Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 31
Filtrar
Mais filtros

Bases de dados
Tipo de documento
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Appetite ; 200: 107535, 2024 09 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38821266

RESUMO

Food choices are driven by an array of motives that have been approached, determined and quantified in a number of ways, mainly in developed countries. The objective of this study was to better understand the motives behind food choices in an emerging economy by collecting information from urban people in South Africa in a series of four studies. (1) Items generated through focus group discussions with low, middle and high income participants by Magano et al. (2023) were checked for content and face validity and (2) 123 statements derived from them were evaluated by 621 respondents. After exploratory factor analysis (EFA), 46 statements best representing the motivational space were (3) presented to another group of respondents (n = 259). Here, the EFA resulted in a 31-item, 7-factor food choice questionnaire for emerging economies (FCQ-EE) which was (4) confirmed by a nationwide sample (n = 814) and further refined to an alternative 19-item, 7-factor solution. The emerging factors were: Healthy eating constraints (HEC), Frugality (FR), Emotional eating (EE), Meat appeal (MA), Weather (WE), Quality seeking (QS) and Cooking constraints (CC). Whether used in the 31-item or 19-item format, this set of statements highlights factors underlying food choice in an emerging economy and offers a way to study their importance in similar contexts. Further research is needed to show the extent to which these factors can predict actual food choices.


Assuntos
Comportamento de Escolha , Preferências Alimentares , Motivação , Humanos , Preferências Alimentares/psicologia , Feminino , Masculino , Adulto , África do Sul , Inquéritos e Questionários , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Adulto Jovem , Comportamento Alimentar/psicologia , Dieta Saudável/psicologia , Dieta Saudável/economia , Grupos Focais , Adolescente , Análise Fatorial , População Urbana
2.
Appetite ; 189: 107001, 2023 10 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37591447

RESUMO

The purpose of this work was to explore and compare food choice drivers of low (LI), middle (MI) and high (HI) income urban people in an emerging economy (South Africa). Here, 13 focus group (FG) discussions [six LI, n = 36, 67% women; four MI, n = 22, 100% women and three HI, n = 17, 76% women; total n = 75) were transcribed, coded inductively and deductively and 17 food choice categories emerged. Eight of these, i.e., aspects related to: plant vs animal protein, food waste, food preparation, availability of resources, food exploration, social aspects and food spoilage, are not typically (e.g., sensory appeal, mood, health, convenience etc.) measured with established food choice questionnaires. Economic factors and Availability of food and resources were mentioned the most by LI participants compared to MI and HI. Whereas, Health; Familiarity and Food exploration were mostly mentioned by MI and HI participants. This study yielded a mixture of individual and environment based motives which add to our understanding of the "why" aspects underlying food choice in an urban and emerging economy. The fact that these aspects are compared by income group provides interesting information on the similarities and differences of how the food choice process unfolds across varying income groups. The insights from this study are useful for the development of an updated, quantitative food choice questionnaire for application in this and other emerging economies.


Assuntos
Alimentos , Eliminação de Resíduos , Feminino , Animais , Masculino , Afeto , Grupos Focais , Manipulação de Alimentos
3.
Br J Nutr ; 126(10): 1592-1600, 2021 11 28.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33787473

RESUMO

The transition from childhood to adolescence is a sensitive period, triggering changes in health- and weight-related behaviours including eating habits which likely vary between girls and boys. We aimed to characterise the changes in the frequency of consumption of select sugary foods and drinks ('sweet treats') among 4237 Finnish girls and boys during a 2-year follow-up period. Additionally, we examined four subgroups: children whose weight or waist normalised as well as children whose weight or waist circumference increased during follow-up. An FFQ was completed at 11·1 (sd 0·9) and again at 13·4 (sd 1·1) years of age. A sum variable sweet treat index (STI, range 0-84) captured the weekly consumption frequencies of sweet treats. From baseline to follow-up, the mean STI decreased among girls from 7·1 (95 % CI 6·9, 7·3) to 6·0 (95 % CI 5·9, 6·2) (P < 0·001) and boys from 8·5 (95 % CI 8·3, 8·8) to 7·8 (95 % CI 7·6, 7·8) (P < 0·001), although both sexes increased their chocolate/sweets consumption: girls from 1·3 (95 % CI 1·3, 1·4) to 1·6 (95 % CI 1·5, 1·6) (P < 0·001) and boys from 1·4 (95 % CI 1·3, 1·4) to 1·6 (95 % CI 1·6, 1·7) (P < 0·001), and boys increased their soft drink consumption from 1·4 (95 % CI 1·3, 1·4) to 1·5 (95 % CI 1·4, 1·5) (P = 0·020). We found similar decreases in both the weight and waist subgroups. To conclude, the total frequency of consumption of sweet treats decreased during early adolescence. A similar trend across subgroups suggests that the frequency of consumption of sweet treats is unrelated to becoming overweight.


Assuntos
Doces , Comportamento Alimentar , Sobrepeso , Adolescente , Índice de Massa Corporal , Criança , Chocolate , Feminino , Finlândia , Humanos , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino , Circunferência da Cintura
4.
Br J Nutr ; 124(6): 631-640, 2020 09 28.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32312332

RESUMO

Convincing evidence suggests that diets laden with added sugar, specifically sugar-sweetened beverages, associate with excess weight in children. The relationships between sugar consumption frequency and BMI remain less well studied. We, therefore, evaluated children's consumption frequency of selected sugary products (n 8461; mean age 11·1 (sd 0·9) years) selected from the Finnish Health in Teens cohort study. Using a sixteen-item FFQ including six sugary products (chocolate/sweets, biscuits/cookies, ice cream, sweet pastry, sugary juice drinks and sugary soft drinks), we calculated a Sweet Treat Index (STI) for the frequency of weekly sugary product consumption and categorised children based on quartiles (Q) into low (Q1, cut-off < 4·0), medium (Q2 + Q3, range 4·0-10·5) and high STI (Q4, cut-off > 10·5), and as thin, normal and overweight/obese based on the measured BMI. Through multinomial logistic regression analyses, we found that subjects with a high STI exhibited a higher risk of being thin (OR 1·20, 95 % CI 1·02, 1·41) and lower risk of being overweight (OR 0·79, 95 % CI 0·67, 0·92), while subjects with a low STI were at higher risk of being overweight (OR 1·32, 95 % CI 1·14, 1·53). High consumption frequencies of salty snacks, pizza and hamburgers most closely were associated with a high STI. Our findings suggest that consuming sugary products at a high frequency does not associate with being overweight. The relationship between a low consumption frequency and being overweight suggests that overweight children's consumption frequency of sugary products may be controlled, restricted or underreported.


Assuntos
Sobrepeso/epidemiologia , Bebidas Adoçadas com Açúcar , Magreza/epidemiologia , Criança , Estudos Transversais , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Fatores de Risco
5.
Appetite ; 75: 1-10, 2014 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24361469

RESUMO

Subgroups based on flavor preferences were identified and their genetic and behavior related characteristics investigated using extensive data from 331 Finnish twins (21-25years, 146 men) including 47 monozygotic (MZ) and 93 dizygotic (DZ) pairs, and 51 twin individuals. The subgroup identification (hierarchical and K-means clustering) was based on liking responses to food names representing sour, umami, and spicy flavor qualities. Furthermore, sensory tests were conducted, a questionnaire on food likes completed, and various eating behavior related traits measured with validated scales. Sensory data included intensity ratings of PROP (6-n-propylthiouracil-impregnated filter paper), hedonic and intensity responses to sourness (orange juice with and without added citric acid, 0.42%), pungency (strawberry jelly with and without added capsaicin 0.00013%) and umami ('mouthfeel flavor' taste solution). Ratings of liking of 41 general food names were categorized into salty-and-fatty, sweet-and-fatty, fruits and vegetables and fish foods. Subgroup differences (complex samples procedure) and the genetics underlying the subgroups (structural equation modeling) were investigated. Of the resulting two groups (basic, n=140, adventurous n=152; non-grouped n=39), the adventurous expressed higher liking for sour and spicy foods, and had more tolerance for capsaicin burn in the sensory-hedonic test. The adventurous were also less food neophobic (25.9±9.1 vs. 32.5±10.6, respectively) and expressed higher liking for fruits and vegetables compared to the basic group. Genetic effects were shown to underlie the subgroups (heritability 72%, CI: 36-92%). Linkage analysis for 27 candidate gene regions revealed suggestively that being adventurous is linked to TAS1R1 and PKD1L3 genes. These results indicate that food neophobia and genetic differences may form a barrier through which individual flavor preferences are generated.


Assuntos
Comportamento Alimentar , Preferências Alimentares/fisiologia , Paladar/genética , Adulto , Feminino , Finlândia , Frutas , Humanos , Masculino , Fenótipo , Análise de Sequência de DNA , Inquéritos e Questionários , Paladar/fisiologia , Gêmeos/genética , Verduras , População Branca , Adulto Jovem
6.
Arch Sex Behav ; 41(6): 1403-8, 2012 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21725680

RESUMO

Androstenone (5α-androst-16-en-3-one) and other androstenes, body odor components occurring in apocrine secretions, may play a role in human chemosignaling. We hypothesized that the odor of androstenone may gain hedonic value from sexual intercourse experiences via associative learning. Young adults (N = 397, 61.5% women, age 21-24 years, randomly sampled regarding sexual experience) rated the intensity and pleasantness of the odors of androstenone, cinnamon, chocolate, isovaleric acid, lemon, and turpentine. Among women who were able to perceive androstenone, the odor was rated as more pleasant (less unpleasant) by those who had had experienced sexual intercourse with at least one partner (n = 175) than by those who reported never having experienced intercourse (n = 12, p = .006). The difference was specific to women. The results suggest that, among women, sexual experience may modify the pleasantness of the odor of androstenone.


Assuntos
Androsterona/administração & dosagem , Aprendizagem por Associação/fisiologia , Coito/psicologia , Emoções/fisiologia , Olfato/fisiologia , Aprendizagem por Associação/efeitos dos fármacos , Emoções/efeitos dos fármacos , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Odorantes , Olfato/efeitos dos fármacos , Gêmeos/psicologia , Adulto Jovem
7.
Appetite ; 58(3): 777-80, 2012 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22326884

RESUMO

The present study investigated whether the familiarity with and affective responses to foods are predicted by the individual trait food neophobia and by parental education in school children. The cross-sectional data collection involved children (N=208, 8- and 11-year-old) from Helsinki, and their parents. A questionnaire assessing children's food neophobia with Food Neophobia Scale (FNS), and familiarity with and affective responses to 36 foods was completed by parents. Children with low FNS were familiar with a larger number of foods than those with high FNS. High FNS was associated with low pleasantness assessment of most food groups, including cheese, fruit/vegetables, fish, starch/cereals, and ethnic/exotic. Children of well educated parents had tasted a larger number of foods, thus displaying lower behavioral neophobia, and had lower FNS scores than children of less educated parents. Both low FNS scores and high parental education predicted the number of tasted foods. Parental education was not associated with pleasantness ratings (exception: cheese). To conclude, high food neophobia lowers the pleasantness ratings of foods, and parental education moderates behavioral neophobia.


Assuntos
Afeto , Dieta/psicologia , Escolaridade , Preferências Alimentares/psicologia , Pais , Personalidade , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Criança , Comportamento Infantil/psicologia , Estudos Transversais , Feminino , Finlândia , Felicidade , Humanos , Masculino , Transtornos Fóbicos , Prazer , Inquéritos e Questionários , Paladar
8.
Appetite ; 58(2): 687-94, 2012 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22245130

RESUMO

Genetic contribution to individual differences in sour taste perception and preference was investigated in a cohort of young adult Finnish twins (n=328, 21-25 years) including 46 complete monozygotic and 92 dizygotic twin pairs and 52 twin individuals without their co-twin. Responses to sour taste were recorded as pleasantness and intensity ratings of orange juice with added citric acid (4.2g/L) relative to untainted orange juice (sensory traits). Pleasantness and use-frequency of 21 food items varying in sourness were rated in a questionnaire. Three food categories emerged in factor analysis: sour berries and fruits, less-sour berries and fruits, and sour dairy products (questionnaire traits). The contribution of genetic and environmental factors to variation and co-variation of the traits were analyzed using quantitative genetic modeling. Genetic factors played a larger role than shared environment, explaining 14% and 31% of the variation in pleasantness and intensity of sour taste, respectively, and 34-50% of the variation in pleasantness and use-frequency of sour foods. Relatively large genetic correlations existed between sensory traits and between questionnaire traits. These results demonstrate a genetic contribution to preference for sour foods.


Assuntos
Preferências Alimentares , Percepção Gustatória/genética , Gêmeos/genética , Adulto , Ácido Cítrico , Laticínios , Meio Ambiente , Frutas , Variação Genética , Humanos , Inquéritos e Questionários , Gêmeos Dizigóticos , Gêmeos Monozigóticos
9.
Behav Genet ; 41(4): 512-21, 2011 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20953688

RESUMO

Food neophobia has been studied extensively in children, but its causal origins and relationship to eating behavior in adults are not well understood. We studied genetic and environmental effects on variation in food neophobia, measured using the Food Neophobia Scale, and explored associations between food neophobia and personality, pleasantness and use frequency of food groups, and body mass index in young adult twins (N = 1175, aged 20-25 years, 54.7% women). In women, additive genetic effects (heritability) accounted for 61% of variation in food neophobia, whereas in men, shared environmental effects explained 45% of the variation. Food neophobia negatively correlated with the personality trait Openness, corrected for the structural overlap (r = -0.23), and in women, these two traits had a genetic correlation (r (g) = -0.39). In addition, food neophobia negatively correlated with pleasantness and use frequency of fruits and vegetables and of fish and with mean pleasantness of foods. Once evolutionarily important, food neophobia should at present be considered in nutrition counseling as a possible barrier to a balanced diet.


Assuntos
Ingestão de Alimentos/genética , Comportamento Alimentar , Preferências Alimentares/psicologia , Genética Comportamental , Adulto , Índice de Massa Corporal , Ingestão de Alimentos/psicologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Genéticos , Modelos Estatísticos , Fenótipo , Fatores Sexuais , Inquéritos e Questionários , Gêmeos , Verduras
10.
Appetite ; 56(3): 633-42, 2011 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21354451

RESUMO

Sensory responses greatly vary between individuals, and individual sensory experiences influence eating behaviour. Three groups responding differently to phenolic astringent stimuli (Low Responding, LR, n=20, Medium Responding, MR, n=37 and High Responding, HR, n=20) were identified from a population of 77 subjects, based on the maintenance vs fluctuation of salivary characteristics after repeated stimulation of the masticatory and taste/somatosensory systems. The effect of LR, MR and HR status on perceived astringency and liking for phenol-containing apple, grape and carrot juices spiked with increasing tannic acid (TA) concentrations was examined. TA induced a greater increase of perceived astringency in HR, compared to MR and LR subjects. A decrease in liking for spiked juices was found in HR and to a lesser extent in MR and LR subjects. No significant differences were found comparing MR and LR groups for both astringency intensity and liking data. Liking for and familiarity with 37 food items, as well as preference for 14 phenol-rich foods and beverages, each paired with a less astringent counter-product, were also examined. An internal preference map was computed on liking scores and product subgroups were identified. An effect of LR/HR status was found for two food subgroups consisting of coffee without sugar, tea without sugar, raw chicory and milk chocolate, tea with sugar, coffee with sugar. LR subjects rated the products with the most astringency higher and those with the least astringency lower than did HR subjects. LR subjects also rated their familiarity with highly astringent products higher than did HR subjects. Thus, individual differences related to the physiological salivatory response to oral stimulations affect responses to astringent stimuli and can influence the overall acceptability of phenol-rich food items.


Assuntos
Comportamento Alimentar/fisiologia , Preferências Alimentares/fisiologia , Fenóis/administração & dosagem , Taninos/administração & dosagem , Paladar/fisiologia , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Bebidas , Comportamento de Escolha/fisiologia , Comportamento do Consumidor , Daucus carota , Comportamento Alimentar/psicologia , Feminino , Preferências Alimentares/psicologia , Humanos , Masculino , Malus , Reconhecimento Psicológico/fisiologia , Saliva/fisiologia , Inquéritos e Questionários , Vitis , Adulto Jovem
11.
Appetite ; 57(3): 718-21, 2011 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21920395

RESUMO

The study explores young men's (n=290) attitudes towards food and eating before and during military service and their associations with eating of sweet and fatty foods. Before service eating of sweet foods was associated negatively with health interest and positively with craving for sweet foods and using food as a reward, and eating of fatty foods negatively with health interest. At six months of service, craving, using food as a reward and pleasure increased, whereas health interest remained stable. At six months, eating of sweet foods was negatively associated with health interest and positively with craving. Mentally and physically hard conditions and easy access to indulgence items, affect the food related attitudes and food consumption among conscripts.


Assuntos
Comportamento Alimentar/psicologia , Preferências Alimentares/psicologia , Conhecimentos, Atitudes e Prática em Saúde , Adolescente , Gorduras na Dieta/administração & dosagem , Comportamentos Relacionados com a Saúde , Humanos , Modelos Lineares , Masculino , Militares , Inquéritos e Questionários , Paladar , Adulto Jovem
12.
Chem Senses ; 35(1): 75-85, 2010 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19942580

RESUMO

The extent of the change in salivary protein characteristics after repeated stimulations was shown to be correlated to differences in perceived astringency. Salivary characteristics of 77 subjects were compared after masticatory (S1) and taste/masticatory (S2) stimulations. The variations (S2 minus S1) of protein concentration and saliva haze-forming capacity (HFC) were used to define 3 subject groups: low responding (LR, n = 20), medium responding (MR, n = 37), and high responding (HR, n = 20). Salivary protein concentration did not change in LR subjects; decreased a little, but significantly, in MR subjects; and strongly decreased in HR subjects. After S2, HFC increased in LR subjects, slightly decreased in MR subjects, and strongly decreased in HR subjects. Salivary protein electrophoresis patterns for HR and LR subjects were analyzed. No significant modifications of glycosylated proline-rich proteins (PRPs), PRPs, and amylases and a slight decrease in cystatins and histatins were found when S2 and S1 samples were compared in LR subjects, whereas HR subjects showed a strong decrease in all the above proteins after S2. Significant modifications of mucins were not found. Tannic acid (TA, 3 g/L) astringency ratings after S1 from HR subjects were significantly higher than those from the other 2 groups, whereas no differences were found comparing LR and MR ratings. The "carryover" effect due to 4 sequential exposures to TA samples (1.4 g/L) was observed in both HR and MR groups, whereas no significant astringency rating variation was found in the LR group. The results support the inhibiting role of proteins with strong phenol-binding activity on astringency elicitation. Individual physiological variations of parotid gland functionality might account for differences in sensitivity to astringent phenolic stimuli.


Assuntos
Adstringentes/farmacologia , Saliva/metabolismo , Proteínas e Peptídeos Salivares/metabolismo , Taninos/farmacologia , Adulto , Amilases/metabolismo , Feminino , Histatinas/metabolismo , Humanos , Masculino , Mucinas/metabolismo , Saliva/efeitos dos fármacos , Cistatinas Salivares/metabolismo , Proteínas e Peptídeos Salivares/química , Estimulação Química , Percepção Gustatória , Fatores de Tempo
13.
Appetite ; 54(2): 418-21, 2010 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20097240

RESUMO

Six unfamiliar foods were introduced to children (8-11 years old, n=72) who responded (yes/no) to questions as to whether they (1) had earlier seen, (2) had earlier tasted, (3) were willing to try, and (4) actually tried each food when given the opportunity. Pleasantness (if not tasted, expected pleasantness) of the foods was also rated. Children who wanted to try a food and tasted it, rated it pleasant. Children who were unwilling to try a food, but in spite of that tasted it, rated the pleasantness negatively and largely similarly to those who rated their expected pleasantness, without tasting. Food neophobia, evaluated by parents, was correlated with mean willingness to try (r=-0.39). The data provide evidence that reluctance to try a food determines subsequent hedonic experience and may act as a barrier to further familiarization with the food.


Assuntos
Ingestão de Alimentos/psicologia , Preferências Alimentares/psicologia , Transtornos Fóbicos/psicologia , Psicologia da Criança , Análise de Variância , Apetite , Criança , Comportamento de Escolha , Comportamento Alimentar/psicologia , Feminino , Alimentos/classificação , Humanos , Masculino , Paladar/fisiologia
14.
Behav Genet ; 38(5): 484-92, 2008 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18543092

RESUMO

Human genes encoding odorant receptors have been identified, but the contribution of genetic effects to total variation in specific odor perceptions is largely unknown. We estimated the relative contributions of genetic and environmental effects to variation in the perceived intensity and pleasantness of cinnamon, chocolate, turpentine, and isovaleric acid (sweaty) odors by quantitative genetic modeling of odor rating data from 856 twin individuals (including 83 complete monozygotic and 275 dizygotic twin pairs) aged 10-60 years (44% males and 56% females) from Australia, Denmark, and Finland. Results from fitting univariate models including components for additive genetic (A), shared environmental (C), and non-shared environmental (E) effects to the data implied that non-shared environmental effects account for the most variation in ratings of individual odors while genetic effects play only a minor role. Multivariate independent pathway model revealed a modest but significant common additive genetic component for intensity ratings, explaining 18% of the total variation. The results promote the importance of inter-individual variation in odor exposures and olfactory plasticity to odor perception.


Assuntos
Meio Ambiente , Olfato/genética , Adolescente , Adulto , Criança , Feminino , Variação Genética , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Modelos Genéticos , Análise Multivariada , Odorantes , Percepção , Gêmeos
15.
Physiol Behav ; 93(1-2): 235-42, 2008 Jan 28.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17897688

RESUMO

The contribution of genetic factors to individual differences in food use was estimated in a large population-based twin cohort of young adults (22- to 27-year-old). Male and female twins (n=2009 complete twin pairs) evaluated use-frequencies of 24 food items using 5 categories (1=never-5=several times a day) in a postal questionnaire. Foods were categorized by factor analysis. Estimates of the relative proportions of additive genetic, shared environmental, and unshared environmental effects on the use-frequency of food items and factor scores were obtained by quantitative genetic modeling of twin data based on linear structural equations. Four factors of food use were identified: "healthy" foods, high-fat foods, sweet foods, and meats. The variance of the use-frequency of food items and food categories was explained by additive genetic and unshared environmental influences, whereas shared environmental factors did not contribute to food use. The average proportions of genetic effects on the total variance of the use-frequency of food items and food categories were 40% and 45%, respectively. Sex differences were observed in the magnitude of genetic influences for use-frequency of four food items (chocolate, other sweets, fried foods, and meat), and in genetic factors underlying the use of three (fresh vegetables, fruits, and cheeses) items. In conclusion, family environment does not appear to influence the food use of young adults and thus nutritional education should be targeted at this age group to support development of healthy eating patterns. In addition, the results illuminate the importance of the sex-specific genetic effects on food use.


Assuntos
Ingestão de Alimentos/genética , Meio Ambiente , Comportamento Alimentar/fisiologia , Preferências Alimentares/fisiologia , Inquéritos Nutricionais , Adulto , Estudos de Coortes , Registros de Dieta , Ingestão de Alimentos/psicologia , Análise Fatorial , Comportamento Alimentar/psicologia , Feminino , Preferências Alimentares/psicologia , Variação Genética , Humanos , Masculino , Fatores Sexuais , Gêmeos Dizigóticos , Gêmeos Monozigóticos
16.
Eur J Hum Genet ; 15(5): 596-602, 2007 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17342154

RESUMO

Although potential odorant receptor genes have been identified, the precise genetic component of perception of odours is still obscure. Although there is some evidence for heritability of a few olfactory-related traits, no genome-wide search for loci harboring underlying genes has been published to date. We performed a genome-wide scan to identify loci affecting the identification, intensity and pleasantness of 12 odours (cinnamon, turpentine, lemon, smoke, chocolate, rose, paint thinner, banana, pineapple, gasoline, soap, onion) using 146 Finnish adults from 26 families. Several of these traits showed heritable variation in the families. Suggestive evidence of linkage was found for the pleasantness of cinnamon odour (h(2)=61%) on chromosome 4q32.3 (multipoint logarithm of the odds (LOD) score 3.01), as well as for the perceived intensity of paint thinner odour (h(2)=31%) on chromosome 2p14 (multipoint LOD score 2.55). As these loci do not contain any known human odorant receptor genes, they may rather harbor genes that affect the central processing than the peripheral detection of the odour signal. Thus, perception of odours is potentially modified by genes other than those encoding odorant receptors.


Assuntos
Cromossomos Humanos Par 2/genética , Cromossomos Humanos Par 4/genética , Ligação Genética , Odorantes , Olfato/genética , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Feminino , Finlândia , Genoma Humano/genética , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Linhagem
17.
Am J Clin Nutr ; 86(6): 1663-9, 2007 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18065584

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Sweet taste preferences are measured by several often correlated measures. OBJECTIVE: We examined the relative proportions of genetic and environmental effects on sweet taste preference indicators and their mutual correlations. DESIGN: A total of 663 female twins (324 complete pairs, 149 monozygous and 175 dizygous pairs) aged 17-80 y rated the liking and intensity of a 20% (wt/vol) sucrose solution, reported the liking and the use-frequency of 6 sweet foods (sweet desserts, sweets, sweet pastry, ice cream, hard candy, and chocolate), and completed a questionnaire on cravings of sweet foods. The estimated contributions of genetic factors, environmental factors shared by a twin pair, and environmental factors unique to each twin individual to the variance and covariance of the traits were obtained with the use of linear structural equation modeling. RESULTS: Approximately half of the variation in liking for sweet solution and liking and use-frequency of sweet foods (49-53%) was explained by genetic factors, whereas the rest of the variation was due to environmental factors unique to each twin individual. Sweet taste preference-related traits were correlated. Tetravariate modeling showed that the correlation between liking for the sweet solution and liking for sweet foods was due to genetic factors (genetic r = 0.27). Correlations between liking, use-frequency, and craving for sweet foods were due to both genetic and unshared environmental factors. CONCLUSION: Detailed information on the associations between preference measures is an important intermediate goal in the determination of the genetic components affecting sweet taste preferences.


Assuntos
Sacarose/administração & dosagem , Paladar/genética , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Análise Fatorial , Feminino , Humanos , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Modelos Genéticos , Projetos Piloto , Inquéritos e Questionários , Gêmeos Dizigóticos , Gêmeos Monozigóticos
18.
Am J Clin Nutr ; 86(1): 55-63, 2007 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17616763

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Humans have an innate preference for sweet taste, but the degree of liking for sweet foods varies individually. OBJECTIVE: The proportion of inherited sweet taste preference was studied. A genome-wide linkage analysis was performed to locate the underlying genetic elements in the genome. DESIGN: A total of 146 subjects (32% men, 68% women) aged 18-78 y from 26 Finnish families evaluated the intensity and pleasantness of 3 suprathreshold solutions of sucrose (3.0%, 7.5%, and 18.75%) and plain water and the intensity of filter paper impregnated with 6-n-propylthiouracil (PROP). The subjects also reported the pleasantness and the use frequency of 5 sweet foods (chocolate, candy, ice cream, sweet desserts, and sweet pastry) and completed a food-behavior questionnaire that measured their craving for sweet foods. RESULTS: Of the chemosensory functions, the pleasantness rating of the strongest (18.75%) sucrose solution and the intensity rating of PROP yielded the highest heritability estimates (41% and 66%, respectively). The pleasantness and the use frequency of sweet foods (both variables calculated as a mean of ratings for 5 food items) and the craving for sweet foods showed significant heritability (40%, 50%, and 31%, respectively). A logarithm of odds score of 3.5 (P=0.00003) was detected for use frequency of sweet foods on chromosome 16p11.2 (marker D16S753). CONCLUSIONS: Sweet taste preferences are partly inherited. Chromosome 16p11.2 may harbor genetic variations that affect the consumption of sweet foods.


Assuntos
Cromossomos Humanos Par 16/genética , Preferências Alimentares , Locos de Características Quantitativas/genética , Paladar/genética , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , DNA/química , DNA/genética , Feminino , Genótipo , Humanos , Escore Lod , Masculino , Repetições de Microssatélites/genética , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Propiltiouracila , Escala de Ansiedade Frente a Teste
19.
Physiol Behav ; 91(5): 573-8, 2007 Aug 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17459432

RESUMO

Food neophobia refers to reluctance to eat unfamiliar foods. We determined the heritability of food neophobia in a family and a twin sample. The family sample consisted of 28 Finnish families (105 females, 50 males, aged 18-78 years, mean age 49 years) and the twin sample of 468 British female twin pairs (211 monozygous and 257 dizygous pairs, aged 17-82 years, mean age 55 years). Food neophobia was measured using the ten-item Food Neophobia Scale (FNS) questionnaire, and its internationally validated six-item modification. The heritability estimate for food neophobia was 69 and 66% in Finnish families (h(2)) and 67 and 66% in British female twins (a(2)+d(2)) using the ten- and six-item versions of the FNS, respectively. The results from both populations suggest that about two thirds of variation in food neophobia is genetically determined.


Assuntos
Ingestão de Alimentos/genética , Preferências Alimentares/psicologia , Transtornos Fóbicos/genética , Característica Quantitativa Herdável , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Comportamento de Escolha , Ingestão de Alimentos/psicologia , Feminino , Variação Genética , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Modelos Genéticos , Linhagem , Transtornos Fóbicos/psicologia , Valores de Referência , Gêmeos Dizigóticos , Gêmeos Monozigóticos
20.
Physiol Behav ; 85(3): 314-23, 2005 Jun 30.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15936048

RESUMO

Means to boost food intake of the elderly (n = 60, age range 61-86 years) varying in olfactory and mood status were studied during home-use of cold cuts. Enhancement group (n = 28) received regular smoked ham and smoked ham flavored with extra strong smoke aroma or with pepper mix, and Variety group (n = 32) received four different cold cuts: smoked ham, cooked ham, pepper ham and bologna. Both groups participated in two successive home-use periods, each over 6 days. Before and after home-use, the cold cuts were rated for odor and flavor intensity and liking, and in home-use only for liking. Cold cuts, 600 g per period, were to be consumed ad libitum, and the returned left-overs were weighed. Scandinavian Odor Identification Test and Profile of Mood States (bi-polar) were used in dividing subjects into normosmics and hyposmics (SOIT, cut-off score: 11) and into mood groups (POMS, depressed-elated, cut-off score: median). Enhancement group liked both the regular and the ham enhanced with pepper mix similarly, while added smoke aroma decreased liking. Regardless of the olfactory capabilities, the subjects >74 years of age were less responsive to the flavor changes in their ratings than those aged 61-74 years. Poor olfactory status increased the intakes regardless of the flavor level or type of cold cuts. Path analysis suggested that in the Enhancement group, poor health, depressed mood and high age predicted a poor olfactory performance which, along with ratings of liking, increased the consumed amounts of cold cuts.


Assuntos
Afeto/fisiologia , Ingestão de Alimentos/fisiologia , Odorantes , Olfato/fisiologia , Paladar/fisiologia , Sensação Térmica/fisiologia , Fatores Etários , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Ingestão de Alimentos/efeitos dos fármacos , Feminino , Aromatizantes/farmacologia , Avaliação Geriátrica , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Modelos Psicológicos , Olfato/efeitos dos fármacos , Paladar/efeitos dos fármacos , Sensação Térmica/efeitos dos fármacos
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA