Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 808
Filtrar
Mais filtros

Tipo de documento
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Cell ; 171(3): 507-521.e17, 2017 Oct 19.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28965758

RESUMO

The medial entorhinal cortex (MEC) contains several discrete classes of GABAergic interneurons, but their specific contributions to spatial pattern formation in this area remain elusive. We employed a pharmacogenetic approach to silence either parvalbumin (PV)- or somatostatin (SOM)-expressing interneurons while MEC cells were recorded in freely moving mice. PV-cell silencing antagonized the hexagonally patterned spatial selectivity of grid cells, especially in layer II of MEC. The impairment was accompanied by reduced speed modulation in colocalized speed cells. Silencing SOM cells, in contrast, had no impact on grid cells or speed cells but instead decreased the spatial selectivity of cells with discrete aperiodic firing fields. Border cells and head direction cells were not affected by either intervention. The findings point to distinct roles for PV and SOM interneurons in the local dynamics underlying periodic and aperiodic firing in spatially modulated cells of the MEC. VIDEO ABSTRACT.


Assuntos
Córtex Entorrinal/citologia , Interneurônios/metabolismo , Parvalbuminas/metabolismo , Somatostatina/metabolismo , Processamento Espacial , Animais , Neurônios GABAérgicos/metabolismo , Células de Grade/citologia , Masculino , Camundongos , Camundongos Endogâmicos C57BL , Vias Neurais
2.
Cell ; 167(4): 933-946.e20, 2016 11 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27881303

RESUMO

To execute accurate movements, animals must continuously adapt their behavior to changes in their bodies and environments. Animals can learn changes in the relationship between their locomotor commands and the resulting distance moved, then adjust command strength to achieve a desired travel distance. It is largely unknown which circuits implement this form of motor learning, or how. Using whole-brain neuronal imaging and circuit manipulations in larval zebrafish, we discovered that the serotonergic dorsal raphe nucleus (DRN) mediates short-term locomotor learning. Serotonergic DRN neurons respond phasically to swim-induced visual motion, but little to motion that is not self-generated. During prolonged exposure to a given motosensory gain, persistent DRN activity emerges that stores the learned efficacy of motor commands and adapts future locomotor drive for tens of seconds. The DRN's ability to track the effectiveness of motor intent may constitute a computational building block for the broader functions of the serotonergic system. VIDEO ABSTRACT.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem , Modelos Neurológicos , Natação , Peixe-Zebra/fisiologia , Animais , Mapeamento Encefálico , Larva , Optogenética , Núcleos da Rafe/fisiologia , Neurônios Serotoninérgicos/citologia , Neurônios Serotoninérgicos/fisiologia , Processamento Espacial
3.
Nature ; 609(7925): 119-127, 2022 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36002570

RESUMO

Throughout their daily lives, animals and humans often switch between different behaviours. However, neuroscience research typically studies the brain while the animal is performing one behavioural task at a time, and little is known about how brain circuits represent switches between different behaviours. Here we tested this question using an ethological setting: two bats flew together in a long 135 m tunnel, and switched between navigation when flying alone (solo) and collision avoidance as they flew past each other (cross-over). Bats increased their echolocation click rate before each cross-over, indicating attention to the other bat1-9. Hippocampal CA1 neurons represented the bat's own position when flying alone (place coding10-14). Notably, during cross-overs, neurons switched rapidly to jointly represent the interbat distance by self-position. This neuronal switch was very fast-as fast as 100 ms-which could be revealed owing to the very rapid natural behavioural switch. The neuronal switch correlated with the attention signal, as indexed by echolocation. Interestingly, the different place fields of the same neuron often exhibited very different tuning to interbat distance, creating a complex non-separable coding of position by distance. Theoretical analysis showed that this complex representation yields more efficient coding. Overall, our results suggest that during dynamic natural behaviour, hippocampal neurons can rapidly switch their core computation to represent the relevant behavioural variables, supporting behavioural flexibility.


Assuntos
Quirópteros , Ecolocação , Voo Animal , Hipocampo , Animais , Região CA1 Hipocampal/citologia , Região CA1 Hipocampal/fisiologia , Quirópteros/fisiologia , Ecolocação/fisiologia , Voo Animal/fisiologia , Hipocampo/citologia , Hipocampo/fisiologia , Neurônios/fisiologia , Orientação Espacial , Navegação Espacial , Processamento Espacial
4.
Nature ; 602(7897): 461-467, 2022 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35140401

RESUMO

Visual cortical neurons encode the position and motion direction of specific stimuli retrospectively, without any locomotion or task demand1. The hippocampus, which is a part of the visual system, is hypothesized to require self-motion or a cognitive task to generate allocentric spatial selectivity that is scalar, abstract2,3 and prospective4-7. Here we measured rodent hippocampal selectivity to a moving bar of light in a body-fixed rat to bridge these seeming disparities. About 70% of dorsal CA1 neurons showed stable activity modulation as a function of the angular position of the bar, independent of behaviour and rewards. One-third of tuned cells also encoded the direction of revolution. In other experiments, neurons encoded the distance of the bar, with preference for approaching motion. Collectively, these demonstrate visually evoked vectorial selectivity (VEVS). Unlike place cells, VEVS was retrospective. Changes in the visual stimulus or its predictability did not cause remapping but only caused gradual changes. Most VEVS-tuned neurons behaved like place cells during spatial exploration and the two selectivities were correlated. Thus, VEVS could form the basic building block of hippocampal activity. When combined with self-motion, reward or multisensory stimuli8, it can generate the complexity of prospective representations including allocentric space9, time10,11 and episodes12.


Assuntos
Hipocampo , Luz , Percepção Espacial , Processamento Espacial , Córtex Visual , Animais , Região CA1 Hipocampal/citologia , Região CA1 Hipocampal/fisiologia , Região CA1 Hipocampal/efeitos da radiação , Hipocampo/citologia , Hipocampo/fisiologia , Hipocampo/efeitos da radiação , Neurônios/fisiologia , Neurônios/efeitos da radiação , Ratos , Córtex Visual/citologia , Córtex Visual/fisiologia
5.
Nature ; 600(7889): 484-488, 2021 12.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34759316

RESUMO

Could learning that uses cognitive control to judiciously use relevant information while ignoring distractions generally improve brain function, beyond forming explicit memories? According to a neuroplasticity hypothesis for how some cognitive behavioural therapies are effective, cognitive control training (CCT) changes neural circuit information processing1-3. Here we investigated whether CCT persistently alters hippocampal neural circuit function. We show that mice learned and remembered a conditioned place avoidance during CCT that required ignoring irrelevant locations of shock. CCT facilitated learning new tasks in novel environments for several weeks, relative to unconditioned controls and control mice that avoided the same place during reduced distraction. CCT rapidly changes entorhinal cortex-to-dentate gyrus synaptic circuit function, resulting in an excitatory-inhibitory subcircuit change that persists for months. CCT increases inhibition that attenuates the dentate response to medial entorhinal cortical input, and through disinhibition, potentiates the response to strong inputs, pointing to overall signal-to-noise enhancement. These neurobiological findings support the neuroplasticity hypothesis that, as well as storing item-event associations, CCT persistently optimizes neural circuit information processing.


Assuntos
Cognição/fisiologia , Hipocampo/fisiologia , Modelos Neurológicos , Vias Neurais/fisiologia , Plasticidade Neuronal/fisiologia , Animais , Aprendizagem da Esquiva/fisiologia , Região CA1 Hipocampal/citologia , Região CA1 Hipocampal/fisiologia , Terapia Cognitivo-Comportamental , Condicionamento Operante/fisiologia , Giro Denteado/citologia , Giro Denteado/fisiologia , Córtex Entorrinal/citologia , Córtex Entorrinal/fisiologia , Feminino , Neurônios GABAérgicos , Hipocampo/citologia , Potenciação de Longa Duração , Masculino , Memória/fisiologia , Camundongos , Camundongos Endogâmicos C57BL , Inibição Neural , Processamento Espacial , Sinapses/fisiologia
6.
Nature ; 566(7745): 533-537, 2019 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30742074

RESUMO

Hippocampal place cells are spatially tuned neurons that serve as elements of a 'cognitive map' in the mammalian brain1. To detect the animal's location, place cells are thought to rely upon two interacting mechanisms: sensing the position of the animal relative to familiar landmarks2,3 and measuring the distance and direction that the animal has travelled from previously occupied locations4-7. The latter mechanism-known as path integration-requires a finely tuned gain factor that relates the animal's self-movement to the updating of position on the internal cognitive map, as well as external landmarks to correct the positional error that accumulates8,9. Models of hippocampal place cells and entorhinal grid cells based on path integration treat the path-integration gain as a constant9-14, but behavioural evidence in humans suggests that the gain is modifiable15. Here we show, using physiological evidence from rat hippocampal place cells, that the path-integration gain is a highly plastic variable that can be altered by persistent conflict between self-motion cues and feedback from external landmarks. In an augmented-reality system, visual landmarks were moved in proportion to the movement of a rat on a circular track, creating continuous conflict with path integration. Sustained exposure to this cue conflict resulted in predictable and prolonged recalibration of the path-integration gain, as estimated from the place cells after the landmarks were turned off. We propose that this rapid plasticity keeps the positional update in register with the movement of the rat in the external world over behavioural timescales. These results also demonstrate that visual landmarks not only provide a signal to correct cumulative error in the path-integration system4,8,16-19, but also rapidly fine-tune the integration computation itself.


Assuntos
Hipocampo/citologia , Plasticidade Neuronal/fisiologia , Células de Lugar/citologia , Células de Lugar/fisiologia , Processamento Espacial/fisiologia , Animais , Sinais (Psicologia) , Retroalimentação Fisiológica , Células de Grade/citologia , Células de Grade/fisiologia , Hipocampo/fisiologia , Masculino , Ratos , Ratos Long-Evans , Navegação Espacial/fisiologia
7.
Nature ; 562(7725): 124-127, 2018 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30202092

RESUMO

A major role of vision is to guide navigation, and navigation is strongly driven by vision1-4. Indeed, the brain's visual and navigational systems are known to interact5,6, and signals related to position in the environment have been suggested to appear as early as in the visual cortex6,7. Here, to establish the nature of these signals, we recorded in the primary visual cortex (V1) and hippocampal area CA1 while mice traversed a corridor in virtual reality. The corridor contained identical visual landmarks in two positions, so that a purely visual neuron would respond similarly at those positions. Most V1 neurons, however, responded solely or more strongly to the landmarks in one position rather than the other. This modulation of visual responses by spatial location was not explained by factors such as running speed. To assess whether the modulation is related to navigational signals and to the animal's subjective estimate of position, we trained the mice to lick for a water reward upon reaching a reward zone in the corridor. Neuronal populations in both CA1 and V1 encoded the animal's position along the corridor, and the errors in their representations were correlated. Moreover, both representations reflected the animal's subjective estimate of position, inferred from the animal's licks, better than its actual position. When animals licked in a given location-whether correctly or incorrectly-neural populations in both V1 and CA1 placed the animal in the reward zone. We conclude that visual responses in V1 are controlled by navigational signals, which are coherent with those encoded in hippocampus and reflect the animal's subjective position. The presence of such navigational signals as early as a primary sensory area suggests that they permeate sensory processing in the cortex.


Assuntos
Hipocampo/fisiologia , Comportamento Espacial/fisiologia , Processamento Espacial/fisiologia , Córtex Visual/fisiologia , Animais , Feminino , Hipocampo/citologia , Masculino , Camundongos Endogâmicos C57BL , Neurônios/fisiologia , Recompensa , Realidade Virtual , Córtex Visual/citologia
8.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 242: 105885, 2024 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38471382

RESUMO

Previous work has suggested a different developmental timeline and role of visual experience for the use of spatial and non-spatial features in haptic object recognition. To investigate this conjecture, we used a haptic ambiguous odd-one-out task in which one object needed to be selected as being different from two other objects. The odd-one-out could be selected based on four characteristics: size, shape (spatial), texture, and weight (non-spatial). We tested sighted children from 4 to 12 years of age; congenitally blind, late blind, and adult participants with low vision; and normally sighted adults. Given the protracted developmental time course for spatial perception, we expected a shift from a preference for non-spatial features toward spatial features during typical development. Due to the dominant influence of vision for spatial perception, we expected congenitally blind adults to show a similar preference for non-spatial features as the youngest children. The results confirmed our first hypothesis; the 4-year-olds demonstrated a lower dominance for spatial features for object classification compared with older children and sighted adults. In contrast, our second hypothesis was not confirmed; congenitally blind adults' preferred categorization criteria were indistinguishable from those of sighted controls. These findings suggest an early development, but late maturation, of spatial processing in haptic object recognition independent of visual experience.


Assuntos
Desenvolvimento Infantil , Processamento Espacial , Adulto , Criança , Humanos , Adolescente , Pré-Escolar , Tecnologia Háptica , Percepção Espacial , Percepção Visual , Tato
9.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 118(33)2021 08 17.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34385306

RESUMO

In the attention schema theory (AST), the brain constructs a model of attention, the attention schema, to aid in the endogenous control of attention. Growing behavioral evidence appears to support the presence of a model of attention. However, a central question remains: does a controller of attention actually benefit by having access to an attention schema? We constructed an artificial deep Q-learning neural network agent that was trained to control a simple form of visuospatial attention, tracking a stimulus with an attention spotlight in order to solve a catch task. The agent was tested with and without access to an attention schema. In both conditions, the agent received sufficient information such that it should, theoretically, be able to learn the task. We found that with an attention schema present, the agent learned to control its attention spotlight and learned the catch task. Once the agent learned, if the attention schema was then disabled, the agent's performance was greatly reduced. If the attention schema was removed before learning began, the agent was impaired at learning. The results show how the presence of even a simple attention schema can provide a profound benefit to a controller of attention. We interpret these results as supporting the central argument of AST: the brain contains an attention schema because of its practical benefit in the endogenous control of attention.


Assuntos
Atenção , Aprendizado Profundo , Redes Neurais de Computação , Processamento Espacial
10.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 44(17): 5972-5981, 2023 12 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37811869

RESUMO

To solve spatial tasks, the human brain asks for support from the visual cortices. Nonetheless, representing spatial information is not fixed but depends on the reference frames in which the spatial inputs are involved. The present study investigates how the kind of spatial representations influences the recruitment of visual areas during multisensory spatial tasks. Our study tested participants in an electroencephalography experiment involving two audio-visual (AV) spatial tasks: a spatial bisection, in which participants estimated the relative position in space of an AV stimulus in relation to the position of two other stimuli, and a spatial localization, in which participants localized one AV stimulus in relation to themselves. Results revealed that spatial tasks specifically modulated the occipital event-related potentials (ERPs) after the onset of the stimuli. We observed a greater contralateral early occipital component (50-90 ms) when participants solved the spatial bisection, and a more robust later occipital response (110-160 ms) when they processed the spatial localization. This observation suggests that different spatial representations elicited by multisensory stimuli are sustained by separate neurophysiological mechanisms.


Assuntos
Processamento Espacial , Córtex Visual , Humanos , Potenciais Evocados/fisiologia , Eletroencefalografia , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Córtex Visual/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Percepção Visual/fisiologia
11.
Anim Cogn ; 26(1): 299-317, 2023 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36369418

RESUMO

Rich behavioral and neurobiological evidence suggests cognitive and neural overlap in how quantitatively comparable dimensions such as quantity, time, and space are processed in humans and animals. While magnitude domains such as physical magnitude, time, and space represent information that can be quantitatively compared (4 "is half of" 8), they also represent information that can be organized ordinally (1→2→3→4). Recent evidence suggests that the common representations seen across physical magnitude, time, and space domains in humans may be due to their common ordinal features rather than their common quantitative features, as these common representations appear to extend beyond magnitude domains to include learned orders. In this review, we bring together separate lines of research on multiple ordinal domains including magnitude-based and learned orders in animals to explore the extent to which there is support for a common cognitive process underlying ordinal processing. Animals show similarities in performance patterns across natural quantitatively comparable ordered domains (physical magnitude, time, space, dominance) and learned orders (acquired through transitive inference or simultaneous chaining). Additionally, they show transfer and interference across tasks within and between ordinal domains that support the theory of a common ordinal representation across domains. This review provides some support for the development of a unified theory of ordinality and suggests areas for future research to better characterize the extent to which there are commonalities in cognitive processing of ordinal information generally.


Assuntos
Cognição , Aprendizagem , Animais , Humanos , Processamento Espacial , Tempo
12.
Eur J Neurol ; 30(7): 2106-2111, 2023 07.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37038631

RESUMO

BACKGROUND AND PURPOSE: Cognitive decline is a frequent and debilitating non-motor symptom for patients with Parkinson's disease (PD). Metabolic alterations in the occipital cortex during visual processing may serve as a biomarker for cognitive decline in patients with PD. METHODS: Sixteen patients with PD (Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale Part 3, OFF, 38.69 ± 17.25) and 10 age- and sex-matched healthy controls (HC) underwent 7-T functional magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS) utilizing a visual checkerboard stimulation. Glutamate metabolite levels during rest versus stimulation were compared. Furthermore, correlates of the functional MRS response with performance in visuo-cognitive tests were investigated. RESULTS: No differences in static MRS between patients with PD and HC were detected, but a dynamic glutamate response was observed in functional MRS in HC upon visual stimulation, which was blunted in patients with PD (F1,22 = 7.13, p = 0.014; η p 2 = 0.245). A diminished glutamate response correlated with poorer performance in the Benton Judgment of Line Orientation test in PD (r = -0.57, p = 0.020). CONCLUSIONS: Our results indicate that functional MRS captures even subtle differences in neural processing linked to the behavioral performance, which would have been missed by conventional, static MRS. Functional MRS thus represents a promising tool for studying molecular alterations at high sensitivity. Its prognostic potential should be evaluated in longitudinal studies, prospectively contributing to earlier diagnosis and individual treatment decisions.


Assuntos
Disfunção Cognitiva , Doença de Parkinson , Processamento Espacial , Humanos , Ácido Glutâmico , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética/métodos , Espectroscopia de Ressonância Magnética , Disfunção Cognitiva/diagnóstico por imagem , Disfunção Cognitiva/etiologia , Disfunção Cognitiva/metabolismo
13.
Brain Topogr ; 36(6): 870-889, 2023 11.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37474691

RESUMO

Spatial reference frames (RFs) play a key role in spatial cognition, especially in perception, spatial memory, and navigation. There are two main types of RFs: egocentric (self-centered) and allocentric (object-centered). Although many fMRI studies examined the neural correlates of egocentric and allocentric RFs, they could not sample the fast temporal dynamics of the underlying cognitive processes. Therefore, the interaction and timing between these two RFs remain unclear. Taking advantage of the high temporal resolution of intracranial EEG (iEEG), we aimed to determine the timing of egocentric and allocentric information processing and describe the brain areas involved. We recorded iEEG and analyzed broad gamma activity (50-150 Hz) in 37 epilepsy patients performing a spatial judgment task in a three-dimensional circular virtual arena. We found overlapping activation for egocentric and allocentric RFs in many brain regions, with several additional egocentric- and allocentric-selective areas. In contrast to the egocentric responses, the allocentric responses peaked later than the control ones in frontal regions with overlapping selectivity. Also, across several egocentric or allocentric selective areas, the egocentric selectivity appeared earlier than the allocentric one. We identified the maximum number of egocentric-selective channels in the medial occipito-temporal region and allocentric-selective channels around the intraparietal sulcus in the parietal cortex. Our findings favor the hypothesis that egocentric spatial coding is a more primary process, and allocentric representations may be derived from egocentric ones. They also broaden the dominant view of the dorsal and ventral streams supporting egocentric and allocentric space coding, respectively.


Assuntos
Percepção Espacial , Processamento Espacial , Humanos , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Eletrocorticografia , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Julgamento/fisiologia
14.
Psychol Res ; 87(1): 242-259, 2023 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35192045

RESUMO

Arrows and gaze stimuli lead to opposite spatial congruency effects. While standard congruency effects are observed for arrows (faster responses for congruent conditions), responses are faster when eye-gaze stimuli are presented on the opposite side of the gazed-at location (incongruent trials), leading to a reversed congruency effect (RCE). Here, we explored the effects of implicit vs. explicit processing of arrows and eye-gaze direction. Participants were required to identify the direction (explicit task) or the colour (implicit task) of left or right looking/pointing gaze or arrows, presented to either the left or right of the fixation point. When participants responded to the direction of stimuli, standard congruency effects for arrows and RCE for eye-gaze stimuli were observed. However, when participants responded to the colour of stimuli, no congruency effects were observed. These results suggest that it is necessary to explicitly pay attention to the direction of eye-gaze and arrows for the congruency effect to occur. The same pattern of data was observed when participants responded either manually or verbally, demonstrating that manual motor components are not responsible for the results observed. These findings are not consistent with some hypotheses previously proposed to explain the RCE observed with eye-gaze stimuli and, therefore, call for an alternative plausible hypothesis.


Assuntos
Sinais (Psicologia) , Processamento Espacial , Humanos , Fixação Ocular , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia
15.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 117(47): 29390-29397, 2020 11 24.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33229557

RESUMO

Observations abound about the power of visual imagery in human intelligence, from how Nobel prize-winning physicists make their discoveries to how children understand bedtime stories. These observations raise an important question for cognitive science, which is, what are the computations taking place in someone's mind when they use visual imagery? Answering this question is not easy and will require much continued research across the multiple disciplines of cognitive science. Here, we focus on a related and more circumscribed question from the perspective of artificial intelligence (AI): If you have an intelligent agent that uses visual imagery-based knowledge representations and reasoning operations, then what kinds of problem solving might be possible, and how would such problem solving work? We highlight recent progress in AI toward answering these questions in the domain of visuospatial reasoning, looking at a case study of how imagery-based artificial agents can solve visuospatial intelligence tests. In particular, we first examine several variations of imagery-based knowledge representations and problem-solving strategies that are sufficient for solving problems from the Raven's Progressive Matrices intelligence test. We then look at how artificial agents, instead of being designed manually by AI researchers, might learn portions of their own knowledge and reasoning procedures from experience, including learning visuospatial domain knowledge, learning and generalizing problem-solving strategies, and learning the actual definition of the task in the first place.


Assuntos
Imaginação/fisiologia , Testes de Inteligência , Aprendizado de Máquina , Resolução de Problemas/fisiologia , Humanos , Modelos Psicológicos , Redes Neurais de Computação , Processamento Espacial/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia
16.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 117(11): 6163-6169, 2020 03 17.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32123077

RESUMO

It is well established that the adult brain contains a mosaic of domain-specific networks. But how do these domain-specific networks develop? Here we tested the hypothesis that the brain comes prewired with connections that precede the development of domain-specific function. Using resting-state fMRI in the youngest sample of newborn humans tested to date, we indeed found that cortical networks that will later develop strong face selectivity (including the "proto" occipital face area and fusiform face area) and scene selectivity (including the "proto" parahippocampal place area and retrosplenial complex) by adulthood, already show domain-specific patterns of functional connectivity as early as 27 d of age (beginning as early as 6 d of age). Furthermore, we asked how these networks are functionally connected to early visual cortex and found that the proto face network shows biased functional connectivity with foveal V1, while the proto scene network shows biased functional connectivity with peripheral V1. Given that faces are almost always experienced at the fovea, while scenes always extend across the entire periphery, these differential inputs may serve to facilitate domain-specific processing in each network after that function develops, or even guide the development of domain-specific function in each network in the first place. Taken together, these findings reveal domain-specific and eccentricity-biased connectivity in the earliest days of life, placing new constraints on our understanding of the origins of domain-specific cortical networks.


Assuntos
Córtex Cerebral/crescimento & desenvolvimento , Reconhecimento Facial/fisiologia , Rede Nervosa/fisiologia , Processamento Espacial/fisiologia , Adulto , Mapeamento Encefálico , Córtex Cerebral/diagnóstico por imagem , Feminino , Voluntários Saudáveis , Humanos , Lactente , Recém-Nascido , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
17.
Hum Factors ; 65(5): 956-965, 2023 08.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34292056

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: Determine whether the size-arrival effect (SAE) occurs with immersive, 3D visual experiences and active collision-avoidance responses. BACKGROUND: When a small near object and a large far object approach the observer at the same speeds, the large object appears to arrive before the small object, known as the size-arrival effect (SAE), which may contribute to crashes between motorcycles and cars. Prior studies of the SAE were limited because they used two dimensional displays and asked participants to make passive judgments. METHOD: Participants viewed approaching objects using a virtual reality (VR) headset. In an active task, participants ducked before the object hit them. In a passive prediction-motion (PM) judgment, the approaching object disappeared, and participants pressed a button when they thought the object would hit them. In a passive relative TTC judgment, participants reported which of two approaching objects would reach them first. RESULTS: The SAE occurred with the PM and relative TTC tasks but not with the ducking task. The SAE can occur in immersive 3D environments but is limited by the nature of the task and display. APPLICATION: Certain traffic situations may be more prone to the SAE and have higher risk for collisions. For example, in left-turn scenarios (e.g., see Levulis, 2018), drivers make passive judgments when oncoming vehicles are far and optical expansion is slow, and binocular disparity putatively is ineffective. Collision-avoidance warning systems may be needed more in such scenarios than when vehicles are near and drivers' judgments of TTC may be more accurate (DeLucia, 2008).


Assuntos
Condução de Veículo , Aprendizagem da Esquiva , Julgamento , Percepção de Movimento , Percepção Espacial , Processamento Espacial , Condução de Veículo/psicologia , Processamento Espacial/fisiologia , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Percepção de Movimento/fisiologia , Julgamento/fisiologia , Aprendizagem da Esquiva/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Feminino , Adolescente , Adulto Jovem , Adulto , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Imageamento Tridimensional , Realidade Virtual , Acidentes de Trânsito/prevenção & controle
18.
J Neurosci ; 41(47): 9720-9731, 2021 11 24.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34663627

RESUMO

It has been proposed that the auditory cortex in the deaf humans might undergo task-specific reorganization. However, evidence remains scarce as previous experiments used only two very specific tasks (temporal processing and face perception) in visual modality. Here, congenitally deaf/hard of hearing and hearing women and men were enrolled in an fMRI experiment as we sought to fill this evidence gap in two ways. First, we compared activation evoked by a temporal processing task performed in two different modalities, visual and tactile. Second, we contrasted this task with a perceptually similar task that focuses on the spatial dimension. Additional control conditions consisted of passive stimulus observation. In line with the task specificity hypothesis, the auditory cortex in the deaf was activated by temporal processing in both visual and tactile modalities. This effect was selective for temporal processing relative to spatial discrimination. However, spatial processing also led to significant auditory cortex recruitment which, unlike temporal processing, occurred even during passive stimulus observation. We conclude that auditory cortex recruitment in the deaf and hard of hearing might involve interplay between task-selective and pluripotential mechanisms of cross-modal reorganization. Our results open several avenues for the investigation of the full complexity of the cross-modal plasticity phenomenon.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Previous studies suggested that the auditory cortex in the deaf may change input modality (sound to vision) while keeping its function (e.g., rhythm processing). We investigated this hypothesis by asking deaf or hard of hearing and hearing adults to discriminate between temporally and spatially complex sequences in visual and tactile modalities. The results show that such function-specific brain reorganization, as has previously been demonstrated in the visual modality, also occurs for tactile processing. On the other hand, they also show that for some stimuli (spatial) the auditory cortex activates automatically, which is suggestive of a take-over by a different kind of cognitive function. The observed differences in processing of sequences might thus result from an interplay of task-specific and pluripotent plasticity.


Assuntos
Córtex Auditivo/fisiologia , Transtornos da Audição , Percepção do Tato/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Plasticidade Neuronal/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Estimulação Física/métodos , Processamento Espacial/fisiologia , Percepção do Tempo/fisiologia
19.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 34(4): 675-686, 2022 03 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35061032

RESUMO

The sense of touch is not restricted to the body but can also extend to external objects. When we use a handheld tool to contact an object, we feel the touch on the tool and not in the hand holding the tool. The ability to perceive touch on a tool actually extends along its entire surface, allowing the user to accurately localize where it is touched similarly as they would on their body. Although the neural mechanisms underlying the ability to localize touch on the body have been largely investigated, those allowing to localize touch on a tool are still unknown. We aimed to fill this gap by recording the electroencephalography signal of participants while they localized tactile stimuli on a handheld rod. We focused on oscillatory activity in the alpha (7-14 Hz) and beta (15-30 Hz) ranges, as they have been previously linked to distinct spatial codes used to localize touch on the body. Beta activity reflects the mapping of touch in skin-based coordinates, whereas alpha activity reflects the mapping of touch in external space. We found that alpha activity was solely modulated by the location of tactile stimuli applied on a handheld rod. Source reconstruction suggested that this alpha power modulation was localized in a network of fronto-parietal regions previously implicated in higher-order tactile and spatial processing. These findings are the first to implicate alpha oscillations in tool-extended sensing and suggest an important role for processing touch in external space when localizing touch on a tool.


Assuntos
Processamento Espacial , Percepção do Tato , Mãos , Humanos , Lobo Parietal , Percepção Espacial , Tato
20.
J Neurophysiol ; 127(1): 290-312, 2022 01 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34879207

RESUMO

The pitch of harmonic complex tones (HCTs) common in speech, music, and animal vocalizations plays a key role in the perceptual organization of sound. Unraveling the neural mechanisms of pitch perception requires animal models, but little is known about complex pitch perception by animals, and some species appear to use different pitch mechanisms than humans. Here, we tested rabbits' ability to discriminate the fundamental frequency (F0) of HCTs with missing fundamentals, using a behavioral paradigm inspired by foraging behavior in which rabbits learned to harness a spatial gradient in F0 to find the location of a virtual target within a room for a food reward. Rabbits were initially trained to discriminate HCTs with F0s in the range 400-800 Hz and with harmonics covering a wide frequency range (800-16,000 Hz) and then tested with stimuli differing in spectral composition to test the role of harmonic resolvability (experiment 1) or in F0 range (experiment 2) or in both F0 and spectral content (experiment 3). Together, these experiments show that rabbits can discriminate HCTs over a wide F0 range (200-1,600 Hz) encompassing the range of conspecific vocalizations and can use either the spectral pattern of harmonics resolved by the cochlea for higher F0s or temporal envelope cues resulting from interaction between unresolved harmonics for lower F0s. The qualitative similarity of these results to human performance supports the use of rabbits as an animal model for studies of pitch mechanisms, providing species differences in cochlear frequency selectivity and F0 range of vocalizations are taken into account.NEW & NOTEWORTHY Understanding the neural mechanisms of pitch perception requires experiments in animal models, but little is known about pitch perception by animals. Here we show that rabbits, a popular animal in auditory neuroscience, can discriminate complex sounds differing in pitch using either spectral cues or temporal cues. The results suggest that the role of spectral cues in pitch perception by animals may have been underestimated by predominantly testing low frequencies in the range of human voice.


Assuntos
Comportamento Animal/fisiologia , Sinais (Psicologia) , Discriminação Psicológica/fisiologia , Percepção da Altura Sonora/fisiologia , Processamento Espacial/fisiologia , Percepção do Tempo/fisiologia , Animais , Coelhos , Vocalização Animal/fisiologia
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA