Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 35
Filtrar
Mais filtros

Bases de dados
País/Região como assunto
Tipo de documento
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Memory ; 29(5): 637-644, 2021 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34018894

RESUMO

This study tested recall of proper names versus other details of a crime in incidental learning conditions designed to parallel recall when an "earwitness" reports what he or she overheard from someone discussing a crime. Participants heard an audio recording of someone discussing details of a crime he had committed, and they then completed filler tasks designed to mislead them as to the study's true purpose. After this short delay, participants had particularly poor recall for names in association with roles in the crime compared to other details about the crime. Their name errors sometimes implicated innocent people, a disturbing finding given the potential ramifications for people incriminated by witnesses reporting hearsay. Somewhat reassuringly, participants frequently did not provide a guess for the name when they were uncertain about who did what, and they reported reduced confidence in their name recall, with particularly low confidence when they recalled incorrect name information. Findings establish the pronounced difficulty of proper name learning in incidental learning conditions, and results suggest that earwitness testimony involving name recall should be treated with particular caution.


Assuntos
Nomes , Aprendizagem por Associação , Crime , Feminino , Humanos , Aprendizagem , Masculino , Rememoração Mental
2.
J Psycholinguist Res ; 47(1): 169-178, 2018 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29019103

RESUMO

We tested the frequent assumption that the difficulty of word retrieval increases when a speaker is being observed and evaluated. We modified the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST) so that participants believed that its evaluative observation components continued throughout the duration of a subsequent word retrieval task, and measured participants' reported tip of the tongue (TOT) states. Participants in this TSST condition experienced more TOTs than participants in a comparable, placebo TSST condition in which there was no suggestion of evaluative observation. This experiment provides initial evidence confirming the assumption that evaluative observation by a third party can be disruptive to word retrieval. We interpret our findings by proposing an extension to a well-supported theoretical model of TOTs.


Assuntos
Afeto , Cognição , Rememoração Mental , Estresse Psicológico/psicologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Psicológicos , Adulto Jovem
3.
Br J Dev Psychol ; 42(2): 177-186, 2024 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38247209

RESUMO

Older adults have even greater difficulty learning name-face associations than young adults, although many variables reflecting properties of the names have been shown to affect young and older adults' name learning similarly. Older adults' name-face association learning was compared for names with high-frequency (HF) first syllables versus names with low-frequency (LF) first syllables. Twenty-eight adults ages 65 to 80 learned five names with HF first syllables and five names with LF first syllables in association with 10 new faces over repeated testing rounds with feedback. Participants learned more name-face associations when the names had HF first syllables than LF first syllables. Findings indicate that older adults benefit from increased frequency of phonological segments within a word on a task other than word retrieval and are consistent with a theoretical framework that accounts for learning new name-face associations, the effects of linguistic properties of the names, and ageing.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem por Associação , Rememoração Mental , Adulto Jovem , Humanos , Idoso , Face , Aprendizagem , Envelhecimento
4.
Psychol Aging ; 39(3): 288-298, 2024 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38829340

RESUMO

Emotional properties of words can profoundly affect their processing, depending on both the valence (pleasantness) and the degree of arousal (excitation) that the word elicits. Words that are strongly emotionally arousing (such as taboo words) can interfere with subsequent language processing (White & Abrams, 2021). However, little is known about whether or how aging affects the processing of highly arousing language. The present study provides a characterization of how adults across the lifespan evaluate highly arousing language with a simple rating task that included taboo words, which have previously been used to examine lexical interference caused by arousal, and humorous words, which are also highly arousing without being negatively valenced. While arousal ratings were strongly positively correlated with both tabooness and humor ratings for young adults, these relationships weakened with age and overall arousal ratings were lower for middle-aged and older adults compared to young adults. Age effects cannot be readily accounted for by age-related differences in psychosocial variables such as self-reported profanity avoidance or religiosity. The effect of age on arousal should be considered in the design of studies examining age-related changes in emotional language processing. Furthermore, age differences in arousal should be considered as a potential mechanism in studies exploring emotional language processing across adulthood. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).


Assuntos
Envelhecimento , Nível de Alerta , Emoções , Idioma , Humanos , Feminino , Masculino , Adulto , Adulto Jovem , Nível de Alerta/fisiologia , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Idoso , Emoções/fisiologia , Envelhecimento/fisiologia , Envelhecimento/psicologia , Adolescente , Fatores Etários , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais
5.
Lang Speech ; : 238309241228863, 2024 Feb 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38357874

RESUMO

Emotion can have a profound effect on language processing, and taboo words have been increasingly used in research as highly emotional, negatively valenced stimuli. However, because taboo words as a lexical category are socially constructed and semantically idiosyncratic, they may also have complex emotional characteristics. This complexity may not be fully considered by researchers using taboo words as research stimuli. This study gathered tabooness, humor, and arousal ratings to provide a resource for researchers to better understand the sources and characteristics of the strong emotions generated by taboo words. A total of 411 participants aged 18-83 were recruited via online platforms, and all participants rated the same 264 words on tabooness, humor, and arousal. Analyses indicated that tabooness and humor ratings were positively related to each other, and both were predicted by arousal ratings. The set of ratings included here provides a tool for researchers using taboo stimuli, and our findings highlight methodological considerations while broadening our understanding of the cognitive and linguistic nature of highly emotional language.

6.
Exp Aging Res ; 39(1): 1-26, 2013.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23316734

RESUMO

UNLABELLED: BACKGROUND/STUDY CONTEXT: Previous tests of the relationship between subjective organization during encoding, aging, and recall have produced inconsistent findings. The present study investigates subjective organization and the acquisition and recall of verbal material across the life span (from 5 to 89 years of age) using two measures, the intertrial repetition paired frequency (PF) measure and the unidirectional subjective organization (SO) measure. METHODS: Participants (N = 2656) were administered a version of the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, including a delayed recall trial. Analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were performed to examine the relationship between age and subjective organization and between age and recall. Mediation and growth curve analyses were performed to further examine the relationship between age, verbal acquisition, and subjective organization. RESULTS: Subjective organization was not predictive of verbal forgetting. Deficits in verbal acquisition and subjective organization were detected among children and elderly adults. Mediational analyses showed that age affected the number of words recalled as well as subjective organization, and that subjective organization affected the number of words recalled in children, young adults and elderly. Latent growth curve modeling suggests that increases in subjective organization over time are related to increases in recall over time for each age group. CONCLUSION: Subjective organization is predictive of recall, and both subjective organization and recall are lowest among children and elderly individuals. Age has direct effects on recall but this effect is partially mediated by subjective organization. Brain imaging studies showing increased prefrontal cortex activation during encoding of remembered words bolster our findings that age affects the relationship between verbal learning and organization of material during encoding.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/psicologia , Rememoração Mental , Aprendizagem Verbal , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Adulto Jovem
7.
Anxiety Stress Coping ; 36(5): 543-554, 2023 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36812297

RESUMO

BACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVE: Mindsets about stress can be altered so that people interpret stress as either a positive or negative force. We exposed participants to a stress mindset intervention to test its effects on a challenging speech production task. DESIGN AND METHOD: Participants (N = 60) were randomly assigned to a stress mindset condition. In the stress-is-enhancing (SIE) condition, they viewed a brief video that characterized stress as a positive force that benefits performance. In the stress-is-debilitating (SID) condition, the video characterized stress as a negative force that should be avoided. Each participant completed a self-report measure of stress mindset, performed a psychological stressor task, and then repeatedly produced tongue twisters aloud. Speech errors and articulation time were scored for the production task. RESULTS: The manipulation check confirmed that stress mindsets were altered after viewing the videos. Participants in the SIE condition articulated the phrases more quickly than those in the SID condition without an accompanying increase in errors. CONCLUSIONS: A stress mindset manipulation affected speech production. This finding indicates that one way to mitigate the negative effects of stress on speech production is to instantiate beliefs that stress is a positive force that can enhance performance.


Assuntos
Fala , Estresse Psicológico , Humanos , Estresse Psicológico/psicologia , Autorrelato
8.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 76(1): 109-116, 2023 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35073799

RESUMO

It is harder to learn a proper name than a common noun in association with a new face, and low-frequency (LF) or rare surnames are harder to learn than high-frequency (HF) or common surnames. A separate body of research has shown that words containing HF phonological components can be easier to retrieve and produce than words with LF phonological components. This study tested for a "downstream" benefit of increased syllable frequency (independent of name frequency) on name-face association learning: surnames with HF first syllables were predicted to be learned more easily than those with LF first syllables. Participants were tasked with learning 5 names with HF first syllables and 5 names with LF first syllables in association with 10 unfamiliar faces over repeated testing rounds with feedback. People learned more names containing HF than LF first syllables, demonstrating a benefit of increased phonological frequency to name learning. Findings support an interactive activation model that accounts for name-face association learning as well as phonological frequency effects on production, along with many other aspects of memory and language.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem por Associação , Nomes , Humanos , Idioma , Aprendizagem , Rememoração Mental
9.
Memory ; 20(2): 155-66, 2012.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22292565

RESUMO

In three experiments we attempted to increase interference using experimental manipulations in a face-name learning paradigm. All experiments included young and older adult participants because ageing is associated with increases in both susceptibility to interference and difficulty in learning face-name associations. None of the experiments produced interference for either age group: The inclusion of confusable (i.e., ambiguous) names and occupations, having to learn an additional piece of information in association with each face, and requiring participants to guess when uncertain all failed to negatively impact name learning. Interference does not appear to be the critical mechanism underlying the difficulty of learning proper names, and it cannot account for older adults' disproportionate decline in name-learning ability.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem por Associação , Inibição Psicológica , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Envelhecimento/psicologia , Face , Humanos , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Nomes , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Percepção Visual
10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31294648

RESUMO

In previous research, young adults who were told they were being observed and evaluated reported more tip-of-the-tongue (TOT) instances than those who were not. We first tested whether the same effect is obtained for older adult participants, and then compared the effects of ostensible evaluative observation on word retrieval for adults across the lifespan. Participants in the observed condition were told they were being evaluated throughout the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST) procedure and a word retrieval task, whereas participants in the unobserved condition performed similar tasks with no mention of observation or evaluation. In Experiment 1, older adult participants in the observed condition experienced more TOTs than those in the unobserved condition. In Experiment 2, observation increased TOTs to a similar extent for adults ages 18-80, replicating earlier findings with young adults and Experiment 1. Observation can impair cognitive performance similarly for adults of a wide range of ages.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/fisiologia , Idioma , Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Estresse Psicológico/fisiopatologia , Análise e Desempenho de Tarefas , Adolescente , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Adulto Jovem
11.
Am J Psychol ; 122(4): 483-96, 2009.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20066927

RESUMO

We investigated whether the use of he as a generic masculine (GM) pronoun affects comprehension. Participants read sentences containing GM or sex-specific pronouns and indicated whether each sentence could refer to a female. GM sentences were less accurately interpreted than sex-specific sentences, indicating that the sex-specific function of masculine pronouns dominates in comprehension. We also varied sentence antecedents, and participants made fewer errors on sentences with predominantly female than predominantly male or neutral antecedents. In another experiment, we tested male and female participants under conditions of time pressure. Participants of both sexes evidenced the error pattern of Experiment 1. Findings support the hypothesis that GM pronouns reduce the likelihood of thoughts of females in what are intended to be non-sex-specific instances.


Assuntos
Compreensão , Preconceito , Semântica , Adolescente , Adulto , Formação de Conceito , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Psicolinguística , Predomínio Social , Adulto Jovem
12.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30058941

RESUMO

The notion that speech becomes less fluent during stressful speaking conditions has received little empirical test, and no research has tested this relationship in older adult participants. We analyzed speeches produced during the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST) or during a less stressful placebo (pTSST) version of the task. We measured young and older adults' speech fillers (e.g., um), unfilled pauses (at least 1 s in duration), and other disfluencies (e.g., repetitions, repairs). Neither young nor older adult participants rated themselves as having greater stress in the TSST than pTSST condition, but behavioral effects were obtained. Participants in the TSST condition produced more mid-phrase speech fillers and unfilled pauses than participants in the pTSST condition. Young adults produced more unfilled pauses than older adults overall, and older adults produced more mid-phrase fillers than young adults. Critically, age group interacted with experimental condition, such that older speakers produced disproportionately more mid-phrase fillers than young adults in the TSST compared to the pTSST condition. In sum, the negative effects of the TSST on fluency were generally similar across age, but this specific age-related increase in mid-phrase fillers indicates that older adults' word retrieval may have been particularly negatively affected. Findings are generally consistent with previous research and add to understanding of how factors internal to the speaker (i.e., demographic, personality, and cognitive variables) and factors external to the speaker (i.e., variables regarding the situation, context, or content of speech) combine to affect speech fluency.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/psicologia , Fala/fisiologia , Estresse Psicológico/psicologia , Adolescente , Fatores Etários , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
13.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30282517

RESUMO

Tip-of-the-tongue states (TOTs) are known to increase in frequency across adulthood, but there is wide variability in older adults' TOT rates, suggesting that individual difference factors contribute to TOT incidence. We investigated the role of affect by examining the relationship between self-reported anxiety and depression symptoms and the frequency of TOTs during a laboratory task. Participants were young, middle-aged and older adults in a population-based sample of adults aged 18-87. Increased anxiety was associated with fewer TOTs for the middle-aged group but more TOTs for the older adult group. There was no relationship between anxiety and TOTs for younger adults and no relationships between depression symptoms and TOT incidence for any age group. We discuss our results in terms of attentional control theory, which provides an explanation of how age may affect the relationship between anxiety and TOTs.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/fisiologia , Ansiedade/fisiopatologia , Depressão/fisiopatologia , Idioma , Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Feminino , Humanos , Individualidade , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Autorrelato , Adulto Jovem
14.
Psychol Aging ; 23(3): 657-64, 2008 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18808254

RESUMO

No previous research has tested whether the specific age-related deficit in learning face-name associations that has been identified using recall tasks also occurs for recognition memory measures. Young and older participants saw pictures of unfamiliar people with a name and an occupation for each person, and were tested on a matching (in Experiment 1) or multiple-choice (in Experiment 2) recognition memory test. For both recognition measures, the pattern of effects was the same as that obtained using a recall measure: More face-occupation associations were remembered than face-name associations, young adults remembered more associated information than older adults overall, and older adults had disproportionately poorer memory for face-name associations. Findings implicate age-related difficulty in forming and retrieving the association between the face and the name as the primary cause of obtained deficits in previous name learning studies.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/psicologia , Aprendizagem por Associação , Face , Memória , Nomes , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Percepção Visual , Adolescente , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Humanos , Transtornos da Memória/psicologia , Rememoração Mental , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Ocupações , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos
15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28585452

RESUMO

We compared young and older adults' speech during an error detection task, with some pictures containing visual errors and anomalies and other pictures error-free. We analyzed three disfluency types: mid-phrase speech fillers (e.g., It's a little, um, girl), repetitions (e.g., He's trying to catch the- the birds), and repairs (e.g., She- you can see her legs). Older adults produced more mid-phrase fillers than young adults only when describing pictures containing errors. These often reflect word retrieval problems and represent clear disruptions to fluency, so this interaction indicates that the need to form and maintain representations of novel information can specifically compromise older adults' speech fluency. Overall, older adults produced more repetitions and repairs than young adults, regardless of picture type, indicating general age-related increases in these disfluencies. The obtained patterns are discussed in the context of the Transmission Deficit Hypothesis and other approaches to age-related changes in speech fluency.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento Cognitivo/psicologia , Fala , Percepção Visual , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Humanos , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Modelos Psicológicos , Psicolinguística , Adulto Jovem
16.
Psychol Aging ; 22(1): 94-103, 2007 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17385987

RESUMO

This research demonstrates 3 new age-linked asymmetries between identifying versus retrieving phonological information. Young and older adults read aloud familiar isolated words (e.g., mind) and novel pseudowords (e.g., mond) in a production task and identified lexical status for identical stimuli in a comprehension task. Young adults made fewer errors than older adults in production but not comprehension (an age-related input-output asymmetry), and they produced pseudowords but not words with fewer errors than older adults (a lexical-status asymmetry). The lexical-status asymmetry also occurred for response onset times but not for output durations (an onset-output asymmetry). All 3 asymmetries were predicted under the transmission deficit hypothesis (D. G. MacKay & D. M. Burke, 1990) but contradict theories such as general slowing that cannot explain why aging affects some types of information processing more than others.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/fisiologia , Idioma , Tempo de Reação , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Idoso , Humanos , Percepção da Fala
17.
J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci ; 62(4): P201-7, 2007 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17673529

RESUMO

The nondescriptive nature of proper names has been suggested as one reason that people experience particular difficulty learning and recalling names. This experiment tested whether the exacerbated difficulty experienced by older adults in retrieving proper names is partly due to names' nondescriptive quality. Young and older participants named pictures of well-known cartoon characters that have either descriptive names (e.g., Snow White, Big Bird) or nondescriptive names (e.g., Charlie Brown, Garfield). Older adults were particularly impaired at retrieving nondescriptive names. Results indicate that theories of name memory must represent the nondescriptive nature of names and account for the decreased retrieval difficulty for descriptive compared with nondescriptive names in aging.


Assuntos
Fatores Etários , Rememoração Mental , Nomes , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Pessoas Famosas , Humanos , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Modelos Psicológicos
18.
J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci ; 72(1): 100-106, 2017 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27371482

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: We tested the claim that age-related increases in knowledge interfere with word retrieval, leading to word finding failures. We did this by relating a measure of crystallized intelligence to tip-of-the-tongue (TOT) states and picture naming accuracy. METHOD: Participants were from a large (N = 708), cross-sectional (aged 18-88 years), population-based sample from the Cambridge Centre for Ageing and Neuroscience cohort (Cam-CAN; www.cam-can.com). They completed (a) the Spot-the-Word Test (STW), a measure of crystallized intelligence in which participants circled the real word in word/nonword pairs, (b) a TOT-inducing task, and (c) a picture naming task. RESULTS: Age and STW independently predicted TOTs, with higher TOTs for older adults and for participants with lower STW scores. Tests of a moderator model examining interactions between STW and age indicated that STW was a significant negative predictor of TOTs in younger adults, but with increasing age, the effect size gradually approached zero. Results using picture naming accuracy replicated these findings. DISCUSSION: These results do not support the hypothesis that lifelong knowledge acquisition leads to interference that causes an age-related increase in TOTs. Instead, crystallized intelligence supports successful word retrieval, although this relationship weakens across adulthood.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento Cognitivo , Rememoração Mental , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Aprendizagem Verbal , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Estudos de Coortes , Feminino , Humanos , Testes de Inteligência/estatística & dados numéricos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Fonética , Psicometria , Adulto Jovem
19.
J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci ; 61(3): P180-3, 2006 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16670188

RESUMO

Existing research is equivocal as to whether aging negatively affects the retrieval of proper names more than other types of words. To clarify previous results, this study had young and older participants name and provide specific biographical information in response to pictures of familiar celebrities; it used two different measures of recall errors. An analysis of tip-of-the-tongue (TOT) responses indicated specific age-related increases in TOTs for names compared with words representing biographical information. An analysis including all other (non-TOT) recall errors did not indicate age-related deficits in name retrieval. These findings highlight the importance of the measures adopted in forming conclusions about age effects on the retrieval of names.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/psicologia , Aprendizagem por Associação , Relações Interpessoais , Rememoração Mental , Nomes , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Face , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Testes Neuropsicológicos/estatística & dados numéricos , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Valores de Referência
20.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26982878

RESUMO

We tested age effects on repetition blindness (RB), defined as the reduced probability of reporting a target word following presentation of the same word in a rapidly presented list. We also tested age effects on homophone blindness (HB), in which the first word is a homophone of the target word rather than a repeated word. Thirty young and 28 older adults viewed rapidly presented lists of words containing repeated, homophone, or unrepeated word pairs and reported all of the words immediately after each list. Older adults exhibited a greater degree of RB and HB than young adults using a conditional scoring method that provides certainty that blindness has occurred. The existence of RB and HB for both age groups, and increased blindness for older compared to young adults, supports predictions of a binding theory that has successfully accounted for a wide range of phenomena in cognitive aging.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento , Atenção/fisiologia , Cognição/fisiologia , Fonética , Priming de Repetição/fisiologia , Idoso , Análise de Variância , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Estimulação Luminosa , Vocabulário , Adulto Jovem
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA