Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 307
Filtrar
Mais filtros

Bases de dados
Tipo de documento
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Perception ; 52(1): 5-20, 2023 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36384361

RESUMO

Opposing aftereffects have been observed for faces categorized by gender, race, and age. In order to form opposing aftereffects, it appears that the two face sets must be both physically distinct and differ in terms of social meaning. The current study tests whether (1) a face set that is diverse with respect to sex and race can produce a coherent aftereffect and (2) whether this diversity itself is socially meaningful enough to support opposing aftereffects. Participants adapted to a homogenous face set consisting of only White male Republican congressmen and a diverse face set consisting of White, Asian, Black, and Latino male and female Democratic congress members. Opposing aftereffects were observed: participants adapted simultaneously and in opposite directions to the face sets. These results are the first evidence of adaptation to a face set that varies based on race and sex, and the first evidence of diversity being perceived as a socially meaningful category marker.


Assuntos
Pós-Efeito de Figura , Humanos , Feminino , Brancos , Face , Adaptação Fisiológica , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos
2.
Perception ; 52(5): 297-311, 2023 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37128745

RESUMO

Visual adaptation occurs after a prolonged exposure to a stimulus. The duration of aftereffects differs across stimuli type, and face aftereffects may be especially long lasting. The current study investigates adaptation decay of category contingent opposing aftereffects. Specifically, we tested whether naïve undergraduate participants' adaptation to photos of faces with explicit religious labels, differed from that of participants who had adapted to the same faces 7 days previously. We also tested whether 7-day old category-contingent opposing aftereffects interfere with the ability to re-adapt to a new condition. In Session 1, undergraduates made attractiveness preference selections before and after adapting to two groups of distorted faces. Participants then returned 7 days later to re-assess the attractiveness of the same faces. Participants were then adapted to the two groups of faces distorted in the opposite direction. Adaptation strength was stronger in Session 1 than in Session 2, although adaptation strength was not related to pre-adaptation selections. Week-old aftereffects interfered with the creation of aftereffects in the opposite direction 7 days later.


Assuntos
Pós-Efeito de Figura , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Humanos , Face , Adaptação Fisiológica , Estudantes
3.
Clin Radiol ; 77(7): 548-552, 2022 07.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35570156

RESUMO

AIM: To report the morphology of maternal kidneys captured on fetal magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) including kidney length, volume, renal pelvis diameter, and corticomedullary differentiation in pregnancy. MATERIALS AND METHODS: A retrospective study of maternal kidney morphology captured incidentally on fetal MRI. Women without chronic kidney disease, with a complete view of both kidneys and a singleton pregnancy were included. Kidney length, maximal renal pelvis diameter, kidney volume, and corticomedullary differentiation ratio were measured independently in duplicate. Associations with maternal and pregnancy variables were explored using linear regression. RESULTS: MRI images from 42 women were performed at 22-32 weeks' gestation. Serum creatinine concentrations are not checked routinely during pregnancy and were available for 15 (36%) women, with a median creatinine of 57 µmol/l (IQR: 50-63 µmol/l). Mean interpolar lengths were 10.9 and 10.4 cm for the left and right kidneys and varied with height. Mean maximal renal pelvis diameters were 9 mm and 12 mm, with upper reference intervals of 17 and 25 mm for the left and right kidneys, respectively. Renal volume in pregnancy was within the non-pregnant reference interval and varied with height and gestation. CONCLUSIONS: Maternal kidney length and volume in pregnancy are within the normal reference intervals for non-pregnant women. Renal pelvis diameter in pregnancy measured using MRI is substantially higher than described previously by ultrasound, with implications for routine reporting.


Assuntos
Rim , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Feminino , Idade Gestacional , Humanos , Rim/diagnóstico por imagem , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética/métodos , Masculino , Gravidez , Estudos Retrospectivos , Ultrassonografia , Ultrassonografia Pré-Natal
4.
Perception ; 50(5): 387-398, 2021 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33951950

RESUMO

Adults describe abstract shapes moving in a goal-directed manner using animate terms. This study tested which variables affect school-aged children's descriptions of moving geometrical shapes. Children aged 5 to 9 years were shown displays of interacting geometrical shapes and were asked to describe them. Across participants, instructions, number of moving figures, whether a figure caught another, and complexity of the scene were manipulated. Nine-year-olds used significantly more animate phrases than 5-year-olds. Furthermore, we found an Age by Condition interaction. Five-year-olds made significantly more animate statements in the animate condition, while 7-year-olds and 9-year-olds were less affected by instructions. Scene complexity increased children's use of animate phrases. Number of agents present on the screen and whether a catch occurred did not impact children's animate attributions. Our results support the hypothesis that children, like adults, are attuned to animacy cues and describe chasing agents in animate terms.


Assuntos
Percepção de Movimento , Adulto , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Sinais (Psicologia) , Humanos , Motivação , Percepção Social
5.
Infant Child Dev ; 30(5): e2263, 2021.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35864890

RESUMO

Theory of mind refers to the ability to reason about others' beliefs and to understand others' behaviour in terms of those beliefs. A large body of previous research has examined theory of mind reasoning in young children using false belief tasks, but tasks to examine this capacity during infancy have only been developed more recently. This research used stimuli developed by Kovács, et al., Science, 2010, 330, 1830-1834, to measure looking time to an agent with a false belief in infants aged 6 and 7 months. Using an eye-tracking procedure, we found looking behaviour consistent with 7-month-olds distinguishing an agent who has a false belief from one who has a true belief, consistent with the results reported by Kovács et al. We did not find evidence of this looking preference among 6-month-olds.

6.
Perception ; 49(8): 822-834, 2020 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32791942

RESUMO

Adults perceive a continuum of emotional facial expressions categorically rather than continuously. Categorical perception is thought to be adaptive and functional, allowing for inferences that inform behavior. To date, studies have demonstrated categorical perception of some emotional facial expressions in infants. However, a recent study reported that 12-month-olds infants do not perceive facial emotional expressions categorically across a happy-sad continuum. In contrast, toddlers at 3.5 years of age appear to use categorical perception along the happy-sad continuum. Using a novel paradigm that employed the use of a looking-time discrimination task and an explicit identification task, this study measured 26-month-old's identification of faces and ability to discriminate between faces along a happy-sad continuum. Results suggest that 26-month-olds perceive facial expressions categorically along the happy-sad continuum.


Assuntos
Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Formação de Conceito/fisiologia , Discriminação Psicológica/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Facial/fisiologia , Felicidade , Tristeza , Percepção Social , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
7.
J Neurophysiol ; 121(3): 1018-1033, 2019 03 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30673362

RESUMO

Little is known about the spatial origins of auditory nerve (AN) compound action potentials (CAPs) evoked by moderate to intense sounds. We studied the spatial origins of AN CAPs evoked by 2- to 16-kHz tone bursts at several sound levels by slowly injecting kainic acid solution into the cochlear apex of anesthetized guinea pigs. As the solution flowed from apex to base, it sequentially reduced CAP responses from low- to high-frequency cochlear regions. The times at which CAPs were reduced, combined with the cochlear location traversed by the solution at that time, showed the cochlear origin of the removed CAP component. For low-level tone bursts, the CAP origin along the cochlea was centered at the characteristic frequency (CF). As sound level increased, the CAP center shifted basally for low-frequency tone bursts but apically for high-frequency tone bursts. The apical shift was surprising because it is opposite the shift expected from AN tuning curve and basilar membrane motion asymmetries. For almost all high-level tone bursts, CAP spatial origins extended over 2 octaves along the cochlea. Surprisingly, CAPs evoked by high-level low-frequency (including 2 kHz) tone bursts showed little CAP contribution from CF regions ≤ 2 kHz. Our results can be mostly explained by spectral splatter from the tone-burst rise times, excitation in AN tuning-curve "tails," and asynchronous AN responses to high-level energy ≤ 2 kHz. This is the first time CAP origins have been identified by a spatially specific technique. Our results show the need for revising the interpretation of the cochlear origins of high-level CAPs-ABR wave 1. NEW & NOTEWORTHY Cochlear compound action potentials (CAPs) and auditory brain stem responses (ABRs) are routinely used in laboratories and clinics. They are typically interpreted as arising from the cochlear region tuned to the stimulus frequency. However, as sound level is increased, the cochlear origins of CAPs from tone bursts of all frequencies become very wide and their centers shift toward the most sensitive cochlear region. The standard interpretation of CAPs and ABRs from moderate to intense stimuli needs revision.


Assuntos
Potenciais de Ação , Núcleo Coclear/fisiologia , Potenciais Evocados Auditivos do Tronco Encefálico , Animais , Feminino , Cobaias , Masculino , Percepção da Altura Sonora
8.
Magn Reson Med ; 82(5): 1631-1645, 2019 11.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31183892

RESUMO

PURPOSE: To investigate the potential of continuous radiofrequency (RF) shifting (SWEEP) as a technique for creating densely sampled data while maintaining a stable signal state for dynamic imaging. METHODS: We present a method where a continuous stable state of magnetization is swept smoothly across the anatomy of interest, creating an efficient approach to dense multiple 2D slice imaging. This is achieved by introducing a linear frequency offset to successive RF pulses shifting the excited slice by a fraction of the slice thickness with each successive repeat times (TR). Simulations and in vivo imaging were performed to assess how this affects the measured signal. Free breathing, respiration resolved 4D volumes in fetal/placental imaging is explored as potential application of this method. RESULTS: The SWEEP method maintained a stable signal state over a full acquisition reducing artifacts from unstable magnetization. Simulations demonstrated that the effects of SWEEP on slice profiles was of the same order as that produced by physiological motion observed with conventional methods. Respiration resolved 4D data acquired with this method shows reduced respiration artifacts and resilience to non-rigid and non-cyclic motion. CONCLUSIONS: The SWEEP method is presented as a technique for improved acquisition efficiency of densely sampled short-TR 2D sequences. Using conventional slice excitation the number of RF pulses required to enter a true steady state is excessively high when using short-TR 2D acquisitions, SWEEP circumvents this limitation by creating a stable signal state that is preserved between slices.


Assuntos
Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética/métodos , Respiração , Artefatos , Mapeamento Encefálico/métodos , Simulação por Computador , Feminino , Humanos , Aumento da Imagem/métodos , Processamento de Imagem Assistida por Computador/métodos , Angiografia por Ressonância Magnética , Placenta/irrigação sanguínea , Placenta/diagnóstico por imagem , Gravidez
9.
BJOG ; 126(7): e142-e151, 2019 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30916430

RESUMO

Heart problems are common in newborn babies, affecting approximately 5-10 in 1000 babies. Some are more serious than others, but most babies born with heart problems do not have other health issues. Of those babies who have a serious heart problem, almost 1 in 4 will have heart surgery in their first year. In the UK, pregnant women are offered a scan at around 20 weeks to try and spot any heart problems. In most cases there is not a clear reason for the problem, but sometimes other issues, such as genetic conditions, are discovered. In recent years the care given to these babies after they are born has improved their chances of surviving. However, it is recognised that babies born with heart problems have a risk of delays in their learning and development. This may be due to their medical condition, or as a result of surgery and complications after birth. In babies with heart problems, there is a need for more research on ultrasound and magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) to understand how the brain develops and why these babies are more likely to have delays in learning and development. This paper discusses the way ultrasound and MRI are used in assessing the baby's brain. Ultrasound is often used to spot any problems, looking at how the baby's brain develops in pregnancy. Advances in ultrasound technologies have made this easier. MRI is well-established and safe in pregnancy, and if problems in the brain have been seen on ultrasound, MRI may be used to look at these problems in more detail. While it is not always clear what unusual MRI findings can mean for the baby in the long term, increased understanding may mean parents can be given more information about possible outcomes for the baby and may help to improve the counselling they are offered before their baby's birth.


Assuntos
Doenças Fetais/diagnóstico , Cardiopatias Congênitas/diagnóstico , Encefalopatias/diagnóstico , Encefalopatias/embriologia , Feminino , Cardiopatias Congênitas/embriologia , Cardiopatias Congênitas/etiologia , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética/métodos , Transtornos do Neurodesenvolvimento/diagnóstico , Exame Neurológico/métodos , Gravidez , Diagnóstico Pré-Natal/métodos , Prognóstico , Ultrassonografia Pré-Natal/métodos
10.
Perception ; 48(3): 228-236, 2019 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30727832

RESUMO

Preferential attention to animate motion develops early in life, and adults and infants are particularly attuned to chasing motion. Adults can detect chasing objects among up to 10 distractors and are better at detecting a chase among nonchasing distractors than a nonchase among chasing distractors. We tested whether an attentional preference for chasing has developed by the age of 4, and whether 4-year-olds can explicitly point out chasing objects. On a touch screen, participants were shown a chasing pair of circles among a varying number of distractors (2,4,6,8,10). Participants had to touch the chaser. Reaction time for adults or 4-year-olds was independent of distractor numbers, consistent with a pop-out effect for chasing stimuli. As early as 4 years of age, children show a pop-out effect for chasing objects and can identify them via touch.


Assuntos
Viés de Atenção/fisiologia , Percepção de Movimento/fisiologia , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Adolescente , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa , Adulto Jovem
11.
Curr Oncol ; 24(5): e403-e422, 2017 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29089811

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: This clinical practice guideline was developed to determine the level of evidence supporting the clinical utility of commercially available multigene profiling assays and to provide guidance about whether certain breast cancer patient populations in Ontario would benefit from alternative tests in addition to Oncotype dx (Genomic Health, Redwood City, CA, U.S.A.). METHODS: A systematic electronic Ovid search of the medline and embase databases sought out systematic reviews and primary literature. A systematic review and practice guideline was written by a working group and was then reviewed and approved by Cancer Care Ontario's Molecular Oncology Advisory Committee. RESULTS: Twenty-four studies assessing the clinical utility of Oncotype dx, Prosigna (NanoString Technologies, Seattle, WA, U.S.A.), EndoPredict (Myriad Genetics, Salt Lake City, U.S.A.), and MammaPrint (Agendia, Irvine, CA, U.S.A.) were included in the evidence base. CONCLUSIONS: The clinical utility of multigene profiling assays is currently established for an appropriate subset of patients with estrogen receptor-positive, her2-negative, node-negative breast cancer for whom a decision to give chemotherapy is difficult to make. For patients with estrogen receptor-positive tumours who receive tamoxifen alone, Oncotype dx, Prosigna, and EndoPredict validly identify a low-risk population with favourable outcomes, indicating that a low-risk assay result is actionable and the decision to withhold chemotherapy is supported. Clinical evidence indicates that a high Oncotype dx recurrence score can predict for chemotherapy benefit, but a high Prosigna or EndoPredict score, although prognostic, is not, based on clinical trial evidence, directly actionable. Prosigna and EndoPredict are statistically more likely to identify a population at risk for recurrence beyond 5 years, but that information is currently not actionable because of a lack of interventional studies.

12.
Neuroimage ; 120: 467-80, 2015 Oct 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26070259

RESUMO

In this study, we construct a spatio-temporal surface atlas of the developing cerebral cortex, which is an important tool for analysing and understanding normal and abnormal cortical development. In utero Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) of 80 healthy fetuses was performed, with a gestational age range of 21.7 to 38.9 weeks. Topologically correct cortical surface models were extracted from reconstructed 3D MRI volumes. Accurate correspondences were obtained by applying a joint spectral analysis to cortices for sets of subjects close to a specific age. Sulcal alignment was found to be accurate in comparison to spherical demons, a state of the art registration technique for aligning 2D cortical representations (average Fréchet distance≈0.4 mm at 30 weeks). We construct consistent, unbiased average cortical surface templates, for each week of gestation, from age-matched groups of surfaces by applying kernel regression in the spectral domain. These were found to accurately capture the average cortical shape of individuals within the cohort, suggesting a good alignment of cortical geometry. Each spectral embedding and its corresponding cortical surface template provide a dual reference space where cortical geometry is aligned and a vertex-wise morphometric analysis can be undertaken.


Assuntos
Atlas como Assunto , Córtex Cerebral/anatomia & histologia , Feto/anatomia & histologia , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética/métodos , Córtex Cerebral/crescimento & desenvolvimento , Feminino , Desenvolvimento Fetal , Idade Gestacional , Humanos , Gravidez
13.
Br J Cancer ; 112 Suppl 1: S124-8, 2015 Mar 31.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25734394

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Older women with breast cancer have poorer relative survival outcomes, but whether achieving earlier stage at diagnosis would translate to substantial reductions in mortality is uncertain. METHODS: We analysed data on East of England women with breast cancer (2006-2010) aged 70+ years. We estimated survival for different stage-deprivation-age group strata using both the observed and a hypothetical stage distribution (assuming that all women aged 75+ years acquired the stage distribution of those aged 70-74 years). We subsequently estimated deaths that could be postponed beyond 5 years from diagnosis if women aged 75+ years had the hypothetical stage distribution. We projected findings to the English population using appropriate age and socioeconomic group weights. RESULTS: For a typically sized annual cohort in the East of England, 27 deaths in women with breast cancer aged 75+ years can be postponed within 5 years from diagnosis if their stage distribution matched that of the women aged 70-74 years (4.8% of all 566 deaths within 5 years post diagnosis in this population). Under assumptions, we estimate that the respective number for England would be 280 deaths (5.0% of all deaths within 5 years post diagnosis in this population). CONCLUSIONS: The findings support ongoing development of targeted campaigns aimed at encouraging prompt presentation in older women.


Assuntos
Neoplasias da Mama/patologia , Carcinoma Ductal de Mama/patologia , Carcinoma Lobular/patologia , Disparidades nos Níveis de Saúde , Disparidades em Assistência à Saúde , Fatores Etários , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Neoplasias da Mama/diagnóstico , Neoplasias da Mama/mortalidade , Carcinoma Ductal de Mama/diagnóstico , Carcinoma Ductal de Mama/mortalidade , Carcinoma Lobular/diagnóstico , Carcinoma Lobular/mortalidade , Estudos de Coortes , Inglaterra , Feminino , Humanos , Fatores Socioeconômicos , Taxa de Sobrevida
14.
Br J Cancer ; 112 Suppl 1: S116-23, 2015 Mar 31.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25734390

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Although inequalities in cancer survival are thought to reflect inequalities in stage at diagnosis, little evidence exists about the size of potential survival gains from eliminating inequalities in stage at diagnosis. METHODS: We used data on patients diagnosed with malignant melanoma in the East of England (2006-2010) to estimate the number of deaths that could be postponed by completely eliminating socioeconomic and sex differences in stage at diagnosis after fitting a flexible parametric excess mortality model. RESULTS: Stage was a strong predictor of survival. There were pronounced socioeconomic and sex inequalities in the proportion of patients diagnosed at stages III-IV (12 and 8% for least deprived men and women and 25 and 18% for most deprived men and women, respectively). For an annual cohort of 1025 incident cases in the East of England, eliminating sex and deprivation differences in stage at diagnosis would postpone approximately 24 deaths to beyond 5 years from diagnosis. Using appropriate weighting, the equivalent estimate for England would be around 215 deaths, representing 11% of all deaths observed within 5 years from diagnosis in this population. CONCLUSIONS: Reducing socioeconomic and sex inequalities in stage at diagnosis would result in substantial reductions in deaths within 5 years of a melanoma diagnosis.


Assuntos
Disparidades nos Níveis de Saúde , Disparidades em Assistência à Saúde , Melanoma/mortalidade , Modelos Estatísticos , Neoplasias Cutâneas/mortalidade , Classe Social , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Inglaterra , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Melanoma/diagnóstico , Melanoma/patologia , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Mortalidade , Estadiamento de Neoplasias , Fatores Sexuais , Neoplasias Cutâneas/diagnóstico , Neoplasias Cutâneas/patologia , Fatores Socioeconômicos , Taxa de Sobrevida
15.
Neuroimage ; 91: 21-32, 2014 May 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24473102

RESUMO

We automatically quantify patterns of normal cortical folding in the developing fetus from in utero MR images (N=80) over a wide gestational age (GA) range (21.7 to 38.9weeks). This work on data from healthy subjects represents a first step towards characterising abnormal folding that may be related to pathology, facilitating earlier diagnosis and intervention. The cortical boundary was delineated by automatically segmenting the brain MR image into a number of key structures. This utilised a spatio-temporal atlas as tissue priors in an expectation-maximization approach with second order Markov random field (MRF) regularization to improve the accuracy of the cortical boundary estimate. An implicit high resolution surface was then used to compute cortical folding measures. We validated the automated segmentations with manual delineations and the average surface discrepancy was of the order of 1mm. Eight curvature-based folding measures were computed for each fetal cortex and used to give summary shape descriptors. These were strongly correlated with GA (R(2)=0.99) confirming the close link between neurological development and cortical convolution. This allowed an age-dependent non-linear model to be accurately fitted to the folding measures. The model supports visual observations that, after a slow initial start, cortical folding increases rapidly between 25 and 30weeks and subsequently slows near birth. The model allows the accurate prediction of fetal age from an observed folding measure with a smaller error where growth is fastest. We also analysed regional patterns in folding by parcellating each fetal cortex using a nine-region anatomical atlas and found that Gompertz models fitted the change in lobar regions. Regional differences in growth rate were detected, with the parietal and posterior temporal lobes exhibiting the fastest growth, while the cingulate, frontal and medial temporal lobes developed more slowly.


Assuntos
Córtex Cerebral/anatomia & histologia , Córtex Cerebral/embriologia , Feto/anatomia & histologia , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética/métodos , Adulto , Algoritmos , Atlas como Assunto , Córtex Cerebral/crescimento & desenvolvimento , Interpretação Estatística de Dados , Feminino , Idade Gestacional , Humanos , Processamento de Imagem Assistida por Computador , Modelos Lineares , Gravidez , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes
16.
Neuroimage ; 101: 633-43, 2014 Nov 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25058899

RESUMO

Motion correction is a key element for imaging the fetal brain in-utero using Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI). Maternal breathing can introduce motion, but a larger effect is frequently due to fetal movement within the womb. Consequently, imaging is frequently performed slice-by-slice using single shot techniques, which are then combined into volumetric images using slice-to-volume reconstruction methods (SVR). For successful SVR, a key preprocessing step is to isolate fetal brain tissues from maternal anatomy before correcting for the motion of the fetal head. This has hitherto been a manual or semi-automatic procedure. We propose an automatic method to localize and segment the brain of the fetus when the image data is acquired as stacks of 2D slices with anatomy misaligned due to fetal motion. We combine this segmentation process with a robust motion correction method, enabling the segmentation to be refined as the reconstruction proceeds. The fetal brain localization process uses Maximally Stable Extremal Regions (MSER), which are classified using a Bag-of-Words model with Scale-Invariant Feature Transform (SIFT) features. The segmentation process is a patch-based propagation of the MSER regions selected during detection, combined with a Conditional Random Field (CRF). The gestational age (GA) is used to incorporate prior knowledge about the size and volume of the fetal brain into the detection and segmentation process. The method was tested in a ten-fold cross-validation experiment on 66 datasets of healthy fetuses whose GA ranged from 22 to 39 weeks. In 85% of the tested cases, our proposed method produced a motion corrected volume of a relevant quality for clinical diagnosis, thus removing the need for manually delineating the contours of the brain before motion correction. Our method automatically generated as a side-product a segmentation of the reconstructed fetal brain with a mean Dice score of 93%, which can be used for further processing.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/anatomia & histologia , Processamento de Imagem Assistida por Computador/métodos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética/métodos , Encéfalo/embriologia , Feminino , Feto , Idade Gestacional , Humanos , Movimento (Física) , Gravidez , Diagnóstico Pré-Natal , Sensibilidade e Especificidade
17.
Anim Genet ; 45(3): 392-9, 2014 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24467435

RESUMO

Appaloosa horses are predisposed to equine recurrent uveitis (ERU), an immune-mediated disease characterized by recurring inflammation of the uveal tract in the eye, which is the leading cause of blindness in horses. Nine genetic markers from the ECA1 region responsible for the spotted coat color of Appaloosa horses, and 13 microsatellites spanning the equine major histocompatibility complex (ELA) on ECA20, were evaluated for association with ERU in a group of 53 Appaloosa ERU cases and 43 healthy Appaloosa controls. Three markers were significantly associated (corrected P-value <0.05): a SNP within intron 11 of the TRPM1 gene on ECA1, an ELA class I microsatellite located near the boundary of the ELA class III and class II regions and an ELA class II microsatellite located in intron 1 of the DRA gene. Association between these three genetic markers and the ERU phenotype was confirmed in a second population of 24 insidious ERU Appaloosa cases and 16 Appaloosa controls. The relative odds of being an ERU case for each allele of these three markers were estimated by fitting a logistic mixed model with each of the associated markers independently and with all three markers simultaneously. The risk model using these markers classified ~80% of ERU cases and 75% of controls in the second population as moderate or high risk, and low risk respectively. Future studies to refine the associations at ECA1 and ELA loci and identify functional variants could uncover alleles conferring susceptibility to ERU in Appaloosa horses.


Assuntos
Doenças dos Cavalos/genética , Uveíte/veterinária , Alelos , Animais , Marcadores Genéticos , Cavalos , Repetições de Microssatélites , Modelos Genéticos , Polimorfismo de Nucleotídeo Único , Fatores de Risco , Uveíte/genética
18.
Acta Psychol (Amst) ; 247: 104328, 2024 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38838493

RESUMO

Face templates can be experimentally manipulated, and category-contingent aftereffects suggest discrete templates across social groups. We tested whether 1) explicit religious labels, 2) food preferences, and 3) country of origin would support religion-contingent aftereffects across Christians and Muslims face sets. While viewing face images, ninety-three participants heard audio that stated either 1) a character's religious identity, 2) preferred food, or 3) country of origin. Participants viewed contracted Christian faces and expanded Muslim faces during the training phase. To measure adaptation, before and after the training phases, participants selected the face out of a pair of expanded and contracted Christian or Muslim faces that they found more attractive. Contingent aftereffects were found in the religious explicit (t(30) = 2.49, p = 0.02, Cohen's d = 0.58) and food conditions (t(30) = -3.77, p < 0.01, Cohen's d = -0.82), but not the country condition (t(30) = 1.64, p = 0.11, Cohen's d = 0.31). This suggests that religious labels and food preferences create socially meaningful groups, but country of origin does not. This is evidence of an impact of social categorization on visual processing.


Assuntos
Preferências Alimentares , Islamismo , Humanos , Feminino , Masculino , Adulto , Adulto Jovem , Religião e Psicologia , Cristianismo , Pós-Efeito de Figura/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Facial/fisiologia , Adolescente
19.
Radiography (Lond) ; 30(1): 288-295, 2024 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38064765

RESUMO

INTRODUCTION: Antenatal imaging provides clinical information regarding fetal growth and development. The additional benefit afforded by imaging for expectant parents in developing an emotional connection (bond) to the unborn baby is also acknowledged. However, the relationship between imaging and bonding is not fully understood, particularly where there are differing parental and pregnancy circumstances, for example use of advanced imaging techniques or the prenatal diagnosis of a congenital fetal condition. This study aimed to explore the role of antenatal imaging in enhancing the developing parent-fetal bond in first-time parents. METHODS: A descriptive, qualitative methodology was used. Semi-structured telephone interviews were conducted with first-time expectant parents attending a London hospital for clinical ultrasound (n = 20) or research MRI (n = 8) imaging during pregnancy. The sample included parents receiving specialist antenatal care for a diagnosed fetal cardiac condition (n = 8). Thematic analysis was conducted. RESULTS: The analysis generated three themes: 1) Our baby, our scan too; 2) Destination parenthood; and 3) Being in the dark, then finding the light. These themes highlight the important, but transient role of antenatal imaging in enhancing parent-fetal bonding, as well as the differing care needs of expectant parents. The integral role of healthcare professionals in providing a personalised, supportive, imaging experience to facilitate bonding is also reflected. CONCLUSION: Adopting parent-centred care approaches which involve expectant parents in fetal imaging influences bonding by helping parents to consider the reality of their impending parenthood. Knowledge acquired during scans is used to create an identity for the unborn baby, which parents can develop an emotional connection to. IMPLICATIONS FOR PRACTICE: To optimise the potential for enhanced parent-fetal bonding, care provision in fetal imaging should be tailored to the individual needs of expectant parents.


Assuntos
Pais , Cuidado Pré-Natal , Gravidez , Humanos , Feminino , Cuidado Pré-Natal/métodos , Pais/psicologia , Diagnóstico Pré-Natal , Diagnóstico por Imagem , Londres
20.
Int J Cancer ; 133(9): 2192-200, 2013 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23595777

RESUMO

Socioeconomic differences in cancer patient survival exist in many countries and across cancer sites. In our article, we estimated the number of deaths in women with breast cancer that could be avoided within 5 years from diagnosis if it were possible to eliminate socioeconomic differences in stage at diagnosis. We analysed data on East of England women with breast cancer (2006-2010). We estimated survival for different stage-age-deprivation strata using both the observed and a hypothetical stage distribution (assuming all women acquired the stage distribution of the most affluent women). Data were analysed on 20,738 women with complete stage information (92%). Affluent women were less likely to be diagnosed in advanced stage. Relative survival decreased with increasing level of deprivation. Eliminating differences in stage at diagnosis could be expected to nearly eliminate differences in relative survival for women in deprivation groups 3 and 4, but would only approximately halve the difference in relative survival for women in the most deprived group (5). This means, for a typical cohort of women diagnosed in a calendar year with breast cancer, eliminating deprivation differences in stage at diagnosis would prevent ∼40 deaths in the East of England from occurring within 5 years from diagnosis. Using appropriate weighting we estimated the respective number of avoidable deaths for the whole of England to be ∼450. The findings suggest that policies aimed at reducing inequalities in stage at diagnosis between women with breast cancer are important to reduce inequalities in breast cancer survival.


Assuntos
Neoplasias da Mama/diagnóstico , Neoplasias da Mama/mortalidade , Classe Social , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Neoplasias da Mama/cirurgia , Feminino , Seguimentos , Humanos , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Estadiamento de Neoplasias , Prognóstico , Fatores Socioeconômicos , Taxa de Sobrevida
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA