Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 57
Filtrar
1.
Dev Sci ; 25(3): e13204, 2022 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34846761

RESUMO

Metacognition plays an essential role in adults' cognitive offloading decisions. Despite possessing basic metacognitive capacities, however, preschool-aged children often fail to offload effectively. Here, we introduced 3- to 5-year-olds to a novel search task in which they were unlikely to perform optimally across trials without setting external reminders about the location of a target. Children watched as an experimenter first hid a target in one of three identical opaque containers. The containers were then shuffled out of view before children had to guess where the target was hidden. In the test phase, children could perform perfectly by simply placing a marker in a transparent jar attached to the target container prior to shuffling, and then later selecting the marked container. Children of all ages used this external strategy above chance levels if they had seen it demonstrated to them, but only the 4- and 5-year-olds independently devised the strategy to improve their future performance. These results suggest that, when necessary for optimal performance, even 4- and 5-year-olds can use metacognitive knowledge about their own future uncertainty to deploy effective external solutions.


Assuntos
Metacognição , Adulto , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Humanos , Incerteza
2.
Psychol Sci ; 32(5): 646-654, 2021 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33825583

RESUMO

Jealousy may have evolved to protect valuable social bonds from interlopers, but some researchers have suggested that it is linked to self-awareness and theory of mind, leading to claims that it is unique to humans. We presented dogs (N = 18; 11 females; age: M = 4.6 years, SD = 1.9) with situations in which they could observe an out-of-sight social interaction between their owner and a fake dog or between their owner and a fleece cylinder. We found evidence for three signatures of jealous behavior in dogs: (a) Jealousy emerged only when the dog's owner interacted with a perceived social rival, (b) it occurred as a consequence of that interaction and not because of the mere presence of a conspecific, and (c) it emerged even for an out-of-sight interaction between the dog's owner and a social rival. These results support claims that dogs display jealous behavior, and they provide the first evidence that dogs can mentally represent jealousy-inducing social interactions.


Assuntos
Ciúme , Interação Social , Animais , Cães , Feminino
3.
Biol Lett ; 17(9): 20210298, 2021 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34582738

RESUMO

Human psychology and animal cognition have increasingly used virtual stimuli to test cognitive abilities, with the expectation that participants are 'naive realists', that is, that they perceive virtual environments as both equivalent and continuous with real-life equivalents. However, there have been no attempts to investigate whether nonhuman subjects in fact behave as if physical processes in the virtual and real worlds are continuous. As kea parrots have previously shown the ability to transfer knowledge between real stimuli and both images on paper and images on touchscreens, here we test whether kea behave as naive realists and so expect physical processes to be continuous between the physical and virtual worlds. We find that, unlike infants, kea do not discriminate between these two contexts, and that they do not exhibit a preference for either. Our findings therefore validate the use of virtual stimuli as a powerful tool for testing the cognition of nonhuman animal species.


Assuntos
Papagaios , Animais , Cognição , Humanos
4.
Proc Biol Sci ; 287(1920): 20192236, 2020 02 12.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32075525

RESUMO

Contagious yawning has been suggested to be a potential signal of empathy in non-human animals. However, few studies have been able to robustly test this claim. Here, we ran a Bayesian multilevel reanalysis of six studies of contagious yawning in dogs. This provided robust support for claims that contagious yawning is present in dogs, but found no evidence that dogs display either a familiarity or gender bias in contagious yawning, two predictions made by the contagious yawning-empathy hypothesis. Furthermore, in an experiment testing the prosociality bias, a novel prediction of the contagious yawning-empathy hypothesis, dogs did not yawn more in response to a prosocial demonstrator than to an antisocial demonstrator. As such, these strands of evidence suggest that contagious yawning, although present in dogs, is not mediated by empathetic mechanisms. This calls into question claims that contagious yawning is a signal of empathy in mammals.


Assuntos
Comportamento Animal/fisiologia , Cães/fisiologia , Bocejo/fisiologia , Animais , Empatia , Feminino , Comportamento Imitativo/fisiologia , Masculino , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Sexismo , Comportamento Social
5.
Anim Cogn ; 23(1): 71-85, 2020 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31630344

RESUMO

Self-control underlies cognitive abilities such as decision making and future planning. Delay of gratification is a measure of self-control and involves obtaining a more valuable outcome in the future by tolerating a delay or investing a greater effort in the present. Contextual issues, such as reward visibility and type, may influence delayed gratification performance, although there has been limited comparative investigation between humans and other animals, particularly non-primate species. Here, we adapted an automated 'rotating tray' paradigm used previously with capuchin monkeys to test for delay of gratification ability that requires little pre-test training, where the subject must forgo an immediate, less preferred reward for a delayed, more preferred one. We tested New Caledonian crows and 3-5-year-old human children. We manipulated reward types to differ in quality or quantity (Experiments 1 and 2) as well as visibility (Experiment 2). In Experiments 1 and 2, both species performed better when the rewards varied in quality as opposed to quantity, though performed above chance in both conditions. In Experiment 1, both crows and children were able to delay gratification when both rewards were visible. In Experiment 2, 5-year-old children outperformed 3- and 4-year olds, though overall children still performed well, while the crows struggled when reward visibility was manipulated, a result which may relate to difficulties in tracking the experimenters' hands during baiting. We discuss these findings in relation to the role of contextual issues on self-control when making species comparisons and investigating the mechanisms of self-control.


Assuntos
Corvos , Desvalorização pelo Atraso , Autocontrole , Animais , Cebus , Humanos , Recompensa
6.
Behav Brain Sci ; 43: e178, 2020 08 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32772986

RESUMO

New Caledonian (NC) crow populations have developed complex tools that show suggestive evidence of cumulative change. These tool designs, therefore, appear to be the product of cumulative technological culture (CTC). We suggest that tool-using NC crows offer highly useful data for current debates over the necessary and sufficient conditions for the emergence of CTC.


Assuntos
Corvos , Evolução Cultural , Comportamento de Utilização de Ferramentas , Animais , Humanos , Tecnologia
7.
Proc Biol Sci ; 286(1894): 20182332, 2019 01 16.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30963864

RESUMO

Humans use a variety of cues to infer an object's weight, including how easily objects can be moved. For example, if we observe an object being blown down the street by the wind, we can infer that it is light. Here, we tested whether New Caledonian crows make this type of inference. After training that only one type of object (either light or heavy) was rewarded when dropped into a food dispenser, birds observed pairs of novel objects (one light and one heavy) suspended from strings in front of an electric fan. The fan was either on-creating a breeze which buffeted the light, but not the heavy, object-or off, leaving both objects stationary. In subsequent test trials, birds could drop one, or both, of the novel objects into the food dispenser. Despite having no opportunity to handle these objects prior to testing, birds touched the correct object (light or heavy) first in 73% of experimental trials, and were at chance in control trials. Our results suggest that birds used pre-existing knowledge about the behaviour exhibited by differently weighted objects in the wind to infer their weight, using this information to guide their choices.


Assuntos
Corvos/fisiologia , Aprendizagem por Discriminação , Animais , Feminino , Masculino
8.
Learn Behav ; 44(1): 18-28, 2016 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26276368

RESUMO

New Caledonian crows make and use tools, and tool types vary over geographic landscapes. Social learning may explain the variation in tool design, but it is unknown to what degree social learning accounts for the maintenance of these designs. Indeed, little is known about the mechanisms these crows use to obtain information from others, despite the question's importance in understanding whether tool behavior is transmitted via social, genetic, or environmental means. For social transmission to account for tool-type variation, copying must utilize a mechanism that is action specific (e.g., pushing left vs. right) as well as context specific (e.g., pushing a particular object vs. any object). To determine whether crows can copy a demonstrator's actions as well as the contexts in which they occur, we conducted a diffusion experiment using a novel foraging task. We used a nontool task to eliminate any confounds introduced by individual differences in their prior tool experience. Two groups had demonstrators (trained in isolation on different options of a four-option task, including a two-action option) and one group did not. We found that crows socially learn about context: After observers see a demonstrator interact with the task, they are more likely to interact with the same parts of the task. In contrast, observers did not copy the demonstrator's specific actions. Our results suggest it is unlikely that observing tool-making behavior transmits tool types. We suggest it is possible that tool types are transmitted when crows copy the physical form of the tools they encounter.


Assuntos
Comunicação , Corvos , Aprendizagem , Comportamento Social , Animais , Comportamento de Utilização de Ferramentas
9.
Am Nat ; 185(4): 562-70, 2015 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25811089

RESUMO

There is no conclusive evidence of any nonhuman animal using the sun as part of its predation strategy. Here, we show that the world's largest predatory fish-the white shark (Carcharodon carcharias)-exploits the sun when approaching baits by positioning the sun directly behind them. On sunny days, sharks reversed their direction of approach along an east-west axis from morning to afternoon but had uniformly distributed approach directions during overcast conditions. These results show that white sharks have sufficient behavioral flexibility to exploit fluctuating environmental features when predating. This sun-tracking predation strategy has a number of potential functional roles, including improvement of prey detection, avoidance of retinal overstimulation, and predator concealment.


Assuntos
Comportamento Predatório , Tubarões/fisiologia , Animais , Orientação , Sistema Solar , Comportamento Espacial
10.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 109(40): 16389-91, 2012 Oct 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22988112

RESUMO

The ability to make inferences about hidden causal mechanisms underpins scientific and religious thought. It also facilitates the understanding of social interactions and the production of sophisticated tool-using behaviors. However, although animals can reason about the outcomes of accidental interventions, only humans have been shown to make inferences about hidden causal mechanisms. Here, we show that tool-making New Caledonian crows react differently to an observable event when it is caused by a hidden causal agent. Eight crows watched two series of events in which a stick moved. In the first set of events, the crows observed a human enter a hide, a stick move, and the human then leave the hide. In the second, the stick moved without a human entering or exiting the hide. The crows inspected the hide and abandoned probing with a tool for food more often after the second, unexplained series of events. This difference shows that the crows can reason about a hidden causal agent. Comparative studies with the methodology outlined here could aid in elucidating the selective pressures that led to the evolution of this cognitive ability.


Assuntos
Cognição/fisiologia , Corvos/fisiologia , Resolução de Problemas/fisiologia , Comportamento de Utilização de Ferramentas/fisiologia , Animais , Nova Caledônia
11.
Proc Biol Sci ; 281(1787)2014 Jul 22.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24920476

RESUMO

Humans are capable of simply observing a correlation between cause and effect, and then producing a novel behavioural pattern in order to recreate the same outcome. However, it is unclear how the ability to create such causal interventions evolved. Here, we show that while 24-month-old children can produce an effective, novel action after observing a correlation, tool-making New Caledonian crows cannot. These results suggest that complex tool behaviours are not sufficient for the evolution of this ability, and that causal interventions can be cognitively and evolutionarily disassociated from other types of causal understanding.


Assuntos
Evolução Biológica , Cognição , Corvos/fisiologia , Aprendizagem , Comportamento de Utilização de Ferramentas , Animais , Pré-Escolar , Condicionamento Operante , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino , Nova Caledônia
12.
Curr Biol ; 34(1): R21-R23, 2024 01 08.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38194922

RESUMO

A new study shows a falcon species, the striated caracara, displays similar levels of behavioural innovation to tool-using parrots when solving a battery test in the wild.


Assuntos
Falconiformes , Papagaios , Animais , Comportamento Animal
13.
Dev Psychol ; 59(6): 995-1005, 2023 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37104806

RESUMO

A cardinal feature of adult cognition is the awareness of our own cognitive struggles and the capacity to draw upon this awareness to offload internal demand into the environment. In this preregistered study conducted in Australia, we investigated whether 3-8-year-olds (N = 72, 36 male, 36 female, mostly White) could self-initiate such an external metacognitive strategy and transfer it across contexts. Children watched as an experimenter demonstrated how to mark the location of a hidden prize, thus helping them successfully retrieve that prize in the future. Children were then given the opportunity to spontaneously adopt an external marking strategy across six test trials. Children who did so at least once were then introduced to a conceptually similar but structurally distinct transfer task. Although most 3-year-olds deployed the demonstrated strategy in the initial test phase, none of them modified that strategy to solve the transfer task. By contrast, many children aged 4 years and older spontaneously devised more than one previously unseen reminder-setting strategy across the six transfer trials, with this tendency increasing with age. From age 6, children deployed effective external strategies on most trials, with the number, combination, and order of unique strategies used varying widely both within and across the older age groups. These results demonstrate young children's remarkable flexibility in the transferral of external strategies across contexts and point to pronounced individual differences in the strategies children devise. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).


Assuntos
Cognição , Metacognição , Adulto , Humanos , Masculino , Criança , Feminino , Idoso , Pré-Escolar , Criatividade , Desenvolvimento Infantil , Austrália
14.
PLoS One ; 18(12): e0289197, 2023.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38055711

RESUMO

Self-control underlies goal-directed behaviour in humans and other animals. Delayed gratification - a measure of self-control - requires the ability to tolerate delays and/or invest more effort to obtain a reward of higher value over one of lower value, such as food or mates. Social context, in particular, the presence of competitors, may influence delayed gratification. We adapted the 'rotating-tray' paradigm, where subjects need to forgo an immediate, lower-quality (i.e. less preferred) reward for a delayed, higher-quality (i.e. more preferred) one, to test social influences on delayed gratification in two corvid species: New Caledonian crows and Eurasian jays. We compared choices for immediate vs. delayed rewards while alone, in the presence of a competitive conspecific and in the presence of a non-competitive conspecific. We predicted that, given the increased risk of losing a reward with a competitor present, both species would similarly, flexibly alter their choices in the presence of a conspecific compared to when alone. We found that species differed: jays were more likely to select the immediate, less preferred reward than the crows. We also found that jays were more likely to select the immediate, less preferred reward when a competitor or non-competitor was present than when alone, or when a competitor was present compared to a non-competitor, while the crows selected the delayed, highly preferred reward irrespective of social presence. We discuss our findings in relation to species differences in socio-ecological factors related to adult sociality and food-caching (storing). New Caledonian crows are more socially tolerant and moderate cachers, while Eurasian jays are highly territorial and intense cachers that may have evolved under the social context of cache pilfering and cache protection strategies. Therefore, flexibility (or inflexibility) in delay of gratification under different social contexts may relate to the species' social tolerance and related risk of competition.


Assuntos
Corvos , Desvalorização pelo Atraso , Passeriformes , Aves Canoras , Animais , Adulto , Humanos , Comportamento Alimentar , Recompensa
15.
Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc ; 98(5): 1548-1563, 2023 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37127535

RESUMO

The nature and evolution of positive emotion is a major question remaining unanswered in science and philosophy. The study of feelings and emotions in humans and animals is dominated by discussion of affective states that have negative valence. Given the clinical and social significance of negative affect, such as depression, it is unsurprising that these emotions have received more attention from scientists. Compared to negative emotions, such as fear that leads to fleeing or avoidance, positive emotions are less likely to result in specific, identifiable, behaviours being expressed by an animal. This makes it particularly challenging to quantify and study positive affect. However, bursts of intense positive emotion (joy) are more likely to be accompanied by externally visible markers, like vocalisations or movement patterns, which make it more amenable to scientific study and more resilient to concerns about anthropomorphism. We define joy as intense, brief, and event-driven (i.e. a response to something), which permits investigation into how animals react to a variety of situations that would provoke joy in humans. This means that behavioural correlates of joy are measurable, either through newly discovered 'laughter' vocalisations, increases in play behaviour, or reactions to cognitive bias tests that can be used across species. There are a range of potential situations that cause joy in humans that have not been studied in other animals, such as whether animals feel joy on sunny days, when they accomplish a difficult feat, or when they are reunited with a familiar companion after a prolonged absence. Observations of species-specific calls and play behaviour can be combined with biometric markers and reactions to ambiguous stimuli in order to enable comparisons of affect between phylogenetically distant taxonomic groups. Identifying positive affect is also important for animal welfare because knowledge of positive emotional states would allow us to monitor animal well-being better. Additionally, measuring if phylogenetically and ecologically distant animals play more, laugh more, or act more optimistically after certain kinds of experiences will also provide insight into the mechanisms underlying the evolution of joy and other positive emotions, and potentially even into the evolution of consciousness.


Assuntos
Bem-Estar do Animal , Emoções , Animais , Emoções/fisiologia
16.
Proc Biol Sci ; 279(1749): 4977-81, 2012 Dec 22.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23097511

RESUMO

Animals rarely solve problems spontaneously. Some bird species, however, can immediately find a solution to the string-pulling problem. They are able to rapidly gain access to food hung on the end of a long string by repeatedly pulling and then stepping on the string. It is currently unclear whether these spontaneous solutions are produced by insight or by a perceptual-motor feedback loop. Here, we presented New Caledonian crows and humans with a novel horizontal string-pulling task. While the humans succeeded, no individual crow showed a significant preference for the connected string, and all but one failed to gain the food even once. These results clearly show that string pulling in New Caledonian crows is generated not by insight, but by perceptual feedback. Animals can spontaneously solve problems without planning their actions.


Assuntos
Cognição , Corvos/fisiologia , Resolução de Problemas , Comportamento de Utilização de Ferramentas , Animais , Condicionamento Operante , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Nova Caledônia , Nova Zelândia , Desempenho Psicomotor , Adulto Jovem
17.
Biol Lett ; 8(2): 205-7, 2012 Apr 23.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21900316

RESUMO

Humans and chimpanzees both exhibit context-dependent tool use. That is, both species choose to use tools when food is within reach, but the context is potentially hazardous. Here, we show that New Caledonian crows used tools more frequently when food was positioned next to a novel model snake than when food was positioned next to a novel teddy bear or a familiar food bowl. However, the crows showed no significant difference in their neophobic reactions towards the teddy bear and the model snake. Therefore, the crows used tools more in response to a risky object resembling a natural predator than to a less-threatening object that provoked a comparable level of neophobia. These results show that New Caledonian crows, like humans and chimpanzees, are capable of context-dependent tool use.


Assuntos
Corvos/fisiologia , Comportamento de Utilização de Ferramentas , Animais , Medo , Comportamento Alimentar , Feminino , Masculino
18.
Behav Brain Sci ; 35(4): 241-2, 2012 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22697797

RESUMO

We agree with Vaesen that there is evidence for cognitive differences between humans and other primates. However, it is too early to draw firm conclusions about the uniqueness of the cognitive mechanisms underlying human tool use. Tests of causal understanding are in their infancy, as is the study of animals more distantly related to humans.


Assuntos
Cognição , Desempenho Psicomotor , Tecnologia , Comportamento de Utilização de Ferramentas , Animais , Humanos
19.
Behav Processes ; 196: 104603, 2022 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35131360

RESUMO

Jumping spiders (Salticidae) use exceptional vision, largely mediated by their forward-facing anterior lateral (AL) and anterior medial (AM) eyes, to pounce on prey from a distance. We evaluated depth perception through the use of 'texture density' (depth estimation through surface texture comparisons, with greater distances having higher textural density) in the salticid Trite planiceps. In visual cliff experiments, spiders tended to choose an area with a false 'low drop' over a false 'high drop' with the same texture densities, but showed no preference for either area when presented with substrates with different texture densities at a constant height. This was corroborated when T. planiceps did not avoid jumping over an illusion resembling a trench compared to a no-illusion control pattern. We then selectively occluded both AL and 1 AM eye (monocular treatment), both AL eyes (ALE-occluded binocular treatment) or no eyes (control), and induced spiders to jump across a gap at different heights. Neither control spiders, spiders with binocular cues from the AM eyes or monocular treatment spiders exhibited a height preference. These results suggest that while T. planiceps accurately perceives depth, it does not appear to rely on texture gradients as a depth cue.


Assuntos
Ilusões , Aranhas , Animais , Sinais (Psicologia) , Visão Ocular
20.
Sci Rep ; 12(1): 17415, 2022 10 18.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36258015

RESUMO

Contrafreeloading-working to access food that could be freely obtained-is rarely exhibited and poorly understood. Based on data from Grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus), researchers proposed a correlation between contrafreeloading and play: that contrafreeloading is more likely when subjects view the task as play. We tested that hypothesis by subjecting a relatively more playful parrot species, the kea (Nestor notabilis), to the same experimental tasks. Experiment 1 presented eight kea with container pairs holding more- or less-preferred free or enclosed food items, and examined three types of contrafreeloading: calculated (working to access preferred food over less-preferred, freely available food); classic (working to access food identical to freely available food); and super (working to access less-preferred food over preferred, freely available food). At the group level, the kea behaved similarly to the Greys: They significantly preferred calculated contrafreeloading, performed classic contrafreeloading at chance, and significantly failed to super contrafreeload. However, overall kea engaged in more contrafreeloading than Greys. Experiment 2 examined a potentially more ecologically relevant task, a choice between shelled and unshelled walnuts. No kea contrafreeloaded for nuts, whereas two of five Greys significantly preferred nut contrafreeloading and one chose at chance. We examine proximate and adaptive explanations for the performances of these differentially playful parrot species to further elucidate the role of play in contrafreeloading.


Assuntos
Papagaios , Humanos , Animais , Alimentos
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA