Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 638
Filtrar
Mais filtros

Tipo de documento
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
J Vis ; 22(12): 13, 2022 11 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36374493

RESUMO

Weber's law is a fundamental psychophysical principle. It states that the just noticeable difference (JND) between stimuli increases with stimulus magnitude; consequently, larger stimuli should be estimated with larger variability. However, visually guided grasping seems to violate this expectation: When repeatedly grasping large objects, the variability is similar to that when grasping small objects. Based on this result, it was often concluded that grasping violated Weber's law. This astonishing finding generated a flurry of research, with contradictory results and potentially far-reaching implications for theorizing about the functional architecture of the brain. We show that previous studies ignored nonlinearities in the scaling of the grasping response. These nonlinearities result from, for example, the finger span being limited such that the opening of the fingers reaches a ceiling for large objects. We describe how to mathematically take these nonlinearities into account and apply this approach to our own data, as well as to the data of three influential studies on this topic. In all four datasets, we found that-when appropriately estimated-JNDs increase with object size, as expected by Weber's law. We conclude that grasping obeys Weber's law, as do essentially all sensory dimensions.


Assuntos
Força da Mão , Desempenho Psicomotor , Humanos , Limiar Diferencial/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Força da Mão/fisiologia , Dedos , Encéfalo
2.
Neural Plast ; 2021: 6692411, 2021.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33531892

RESUMO

Postinhibitory facilitation (PIF) of neural firing presents a paradoxical phenomenon that the inhibitory effect induces enhancement instead of reduction of the firing activity, which plays important roles in sound location of the auditory nervous system, awaited theoretical explanations. In the present paper, excitability and threshold mechanism for the PIF phenomenon is presented in the Morris-Lecar model with type I, II, and III excitabilities. Firstly, compared with the purely excitatory stimulations applied to the steady state, the inhibitory preceding excitatory stimulation to form pairs induces the firing rate increased for type II and III excitabilities instead of type I excitability, when the interval between the inhibitory and excitatory stimulation within each pair is suitable. Secondly, the threshold mechanism for the PIF phenomenon is acquired. For type II and III excitabilities, the inhibitory stimulation induces subthreshold oscillations around the steady state. During the middle and ending phase of the ascending part and the beginning phase of the descending part within a period of the subthreshold oscillations, the threshold to evoke an action potential by an excitatory stimulation becomes weaker, which is the cause for the PIF phenomenon. Last, a theoretical estimation for the range of the interval between the inhibitory and excitatory stimulation for the PIF phenomenon is acquired, which approximates half of the intrinsic period of the subthreshold oscillations for the relatively strong stimulations and becomes narrower for the relatively weak stimulations. The interval for the PIF phenomenon is much shorter for type III excitability, which is closer to the experiment observation, due to the shorter period of the subthreshold oscillations. The results present the excitability and threshold mechanism for the PIF phenomenon, which provide comprehensive and deep explanations to the PIF phenomenon.


Assuntos
Potenciais de Ação/fisiologia , Modelos Neurológicos , Inibição Neural/fisiologia , Neurônios/fisiologia , Limiar Diferencial/fisiologia , Humanos
3.
Child Dev ; 91(6): e1178-e1193, 2020 11.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32767767

RESUMO

We examined children's spontaneous information seeking in response to referential ambiguity. Children ages 2-5 (n = 160) identified the referents of familiar and novel labels. We manipulated ambiguity by changing the number of objects present and their familiarity (Experiments 1 and 2), and the availability of referential gaze (Experiment 2). In both experiments, children looked to the face of the experimenter more often while responding, specifically when the referent was ambiguous. In Experiment 2, 3- to 4-year olds also demonstrated sensitivity to graded referential evidence. These results suggest that social information seeking is an active learning behavior that could contribute to language acquisition in early childhood.


Assuntos
Comportamento Infantil/fisiologia , Comportamento de Busca de Informação/fisiologia , Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Reconhecimento Psicológico/fisiologia , Pré-Escolar , Limiar Diferencial/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Comportamento Social
4.
Exp Brain Res ; 237(10): 2729-2734, 2019 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31432227

RESUMO

The cholinergic system is known to strongly modulate perceptual and cognitive processes, and the alpha7 subunit of the cholinergic nicotinic receptor (CHRNA7) is broadly expressed within the visual system. Here, we assessed whether genetic variations of CHRNA7 affect coherent motion perception. Motion perception has been shown to decline with age, and it has previously been suggested that the effects of genetic variations are magnified by age. Therefore, we tested both older (n = 62) and younger adults (n = 63). We found that motion coherence thresholds were significantly higher for older compared to younger adults, which is in accordance with previous studies. Interestingly, there was a strong relationship between variants of the SNP rs2337980 of the CHRNA7 and motion direction discrimination. In particular, participants carrying the TC genotype had considerably lower motion coherence thresholds than CC carriers. The effect of genotype did not interact with age. Our results show that genetic variations are associated with perceptual performance, but are unlikely to explain age-related changes.


Assuntos
Percepção de Movimento/fisiologia , Limiar Sensorial/fisiologia , Receptor Nicotínico de Acetilcolina alfa7/genética , Adulto , Limiar Diferencial/fisiologia , Feminino , Genótipo , Humanos , Masculino , Polimorfismo de Nucleotídeo Único , Receptores Nicotínicos/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
5.
Psychol Res ; 83(5): 977-988, 2019 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28871420

RESUMO

Visually guided grasping movements directed to real, 3D objects are characterized by a distinguishable trajectory pattern that evades the influence of Weber's law, a basic principle of perception. Conversely, grasping trajectories directed to 2D line drawings of objects adhere to Weber's law. It can be argued, therefore, that during 2D grasping, the visuomotor system fails at operating in analytic mode and is intruded by irrelevant perceptual information. Here, we explored the visual and tactile cues that enable such analytic processing during grasping. In Experiment 1, we compared grasping directed to 3D objects with grasping directed to 2D object photos. Grasping directed to photos adhered to Weber's law, suggesting that richness in visual detail does not contribute to analytic processing. In Experiment 2, we tested whether the visual presentation of 3D objects could support analytic processing even when only partial object-specific tactile information is provided. Surprisingly, grasping could be performed in an analytic fashion, violating Weber's law. In Experiment 3, participants were denied of any haptic feedback at the end of the movement and grasping trajectories again showed adherence to Weber's law. Taken together, the findings suggest that the presentation of real objects combined with indirect haptic information at the end of the movement is sufficient to allow analytic processing during grasp.


Assuntos
Limiar Diferencial/fisiologia , Força da Mão/fisiologia , Percepção do Tato , Percepção Visual , Sinais (Psicologia) , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Movimento/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
6.
Exp Brain Res ; 236(8): 2439-2446, 2018 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29923096

RESUMO

Recent work proposed that biomechanical constraints in aperture separation limit the utility of Weber's law in determining whether dissociable visual codes support grasping and manual estimation. We tested this assertion by having participants precision grasp, manually estimate and complete a method of adjustment task to targets scaled within and beyond the range of their maximal aperture separation (i.e., from 20 to 140% of participant-specific maximal aperture separation: MAS). For grasping and manual estimation tasks, just-noticeable-difference (JND) scores were computed via the within-participant standard deviations in peak grip aperture, whereas method of adjustment JNDs were computed via the within-participant standard deviations in response output. Method of adjustment JNDs increased linearly across the range of targets; that is, responses adhered to Weber's law. Manual estimation JNDs linearly increased for targets 20-100% of MAS and then decreased for targets 120-140% of MAS. In turn, grasping JNDs for targets 20% through 80% of MAS did not differ and were larger than targets 100-140% of MAS. That manual estimation and grasping showed a decrease in JNDs for the largest targets indicates that participants were at their biomechanical limits in aperture shaping, and the fact that the target showing the JND decrease differed between tasks (i.e., manual estimation = 100% of MAS; grasping = 80% of MAS) is attributed to the fact that grasping-but not manual estimation-requires a safety-margin task-set. Accordingly, manual estimations and grasping across a range of functionally 'graspable' targets, respectively, adhered to and violated Weber's law-a result interpreted to reflect the use of dissociable visual codes.


Assuntos
Limiar Diferencial/fisiologia , Força da Mão/fisiologia , Mãos/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Adulto , Antropometria , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Psicofísica , Adulto Jovem
7.
Int Tinnitus J ; 22(1): 1-9, 2018 Jun 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29993210

RESUMO

Animal models of tinnitus rely on interpretation of behavioural or reflexive tests to determine the presence of this phantom perception. A commonly used test is the gap prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle (GPIAS), which is often combined with prepulse inhibition (PPI) to ensure that reduced GPIAS suppression is not due to hearing loss caused by the acoustic trauma commonly used to trigger tinnitus development. In our laboratory GPIAS and PPI are routinely used on two colonies of outbred tri-colour guinea pigs. However, our results show that these colonies show divergent results even before any tinnitus-inducing treatment, which impacts their suitability in tinnitus models. Although colony 1 and 2 show similar results in PPI (~95% of animals showing significant suppression), only ~30% of colony 2 also shows significant suppression in GPIAS compared to ~75% of colony 1. Cochlear sensitivity measured using compound action potentials showed no significant differences between colonies. Therefore, peripheral threshold loss was excluded as a possible factor. Our results show that similar strains of laboratory animals can show highly divergent results and GPIAS testing for tinnitus will not work for every animal strain. In addition, our data support the notion that PPI and GPIAS responses may rely on different neural circuitry.


Assuntos
Potenciais de Ação/fisiologia , Cóclea/fisiopatologia , Inibição Pré-Pulso/fisiologia , Reflexo de Sobressalto/fisiologia , Zumbido/fisiopatologia , Estimulação Acústica , Animais , Limiar Diferencial/fisiologia , Cobaias , Modelos Animais , Zumbido/etiologia
8.
Am J Physiol Cell Physiol ; 312(2): C144-C154, 2017 Feb 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27903582

RESUMO

l-Arginine (L-Arg) is the substrate for nitric oxide synthase (NOS) to produce nitric oxide (NO), a signaling molecule that is key in cardiovascular physiology and pathology. In cardiac myocytes, L-Arg is incorporated from the circulation through the functioning of system-y+ cationic amino acid transporters. Depletion of L-Arg leads to NOS uncoupling, with O2 rather than L-Arg as the terminal electron acceptor, resulting in superoxide formation. The reactive oxygen species (ROS) superoxide (O2˙-), combined with NO, may lead to the production of the reactive nitrogen species (RNS) peroxynitrite (ONOO-), which is recognized as a major contributor to myocardial depression. In this study we aimed to determine the levels of external L-Arg that trigger ROS/RNS production in cardiac myocytes. To this goal, we used a two-step experimental design in which acutely isolated cardiomyocytes were loaded with the dye coelenterazine that greatly increases its fluorescence quantum yield in the presence of ONOO- and O2˙- Cells were then exposed to different concentrations of extracellular L-Arg and changes in fluorescence were followed spectrofluorometrically. It was found that below a threshold value of ~100 µM, decreasing concentrations of L-Arg progressively increased ONOO-/ O2˙--induced fluorescence, an effect that was not mimicked by d-arginine or l-lysine and was fully blocked by the NOS inhibitor l-NAME. These results can be explained by NOS aberrant enzymatic activity and provide an estimate for the levels of circulating L-Arg below which ROS/RNS-mediated harmful effects arise in cardiac muscle.


Assuntos
Arginina/administração & dosagem , Ventrículos do Coração/metabolismo , Miócitos Cardíacos/metabolismo , Óxido Nítrico Sintase/metabolismo , Espécies Reativas de Nitrogênio/metabolismo , Espécies Reativas de Oxigênio/metabolismo , Animais , Células Cultivadas , Limiar Diferencial/fisiologia , Relação Dose-Resposta a Droga , Líquido Extracelular/efeitos dos fármacos , Líquido Extracelular/metabolismo , Ventrículos do Coração/citologia , Ventrículos do Coração/efeitos dos fármacos , Masculino , Miócitos Cardíacos/citologia , Miócitos Cardíacos/efeitos dos fármacos , Ratos , Ratos Sprague-Dawley , Transdução de Sinais/efeitos dos fármacos , Transdução de Sinais/fisiologia
9.
Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol ; 312(5): R727-R738, 2017 05 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28202437

RESUMO

µ-Opioid receptors are distributed widely in the brain stem respiratory network, and opioids with selectivity for µ-type receptors slow in vivo respiratory rhythm in lowest effective doses. Several studies have reported µ-opioid receptor effects on the three-phase rhythm of respiratory neurons, but there are until now no reports of opioid effects on oscillatory activity within respiratory discharges. In this study, effects of the µ-opioid receptor agonist fentanyl on spike train discharge properties of several different types of rhythm-modulating medullary respiratory neuron discharges were analyzed. Doses of fentanyl that were just sufficient for prolongation of discharges and slowing of the three-phase respiratory rhythm also produced pronounced enhancement of spike train properties. Oscillation and burst patterns detected by autocorrelation measurements were greatly enhanced, and interspike intervals were prolonged. Spike train properties under control conditions and after fentanyl were uniform within each experiment, but varied considerably between experiments, which might be related to variability in acid-base balance in the brain stem extracellular fluid. Discharge threshold was shifted to more negative levels of membrane potential. The effects on threshold are postulated to result from opioid-mediated disinhibition and postsynaptic enhancement of N-methyl-d- aspartate receptor current. Lowering of firing threshold, enhancement of spike train oscillations and bursts and prolongation of discharges by lowest effective doses of fentanyl could represent compensatory adjustments in the brain stem respiratory network to override opioid blunting of CO2/pH chemosensitivity.


Assuntos
Potenciais de Ação/fisiologia , Relógios Biológicos/fisiologia , Fentanila/administração & dosagem , Bulbo/fisiologia , Neurônios/fisiologia , Receptores Opioides mu/antagonistas & inibidores , Respiração/efeitos dos fármacos , Potenciais de Ação/efeitos dos fármacos , Animais , Relógios Biológicos/efeitos dos fármacos , Gatos , Geradores de Padrão Central/fisiologia , Limiar Diferencial/efeitos dos fármacos , Limiar Diferencial/fisiologia , Relação Dose-Resposta a Droga , Bulbo/efeitos dos fármacos , Antagonistas de Entorpecentes/administração & dosagem , Neurônios/efeitos dos fármacos , Receptores Opioides mu/metabolismo
10.
Exp Brain Res ; 235(6): 1701-1707, 2017 06.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28280878

RESUMO

The manual estimation task requires that participants separate the distance between their thumb and forefinger until they perceive it to match the size of a target object. Ganel and colleagues (Curr Biol 18:R599-R601, 2008a) demonstrated that manual estimations yield just-noticeable-difference (JND) scores that linearly increased with increasing target object size; that is, responses adhered to Weber's law and thus evince response mediation via relative and perception-based visual information. In turn, more recent work has reported that the size of a target object influences whether JNDs provide a reliable metric for evaluating the nature of the visual information supporting manual estimations. In particular, Bruno et al. (Neuropsychologia 91:327-334, 2016) reported that JNDs for 'large' target objects (i.e., 80 and 120 mm) violate Weber's law due to biomechanical limits in aperture opening. It is, however, important to recognize that the absolute size of the 'large' target objects employed by Bruno et al. may have exceeded some participants' functional aperture separation and resulted in a biomechanical strategy serving as the only viable response mode. Hence, the present investigation employed a manual estimation task wherein target object sizes were proportionately matched to decile increments (i.e., 10, 20, …, 70 and 80%) of individual participants' maximal aperture separation. Results showed that JNDs increased linearly with increasing target object size. Accordingly, we propose that manual estimations of target objects within a functionally 'graspable' range adhere to Weber's law and are mediated via relative and perception-based visual information.


Assuntos
Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Percepção de Tamanho/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Adulto , Limiar Diferencial/fisiologia , Feminino , Dedos , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
11.
Neural Plast ; 2017: 9848070, 2017.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29062576

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: The somatosensory temporal discrimination threshold (STDT) is defined as the shortest interval at which an individual recognizes two stimuli as asynchronous. Some evidence suggests that STDT depends on cortical inhibitory interneurons in the basal ganglia and in primary somatosensory cortex. Several studies have reported that the STDT in patients with dystonia is abnormal. No longitudinal studies have yet investigated whether STDT values in different forms of focal dystonia change during the course of the disease. METHODS: We designed a follow-up study on 25 patients with dystonia (15 with blepharospasm and 10 with cervical dystonia) who were tested twice: upon enrolment and 8 years later. STDT values from dystonic patients at the baseline were also compared with those from a group of 30 age-matched healthy subjects. RESULTS: Our findings show that the abnormally high STDT values observed in patients with focal dystonia remained unchanged at the 8-year follow-up assessment whereas disease severity worsened. CONCLUSIONS: Our observation that STDT abnormalities in dystonia remain unmodified during the course of the disease suggests that the altered activity of inhibitory interneurons-either at cortical or at subcortical level-responsible for the increased STDT does not deteriorate as the disease progresses.


Assuntos
Limiar Diferencial/fisiologia , Distúrbios Distônicos/fisiopatologia , Percepção do Tempo/fisiologia , Percepção do Tato/fisiologia , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Feminino , Seguimentos , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade
12.
PLoS Comput Biol ; 11(10): e1004479, 2015 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26513150

RESUMO

Power laws describe brain functions at many levels (from biophysics to psychophysics). It is therefore possible that they are generated by similar underlying mechanisms. Previously, the response properties of a collision-sensitive neuron were reproduced by a model which used a power law for scaling its inhibitory input. A common characteristic of such neurons is that they integrate information across a large part of the visual field. Here we present a biophysically plausible model of collision-sensitive neurons with η-like response properties, in which we assume that each information channel is noisy and has a response threshold. Then, an approximative power law is obtained as a result of pooling these channels. We show that with this mechanism one can successfully predict many response characteristics of the Lobula Giant Movement Detector Neuron (LGMD). Moreover, the results depend critically on noise in the inhibitory pathway, but they are fairly robust against noise in the excitatory pathway.


Assuntos
Dendritos/fisiologia , Modelos Neurológicos , Modelos Estatísticos , Percepção de Movimento/fisiologia , Neurônios Aferentes/fisiologia , Sinapses/fisiologia , Animais , Simulação por Computador , Limiar Diferencial/fisiologia , Gafanhotos/fisiologia , Limiar Sensorial/fisiologia , Razão Sinal-Ruído
13.
PLoS Comput Biol ; 11(4): e1004114, 2015 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25856629

RESUMO

A large variety of neuron models are used in theoretical and computational neuroscience, and among these, single-compartment models are a popular kind. These models do not explicitly include the dendrites or the axon, and range from the Hodgkin-Huxley (HH) model to various flavors of integrate-and-fire (IF) models. The main classes of models differ in the way spikes are initiated. Which one is the most realistic? Starting with some general epistemological considerations, I show that the notion of realism comes in two dimensions: empirical content (the sort of predictions that a model can produce) and empirical accuracy (whether these predictions are correct). I then examine the realism of the main classes of single-compartment models along these two dimensions, in light of recent experimental evidence.


Assuntos
Potenciais de Ação/fisiologia , Canais Iônicos/fisiologia , Potenciais da Membrana/fisiologia , Modelos Neurológicos , Transmissão Sináptica/fisiologia , Animais , Simulação por Computador , Limiar Diferencial/fisiologia , Humanos , Ativação do Canal Iônico/fisiologia , Modelos Estatísticos
14.
J Vis ; 16(10): 5, 2016 08 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27537702

RESUMO

Sensory systems are faced with an essentially infinite number of possible environmental events but have limited processing resources. Posed with this challenge, it makes sense to allocate these resources to prioritize the discrimination of events with the most behavioral relevance. Here, we asked if such relevance is reflected in the processing and perception of motion. We compared human performance on a rapid motion direction discrimination task, including monocular and binocular viewing. In particular, we determined sensitivity and bias for a binocular motion-in-depth (three-dimensional; 3D) stimulus and for its constituent monocular (two-dimensional; 2D) signals over a broad range of speeds. Consistent with prior work, we found that binocular 3D sensitivity was lower than monocular sensitivity for all speeds. Although overall sensitivity was worse for 3D discrimination, we found that the transformation from 2D to 3D motion processing also incorporated a pattern of potentially advantageous biases. One such bias is reflected by a criterion shift that occurs at the level of 3D motion processing and results in an increased hit rate for motion toward the head. We also observed an increase in sensitivity for 3D motion trajectories presented on crossed rather than uncrossed disparity pedestals, privileging motion trajectories closer to the observer. We used these measurements to determine the range of real-world trajectories for which rapid 3D motion discrimination is most useful. These results suggest that the neural mechanisms that underlie motion perception privilege behaviorally relevant motion and provide insights into the nature of human motion sensitivity in the real world.


Assuntos
Percepção de Profundidade/fisiologia , Discriminação Psicológica/fisiologia , Percepção de Movimento/fisiologia , Movimento (Física) , Disparidade Visual , Visão Binocular/fisiologia , Adulto , Limiar Diferencial/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Matemática , Adulto Jovem
15.
J Vis ; 16(9): 2, 2016 07 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27380471

RESUMO

We have shown in previous work that the perception of order in point patterns is consistent with an interval scale structure (Protonotarios, Baum, Johnston, Hunter, & Griffin, 2014). The psychophysical scaling method used relies on the confusion between stimuli with similar levels of order, and the resulting discrimination scale is expressed in just-noticeable differences (jnds). As with other perceptual dimensions, an interesting question is whether suprathreshold (perceptual) differences are consistent with distances between stimuli on the discrimination scale. To test that, we collected discrimination data, and data based on comparison of perceptual differences. The stimuli were jittered square lattices of dots, covering the range from total disorder (Poisson) to perfect order (square lattice), roughly equally spaced on the discrimination scale. Observers picked the most ordered pattern from a pair, and the pair of patterns with the greatest difference in order from two pairs. Although the judgments of perceptual difference were found to be consistent with an interval scale, like the discrimination judgments, no common interval scale that could predict both sets of data was possible. In particular, the midpattern of the perceptual scale is 11 jnds away from the ordered end, and 5 jnds from the disordered end of the discrimination scale.


Assuntos
Limiar Diferencial/fisiologia , Discriminação Psicológica/fisiologia , Julgamento/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Psicofísica/métodos , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
16.
PLoS Comput Biol ; 10(5): e1003574, 2014 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24784237

RESUMO

High-frequency oscillations (above 30 Hz) have been observed in sensory and higher-order brain areas, and are believed to constitute a general hallmark of functional neuronal activation. Fast inhibition in interneuronal networks has been suggested as a general mechanism for the generation of high-frequency oscillations. Certain classes of interneurons exhibit subthreshold oscillations, but the effect of this intrinsic neuronal property on the population rhythm is not completely understood. We study the influence of intrinsic damped subthreshold oscillations in the emergence of collective high-frequency oscillations, and elucidate the dynamical mechanisms that underlie this phenomenon. We simulate neuronal networks composed of either Integrate-and-Fire (IF) or Generalized Integrate-and-Fire (GIF) neurons. The IF model displays purely passive subthreshold dynamics, while the GIF model exhibits subthreshold damped oscillations. Individual neurons receive inhibitory synaptic currents mediated by spiking activity in their neighbors as well as noisy synaptic bombardment, and fire irregularly at a lower rate than population frequency. We identify three factors that affect the influence of single-neuron properties on synchronization mediated by inhibition: i) the firing rate response to the noisy background input, ii) the membrane potential distribution, and iii) the shape of Inhibitory Post-Synaptic Potentials (IPSPs). For hyperpolarizing inhibition, the GIF IPSP profile (factor iii)) exhibits post-inhibitory rebound, which induces a coherent spike-mediated depolarization across cells that greatly facilitates synchronous oscillations. This effect dominates the network dynamics, hence GIF networks display stronger oscillations than IF networks. However, the restorative current in the GIF neuron lowers firing rates and narrows the membrane potential distribution (factors i) and ii), respectively), which tend to decrease synchrony. If inhibition is shunting instead of hyperpolarizing, post-inhibitory rebound is not elicited and factors i) and ii) dominate, yielding lower synchrony in GIF networks than in IF networks.


Assuntos
Potenciais de Ação/fisiologia , Relógios Biológicos/fisiologia , Interneurônios/fisiologia , Modelos Neurológicos , Rede Nervosa/fisiologia , Sinapses/fisiologia , Transmissão Sináptica/fisiologia , Animais , Simulação por Computador , Limiar Diferencial/fisiologia , Condutividade Elétrica , Humanos
17.
Biol Cybern ; 109(4-5): 479-91, 2015 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26228799

RESUMO

Sensitization is an example of malfunctioning of the nociceptive pathway in either the peripheral or central nervous system. Using quantitative sensory testing, one can only infer sensitization, but not determine the defective subsystem. The states of the subsystems may be characterized using computational modeling together with experimental data. Here, we develop a neurophysiologically plausible model replicating experimental observations from a psychophysical human subject study. We study the effects of single temporal stimulus parameters on detection thresholds corresponding to a 0.5 detection probability. To model peripheral activation and central processing, we adapt a stochastic drift-diffusion model and a probabilistic hazard model to our experimental setting without reaction times. We retain six lumped parameters in both models characterizing peripheral and central mechanisms. Both models have similar psychophysical functions, but the hazard model is computationally more efficient. The model-based effects of temporal stimulus parameters on detection thresholds are consistent with those from human subject data.


Assuntos
Simulação por Computador , Modelos Biológicos , Fibras Nervosas/fisiologia , Peptídeos Opioides/fisiologia , Detecção de Sinal Psicológico/fisiologia , Análise de Variância , Limiar Diferencial/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Modelos Logísticos , Masculino , Psicofísica , Tempo de Reação , Estimulação Elétrica Nervosa Transcutânea , Nociceptina
18.
Somatosens Mot Res ; 32(3): 163-71, 2015.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26053790

RESUMO

Many obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) patients experience sensory phenomena, such as bodily sensations and "just-right" perceptions accompanying compulsions. We studied tactile processing in OCD by psychophysical experiments targeting the somatosensory cortex. Thirty-two children and adolescents with OCD (8 tic-related, 19 with sensory phenomena (SP)) and their sex- and age-matched controls participated in the study. After clinical assessments, two questionnaires were completed for sensory problems (Sensory Profile and Touch Inventory for Elementary-School-Aged Children). The psychophysical experiments consisted of five tasks: simple reaction time, choice reaction time, dynamic (detection) threshold, amplitude discrimination, and amplitude discrimination with single-site adaptation. The tactile stimuli were sinusoidal mechanical vibrations (frequency: 25 Hz) applied on the fingertips. Just-noticeable differences (JNDs) were found in amplitude discrimination tasks. There was no difference between the OCD group and controls in detection thresholds. However, the OCD group (especially young males) had worse amplitude discrimination (i.e., higher JNDs) than controls. Young OCD participants had reduced adaptation than young controls. Tic-related OCD participants and those with SP had higher detection thresholds than those without. Additionally, the OCD group reported more problems than controls in the Emotional/Social subset of the Sensory Profile questionnaire. The discrimination results show altered tactile processing in OCD at suprathreshold levels. This can be explained by a scaling factor modifying the sensory signal with decreasing slope at higher input levels to achieve normal Weber fractions internally. Quadratic discriminant analysis gave the best positive (76%) and negative (60%) predictive values for classifying individuals (into "OCD" or "control" groups) based on psychophysical data alone.


Assuntos
Limiar Diferencial/fisiologia , Transtorno Obsessivo-Compulsivo/fisiopatologia , Tato/fisiologia , Adolescente , Fatores Etários , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Criança , Discriminação Psicológica , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulação Física , Escalas de Graduação Psiquiátrica , Psicofísica , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Fatores Sexuais , Estatística como Assunto , Inquéritos e Questionários
19.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 138(5): 2782-90, 2015 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26627754

RESUMO

This study investigated whether native listeners processed speech differently from non-native listeners in a speech detection task. Detection thresholds of Mandarin Chinese and Korean vowels and non-speech sounds in noise, frequency selectivity, and the nativeness of Mandarin Chinese and Korean vowels were measured for Mandarin Chinese- and Korean-native listeners. The two groups of listeners exhibited similar non-speech sound detection and frequency selectivity; however, the Korean listeners had better detection thresholds of Korean vowels than Chinese listeners, while the Chinese listeners performed no better at Chinese vowel detection than the Korean listeners. Moreover, thresholds predicted from an auditory model highly correlated with behavioral thresholds of the two groups of listeners, suggesting that detection of speech sounds not only depended on listeners' frequency selectivity, but also might be affected by their native language experience. Listeners evaluated their native vowels with higher nativeness scores than non-native listeners. Native listeners may have advantages over non-native listeners when processing speech sounds in noise, even without the required phonetic processing; however, such native speech advantages might be offset by Chinese listeners' lower sensitivity to vowel sounds, a characteristic possibly resulting from their sparse vowel system and their greater cognitive and attentional demands for vowel processing.


Assuntos
Limiar Auditivo/fisiologia , Limiar Diferencial/fisiologia , Idioma , Fonética , Percepção da Fala/fisiologia , Adulto , Povo Asiático/psicologia , Atenção , China/etnologia , Cognição , Feminino , Humanos , República da Coreia/etnologia , Razão Sinal-Ruído , Espectrografia do Som , Adulto Jovem
20.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 138(5): 3093-104, 2015 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26627783

RESUMO

The question of how frequency is coded in the peripheral auditory system remains unresolved. Previous research has suggested that slow rates of frequency modulation (FM) of a low carrier frequency may be coded via phase-locked temporal information in the auditory nerve, whereas FM at higher rates and/or high carrier frequencies may be coded via a rate-place (tonotopic) code. This hypothesis was tested in a cohort of 100 young normal-hearing listeners by comparing individual sensitivity to slow-rate (1-Hz) and fast-rate (20-Hz) FM at a carrier frequency of 500 Hz with independent measures of phase-locking (using dynamic interaural time difference, ITD, discrimination), level coding (using amplitude modulation, AM, detection), and frequency selectivity (using forward-masking patterns). All FM and AM thresholds were highly correlated with each other. However, no evidence was obtained for stronger correlations between measures thought to reflect phase-locking (e.g., slow-rate FM and ITD sensitivity), or between measures thought to reflect tonotopic coding (fast-rate FM and forward-masking patterns). The results suggest that either psychoacoustic performance in young normal-hearing listeners is not limited by peripheral coding, or that similar peripheral mechanisms limit both high- and low-rate FM coding.


Assuntos
Percepção Auditiva/fisiologia , Cóclea/fisiologia , Nervo Coclear/fisiologia , Sinais (Psicologia) , Individualidade , Modelos Neurológicos , Estimulação Acústica , Adolescente , Adulto , Limiar Auditivo , Testes com Listas de Dissílabos , Limiar Diferencial/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Mascaramento Perceptivo , Periodicidade , Discriminação da Altura Tonal/fisiologia , Análise de Componente Principal , Psicoacústica , Fatores de Tempo , Adulto Jovem
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA