 111-8, 2012 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21442406

RESUMO

Two experiments examined the hypothesis that preparing an action with a specific affective connotation involves the binding of this action to an affective code reflecting this connotation. This integration into an action plan should lead to a temporary occupation of the affective code, which should impair the concurrent representation of affectively congruent events, such as the planning of another action with the same valence. This hypothesis was tested with a dual-task setup that required a speeded choice between approach- and avoidance-type lever movements after having planned and before having executed an evaluative button press. In line with the code-occupation hypothesis, slower lever movements were observed when the lever movement was affectively compatible with the prepared evaluative button press than when the two actions were affectively incompatible. Lever movements related to approach and avoidance and evaluative button presses thus seem to share a code that represents affective meaning. A model of affective action control that is based on the theory of event coding is discussed.


Assuntos
Afeto/fisiologia , Tempo de Reação , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Desempenho Psicomotor
16.
J Exp Psychol Gen ; 151(11): 2879-2892, 2022 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35604709

RESUMO

Theoretical accounts of self-representation assume a privileged role for information that is linked to the self and suggest that self-relevant stimuli capture attention in a seemingly obligatory manner. However, attention is not only biased toward self-relevant information, but self-relevant information might also tune attention more broadly, for instance, by engaging cognitive control processes that regulate allocation of attention. Indeed, research in social, clinical, and developmental sciences predicts a close link between a cognitive representation of the self and cognitive control processes. The present research is concerned with such a possible signaling function of the self to recruit cognitive control and tested predictions that follow from this view using the well-known Stroop task. Participants identified the print color of words. Self-reference was manipulated such that a prime was presented before or together with a Stroop word that comprised of either a possessive pronoun (e.g., "my green") or a definite/indefinite article as control (e.g., "the/a green"). Results of three experiments (Ntotal = 137) showed that self-reference priming reduced the congruency effect in the Stroop task relative to control conditions. This finding is incompatible with an attentional bias account assuming that self-relevant distractors always impair performance, but rather suggests that stimuli relevant to the self can facilitate cognitive control processes. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).


Assuntos
Atenção , Atenção/fisiologia , Humanos , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Teste de Stroop
17.
Sci Rep ; 12(1): 11247, 2022 07 04.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35787636

RESUMO

Previous research suggested that people prefer to administer unpleasant electric shocks to themselves rather than being left alone with their thoughts because engagement in thinking is an unpleasant activity. The present research examined this negative reinforcement hypothesis by giving participants a choice of distracting themselves with the generation of electric shock causing no to intense pain. Four experiments (N = 254) replicated the result that a large proportion of participants opted to administer painful shocks to themselves during the thinking period. However, they administered strong electric shocks to themselves even when an innocuous response option generating no or a mild shock was available. Furthermore, participants inflicted pain to themselves when they were assisted in the generation of pleasant thoughts during the waiting period, with no difference between pleasant versus unpleasant thought conditions. Overall, these results question that the primary motivation for the self-administration of painful shocks is avoidance of thinking. Instead, it seems that the self-infliction of pain was attractive for many participants, because they were curious about the shocks, their intensities, and the effects they would have on them.


Assuntos
Motivação , Prazer , Emoções , Humanos , Dor , Medição da Dor/métodos
18.
Cogn Emot ; 25(3): 478-89, 2011 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21432688

RESUMO

Past research has established that people can strategically enhance or override impulsive emotional behaviour with implementation intentions (Eder, Rothermund, & Proctor, 2010). However, it is unclear whether emotional action tendencies change by intentional processes or by habit formation processes due to repeated enactment of the intention (or both). The present study shows that forming implementation intentions is sufficient to modulate emotional action tendencies. Participants received instructions about how to respond to positive and negative stimuli on evaluation trials but no such trials were actually presented. Results showed that merely intending to approach and avoid affective stimuli influenced emotional action tendencies in a modified affective Simon task in which affective valence was irrelevant. An affective Simon effect (i.e., faster reactions when the valence of the stimulus corresponded with the valence of the movement) was observed when participants intended evaluations with affectively congruent responses (i.e., positive-approach, negative-avoid); in contrast, the effect was reversed in direction when participants planned evaluations with incongruent responses (i.e., positive-avoid, negative-approach). Thus, implementation intentions can regulate implicit emotional responses even in the absence of possible habit formation processes. Implications for dual-system accounts of emotion regulation are discussed.


Assuntos
Emoções , Comportamento Impulsivo/psicologia , Intenção , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Desempenho Psicomotor , Tempo de Reação , Controles Informais da Sociedade
19.
Psychophysiology ; 58(8): e13835, 2021 08.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33934377

RESUMO

The suffering of an opponent is an important social affective cue that modulates how aggressive interactions progress. To investigate the affective consequences of opponent suffering on a revenge seeking individual, two experiments (total N = 82) recorded facial muscle activity while participants observed the reaction of a provoking opponent to a (retaliatory) sound punishment in a laboratory aggression task. Opponents reacted via prerecorded videos either with facial displays of pain, sadness, or neutrality. Results indicate that participants enjoyed seeing the provocateur suffer: indexed by a coordinated muscle response featuring an increase in zygomaticus major (and orbicularis oculi muscle) activation accompanied by a decrease in corrugator supercilii activation. This positive facial reaction was only shown while a provoking opponent expressed pain. Expressions of sadness, and administration of sound blasts to nonprovoking opponents, did not modulate facial activity. Overall, the results suggest that revenge-seeking individuals enjoy observing the offender suffer, which could represent schadenfreude or satisfaction of having succeeded in the retaliation goal.


Assuntos
Agressão/fisiologia , Expressão Facial , Dor , Tristeza , Comportamento Social , Percepção Social , Adulto , Eletromiografia , Músculos Faciais/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Punição , Adulto Jovem
20.
Cogn Res Princ Implic ; 6(1): 75, 2021 11 21.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34806154

RESUMO

Facial masks have become and may remain ubiquitous. Though important for preventing infection, they may also serve as a reminder of the risks of disease. Thus, they may either act as cues for threat, priming avoidance-related behavior, or as cues for a safe interaction, priming social approach. To distinguish between these possibilities, we assessed implicit and explicit evaluations of masked individuals as well as avoidance bias toward relatively unsafe interactions with unmasked individuals in an approach-avoidance task in an online study. We further assessed Covid19 anxiety and specific attitudes toward mask-wearing, including mask effectiveness and desirability, hindrance of communication from masks, aesthetic appeal of masks, and mask-related worrying. Across one sample of younger (18-35 years, N = 147) and one of older adults (60+ years, N = 150), we found neither an average approach nor avoidance bias toward mask-wearing compared to unmasked individuals in the indirect behavior measurement task. However, across the combined sample, self-reported mask-related worrying correlated with reduced avoidance tendencies toward unmasked individuals when Covid19 anxiety was low, but not when it was high. This relationship was specific to avoidance tendencies and was not observed in respect to explicit or implicit preference for mask-wearing individuals. We conclude that unsafe interaction styles may be reduced by targeting mask-related worrying with public interventions, in particular for populations that otherwise have low generalized Covid19 anxiety.


Assuntos
COVID-19 , Idoso , Ansiedade , Transtornos de Ansiedade , Humanos , SARS-CoV-2 , Autorrelato
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA