Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 29
Filtrar
1.
Cereb Cortex ; 33(10): 5829-5838, 2023 05 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36482814

RESUMO

In the absence of hearing during development, the brain adapts and repurposes what was destined to become auditory cortex. As cortical thickness is commonly used as a proxy to identify cortical regions that have undergone plastic changes, the purpose of this investigation was to compare cortical thickness patterns between hearing and deaf cats. In this study, normal hearing (n = 29) and deaf (n = 26) cats were scanned to examine cortical thickness in hearing controls, as well as differential changes in thickness as a consequence of deafness. In hearing cats, a gradient in cortical thickness was identified across auditory cortex in which it is thinner in more dorsal regions and thicker in more ventral regions. Compared with hearing controls, differential thickening and thinning was observed in specific regions of deaf auditory cortex. More dorsal regions were found to be bilaterally thicker in the deaf group, while more ventral regions in the left hemisphere were thinner. The location and nature of these changes creates a gradient along the dorsoventral axis, wherein dorsal auditory cortical fields are thicker, whereas more ventral fields are thinner in deaf animals compared with hearing controls.


Assuntos
Córtex Auditivo , Surdez , Animais , Córtex Auditivo/diagnóstico por imagem , Surdez/diagnóstico por imagem , Substância Cinzenta/diagnóstico por imagem , Audição , Espessura Cortical do Cérebro , Plasticidade Neuronal
2.
J Neurosci ; 38(16): 4048-4058, 2018 04 18.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29610441

RESUMO

Following the loss of a sensory modality, such as deafness or blindness, crossmodal plasticity is commonly identified in regions of the cerebrum that normally process the deprived modality. It has been hypothesized that significant changes in the patterns of cortical afferent and efferent projections may underlie these functional crossmodal changes. However, studies of thalamocortical and corticocortical connections have refuted this hypothesis, instead revealing a profound resilience of cortical afferent projections following deafness and blindness. This report is the first study of cortical outputs following sensory deprivation, characterizing cortical projections to the superior colliculus in mature cats (N = 5, 3 female) with perinatal-onset deafness. The superior colliculus was exposed to a retrograde pathway tracer, and subsequently labeled cells throughout the cerebrum were identified and quantified. Overall, the percentage of cortical projections arising from auditory cortex was substantially increased, not decreased, in early-deaf cats compared with intact animals. Furthermore, the distribution of labeled cortical neurons was no longer localized to a particular cortical subregion of auditory cortex but dispersed across auditory cortical regions. Collectively, these results demonstrate that, although patterns of cortical afferents are stable following perinatal deafness, the patterns of cortical efferents to the superior colliculus are highly mutable.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT When a sense is lost, the remaining senses are functionally enhanced through compensatory crossmodal plasticity. In deafness, brain regions that normally process sound contribute to enhanced visual and somatosensory perception. We demonstrate that hearing loss alters connectivity between sensory cortex and the superior colliculus, a midbrain region that integrates sensory representations to guide orientation behavior. Contrasting expectation, the proportion of projections from auditory cortex increased in deaf animals compared with normal hearing, with a broad distribution across auditory fields. This is the first description of changes in cortical efferents following sensory loss and provides support for models predicting an inability to form a coherent, multisensory percept of the environment following periods of abnormal development.


Assuntos
Córtex Auditivo/fisiopatologia , Surdez/fisiopatologia , Colículos Superiores/fisiopatologia , Animais , Córtex Auditivo/patologia , Vias Auditivas/patologia , Vias Auditivas/fisiopatologia , Gatos , Surdez/patologia , Feminino , Masculino , Neurônios Eferentes/patologia , Colículos Superiores/patologia
3.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 31(8): 1091-1109, 2019 08.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31112472

RESUMO

Over the past decade, there has been an unprecedented level of interest and progress into understanding visual processing in the brain of the deaf. Specifically, when the brain is deprived of input from one sensory modality (such as hearing), it often compensates with supranormal performance in one or more of the intact sensory systems (such as vision). Recent psychophysical, functional imaging, and reversible deactivation studies have converged to define the specific visual abilities that are enhanced in the deaf, as well as the cortical loci that undergo crossmodal plasticity in the deaf and are responsible for mediating these superior visual functions. Examination of these investigations reveals that central visual functions, such as object and facial discrimination, and peripheral visual functions, such as motion detection, visual localization, visuomotor synchronization, and Vernier acuity (measured in the periphery), are specifically enhanced in the deaf, compared with hearing participants. Furthermore, the cortical loci identified to mediate these functions reside in deaf auditory cortex: BA 41, BA 42, and BA 22, in addition to the rostral area, planum temporale, Te3, and temporal voice area in humans; primary auditory cortex, anterior auditory field, dorsal zone of auditory cortex, auditory field of the anterior ectosylvian sulcus, and posterior auditory field in cats; and primary auditory cortex and anterior auditory field in both ferrets and mice. Overall, the findings from these studies show that crossmodal reorganization in auditory cortex of the deaf is responsible for the superior visual abilities of the deaf.


Assuntos
Córtex Auditivo/fisiopatologia , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Perda Auditiva/fisiopatologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Adulto , Animais , Criança , Humanos
4.
Neuroimage ; 165: 69-82, 2018 01 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28988830

RESUMO

Normal brain development depends on early sensory experience. Behavioral consequences of brain maturation in the absence of sensory input early in life are well documented. For example, experiments with mature, neonatally deaf human or animal subjects have revealed improved peripheral visual motion detection and spatial localization abilities. Such supranormal behavioral abilities in the nondeprived sensory modality are evidence of compensatory plasticity occurring in deprived brain regions at some point or throughout development. Sensory deprived brain regions may simply become unused neural real-estate resulting in a loss of function. Compensatory plasticity and loss of function are likely reflected in the differences in correlations between brain networks in deaf compared with hearing subjects. To address this, we used resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in lightly anesthetized hearing and neonatally deafened cats. Group independent component analysis (ICA) was used to identify 20 spatially distinct brain networks across all animals including auditory, visual, somatosensory, cingulate, insular, cerebellar, and subcortical networks. The resulting group ICA components were back-reconstructed to individual animal brains. The maximum correlations between the time-courses associated with each spatial component were computed using functional network connectivity (FNC). While no significant differences in the delay to peak correlations were identified between hearing and deaf cats, we observed 10 (of 190) significant differences in the amplitudes of between-network correlations. Six of the significant differences involved auditory-related networks and four involved visual, cingulate, or somatosensory networks. The results are discussed in context of known behavioral, electrophysiological, and anatomical differences following neonatal deafness. Furthermore, these results identify novel targets for future investigations at the neuronal level.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/fisiopatologia , Surdez/fisiopatologia , Vias Neurais/fisiopatologia , Animais , Animais Recém-Nascidos , Gatos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética
5.
Neuroimage ; 127: 44-57, 2016 Feb 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26658927

RESUMO

Sensory systems are typically constructed in a hierarchical fashion such that lower level subcortical and cortical areas process basic stimulus features, while higher level areas reassemble these features into object-level representations. A number of anatomical pathway tracing studies have suggested that the auditory cortical hierarchy of the cat extends from a core region, consisting of the primary auditory cortex (A1) and the anterior auditory field (AAF), to higher level auditory fields that are located ventrally. Unfortunately, limitations on electrophysiological examination of these higher level fields have resulted in an incomplete understanding of the functional organization of the auditory cortex. Thus, the current study uses functional MRI in conjunction with a variety of simple and complex auditory stimuli to provide the first comprehensive examination of function across the entire cortical hierarchy. Auditory cortex function is shown to be largely lateralized to the left hemisphere, and is concentrated bilaterally in fields surrounding the posterior ectosylvian sulcus. The use of narrowband noise stimuli enables the visualization of tonotopic gradients in the posterior auditory field (PAF) and ventral posterior auditory field (VPAF) that have previously been unverifiable using fMRI and pure tones. Furthermore, auditory fields that are inaccessible to more invasive techniques, such as the insular (IN) and temporal (T) cortices, are shown to be selectively responsive to vocalizations. Collectively, these data provide a much needed functional correlate for anatomical examinations of the hierarchy of cortical structures within the cat auditory cortex.


Assuntos
Córtex Auditivo/anatomia & histologia , Córtex Auditivo/fisiologia , Gatos/anatomia & histologia , Gatos/fisiologia , Estimulação Acústica , Animais , Vias Auditivas/anatomia & histologia , Vias Auditivas/fisiologia , Mapeamento Encefálico , Processamento de Imagem Assistida por Computador , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética
6.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 27(5): 1060-7, 2015 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25436670

RESUMO

Sound waves emitted by two or more simultaneous sources reach the ear as one complex waveform. Auditory scene analysis involves parsing a complex waveform into separate perceptual representations of the sound sources [Bregman, A. S. Auditory scene analysis: The perceptual organization of sounds. London: MIT Press, 1990]. Harmonicity provides an important cue for auditory scene analysis. Normally, harmonics at integer multiples of a fundamental frequency are perceived as one sound with a pitch corresponding to the fundamental frequency. However, when one harmonic in such a complex, pitch-evoking sound is sufficiently mistuned, that harmonic emerges from the complex tone and is perceived as a separate auditory object. Previous work has shown that the percept of two objects is indexed in both children and adults by the object-related negativity component of the ERP derived from EEG recordings [Alain, C., Arnott, S. T., & Picton, T. W. Bottom-up and top-down influences on auditory scene analysis: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 1072-1089, 2001]. Here we examine the emergence of object-related responses to an 8% harmonic mistuning in infants between 2 and 12 months of age. Two-month-old infants showed no significant object-related response. However, in 4- to 12-month-old infants, a significant frontally positive component was present, and by 8-12 months, a significant frontocentral object-related negativity was present, similar to that seen in older children and adults. This is in accordance with previous research demonstrating that infants younger than 4 months of age do not integrate harmonic information to perceive pitch when the fundamental is missing [He, C., Hotson, L., & Trainor, L. J. Maturation of cortical mismatch mismatch responses to occasional pitch change in early infancy: Effects of presentation rate and magnitude of change. Neuropsychologia, 47, 218-229, 2009]. The results indicate that the ability to use harmonic information to segregate simultaneous sounds emerges at the cortical level between 2 and 4 months of age.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/fisiologia , Córtex Auditivo/fisiologia , Mapeamento Encefálico , Potenciais Evocados Auditivos/fisiologia , Percepção da Altura Sonora/fisiologia , Estimulação Acústica , Fatores Etários , Eletroencefalografia , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino , Matemática
7.
Brain Struct Funct ; 2024 Jul 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38981886

RESUMO

The cerebral cortex comprises many distinct regions that differ in structure, function, and patterns of connectivity. Current approaches to parcellating these regions often take advantage of functional neuroimaging approaches that can identify regions involved in a particular process with reasonable spatial resolution. However, neuroanatomical biomarkers are also very useful in identifying distinct cortical regions either in addition to, or in place of functional measures. For example, differences in myelin density are thought to relate to functional differences between regions, are sensitive to individual patterns of experience, and have been shown to vary across functional hierarchies in a predictable manner. Accordingly, the current study provides quantitative stereological estimates of myelin density for each of the 13 regions that make up the feline auditory cortex. We demonstrate that significant differences can be observed between auditory cortical regions, with the highest myelin density observed in the regions that comprise the auditory core (i.e., the primary auditory cortex and anterior auditory field). Moreover, our myeloarchitectonic map suggests that myelin density varies in a hierarchical fashion that conforms to the traditional model of spatial organization in auditory cortex. Taken together, these results establish myelin as a useful biomarker for parcellating auditory cortical regions, and provide detailed estimates against which other, less invasive methods of quantifying cortical myelination may be compared.

8.
Brain Cogn ; 83(3): 271-8, 2013 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24095845

RESUMO

Pitch is derived by the auditory system through complex spectrotemporal processing. Pitch extraction is thought to depend on both spectral cues arising from lower harmonics that are resolved by cochlear filters in the inner ear, and on temporal cues arising from the pattern of action potentials contained in the cochlear output. Adults are capable of extracting pitch in the absence of robust spectral cues, taking advantage of the temporal cues that remain. However, recent behavioral evidence suggests that infants have difficulty discriminating between stimuli with different pitches when resolvable spectral cues are absent. In the current experiments, we used the mismatch negativity (MMN) component of the event related potential derived from electroencephalographic (EEG) recordings to examine a cortical representation of pitch discrimination for iterated rippled noise (IRN) stimuli in 4- and 8-month-old infants. IRN stimuli are pitch-evoking sounds generated by repeatedly adding a segment of white noise to itself at a constant delay. We created IRN stimuli (delays of 5 and 6ms creating pitch percepts of 200 and 167Hz) and high-pass filtered them to remove all resolvable spectral pitch cues. In experiment 1, we did not find EEG evidence that infants could detect the change in the pitch of these IRN stimuli. However, in Experiment 2, after a brief period of pitch-priming during which we added a sine wave component to the IRN stimulus at its perceived pitch, infants did show significant MMN in response to pitch changes in the IRN stimuli with sine waves removed. This suggests that (1) infants can use temporal cues to process pitch, although such processing is not mature and (2) that a short amount of pitch-priming experience can alter pitch representations in auditory cortex during infancy.


Assuntos
Córtex Auditivo/fisiologia , Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Orelha Interna/fisiologia , Potenciais Evocados/fisiologia , Discriminação da Altura Tonal/fisiologia , Fatores Etários , Eletroencefalografia , Potenciais Evocados Auditivos/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino , Fatores de Tempo
9.
Sci Rep ; 13(1): 15328, 2023 09 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37714887

RESUMO

Sensory and language experience can affect brain organization and domain-general abilities. For example, D/deaf individuals show superior visual perception compared to hearing controls in several domains, including the perception of faces and peripheral motion. While these enhancements may result from sensory loss and subsequent neural plasticity, they may also reflect experience using a visual-manual language, like American Sign Language (ASL), where signers must process moving hand signs and facial cues simultaneously. In an effort to disentangle these concurrent sensory experiences, we examined how learning sign language influences visual abilities by comparing bimodal bilinguals (i.e., sign language users with typical hearing) and hearing non-signers. Bimodal bilinguals and hearing non-signers completed online psychophysical measures of face matching and biological motion discrimination. No significant group differences were observed across these two tasks, suggesting that sign language experience is insufficient to induce perceptual advantages in typical-hearing adults. However, ASL proficiency (but not years of experience or age of acquisition) was found to predict performance on the motion perception task among bimodal bilinguals. Overall, the results presented here highlight a need for more nuanced study of how linguistic environments, sensory experience, and cognitive functions impact broad perceptual processes and underlying neural correlates.


Assuntos
Percepção de Movimento , Língua de Sinais , Adulto , Humanos , Idioma , Audição , Encéfalo
10.
Emotion ; 23(4): 1088-1101, 2023 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35980688

RESUMO

Emotional stimuli can disrupt or enhance task performance according to factors that are presently poorly understood. One potentially important determinant is the sensory modality involved. In unimodal visual paradigms (visual task-irrelevant stimuli during a visual task) emotional stimuli frequently produce distraction effects; however, the effects across modalities appear more complex and may also depend on factors related to stimulus timing. It is entirely unclear how task-irrelevant visual stimuli impact auditory task performance in cross-modal paradigms. This project explored task performance as a function of sensory modality, emotional valence, and stimulus timing. In Study 1, participants (N = 50) completed a visual stimulus detection task in the presence of task-irrelevant negative and neutral images and sounds. Accuracy was disrupted in the presence of visual but not auditory emotional stimuli, particularly when the target and task-irrelevant stimulus appeared simultaneously. In Study 2, participants (N = 38) completed an equivalent auditory stimulus detection task. In sharp contrast to the effects observed with visual targets, response times and accuracy were enhanced in the presence of auditory emotional stimuli at the first timepoint but disrupted at later timepoints. However, there was no effect of task-irrelevant visual stimuli on auditory task performance. These findings demonstrate the importance of both sensory modality and timing in determining how emotional stimuli affect task performance and lay the groundwork for future studies examining the interaction between emotional and attentional processes. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).


Assuntos
Atenção , Emoções , Humanos , Atenção/fisiologia , Emoções/fisiologia , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Estimulação Acústica/métodos , Som , Percepção Auditiva/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Percepção Visual/fisiologia
11.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 131(1): 993-7, 2012 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22280722

RESUMO

The ability to separate simultaneous auditory objects is crucial to infant auditory development. Music in particular relies on the ability to separate musical notes, chords, and melodic lines. Little research addresses how infants process simultaneous sounds. The present study used a conditioned head-turn procedure to examine whether 6-month-old infants are able to discriminate a complex tone (240 Hz, 500 ms, six harmonics in random phase with a 6 dB roll-off per octave) from a version with the third harmonic mistuned. Adults perceive such stimuli as containing two auditory objects, one with the pitch of the mistuned harmonic and the other with pitch corresponding to the fundamental of the complex tone. Adult thresholds were between 1% and 2% mistuning. Infants performed above chance levels for 8%, 6%, and 4% mistunings, with no significant difference between conditions. However, performance was not significantly different from chance for 2% mistuning and significantly worse for 2% compared to all larger mistunings. These results indicate that 6-month-old infants are sensitive to violations of harmonic structure and suggest that they are able to separate two simultaneously sounding objects.


Assuntos
Limiar Auditivo/fisiologia , Discriminação da Altura Tonal/fisiologia , Estimulação Acústica , Acústica , Análise de Variância , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino , Mascaramento Perceptivo/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
12.
Neuropsychologia ; 174: 108336, 2022 09 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35872233

RESUMO

Integrating sensory information from multiple modalities leads to more precise and efficient perception and behaviour. The process of determining which sensory information should be perceptually bound is reliant on both low-level stimulus features, as well as multisensory associations learned throughout development based on the statistics of our environment. Here, we explored the relationship between multisensory associative learning and multisensory integration using encephalography (EEG) and behavioural measures. Sixty-one participants completed a three-phase study. First, participants were exposed to novel audiovisual shape-tone pairings with frequent and infrequent stimulus pairings and completed a target detection task. EEG recordings of the mismatch negativity (MMN) and P3 were calculated as neural indices of multisensory associative learning. Next, the same learned stimulus pairs were presented in audiovisual as well as unisensory auditory and visual modalities while both early (<100 ms) and late neural indices of multisensory integration were recorded. Finally, participants completed an analogous behavioural speeded-response task, with behavioural indices of multisensory gain calculated using the Race Model. Significant relationships were found in fronto-central and occipital areas between neural measures of associative learning and both early and late indices of multisensory integration in frontal and centro-parietal areas, respectively. Participants who showed stronger indices of associative learning also exhibited stronger indices of multisensory integration of the stimuli they learned to associate. Furthermore, a significant relationship was found between neural index of early multisensory integration and behavioural indices of multisensory gain. These results provide insight into the neural underpinnings of how higher-order processes such as associative learning guide multisensory integration.


Assuntos
Percepção Auditiva , Percepção Visual , Estimulação Acústica , Percepção Auditiva/fisiologia , Condicionamento Clássico , Humanos , Estimulação Luminosa , Percepção Visual/fisiologia
13.
Int J Audiol ; 50(8): 530-9, 2011 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21751943

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: The purpose of this study was to evaluate changes in distortion product otoacoustic emission (DPOAE) level elicited by contralateral noise in children with normal hearing, and those with auditory processing disorders (APD) whose audiometric thresholds were normal. It was hypothesized that children with APD would demonstrate smaller changes. DESIGN: Levels of DPOAEs were recorded for f(2) stimulus tones fixed at 2, 3, and 4 kHz while the f(1) tone was ramped around nominal stimulus frequency ratios of f(2)/f(1) = 1.22 and 1.10. Mean and maximum absolute changes resulting from contralateral broadband noise presented at 60 dB SPL were evaluated across the DPOAE frequency bands for each individual and for both groups of subjects. STUDY SAMPLE: Eight normal-hearing children and eight children with APD whose audiometric thresholds were normal participated. RESULTS: There were no significant differences in DPOAE inhibition between normal hearing and APD groups, or previously recorded adult data. Mean absolute changes were typically near 1 dB, except for f(2) = 4 kHz and the stimulus frequency ratio 1.22 where inhibition was only 0.5 dB. However, there were individual children in both groups who demonstrated larger DPOAE changes for some stimulus parameters. CONCLUSIONS: The inhibition of otoacoustic emissions requires further study in APD children.


Assuntos
Vias Auditivas/fisiopatologia , Transtornos da Percepção Auditiva/fisiopatologia , Emissões Otoacústicas Espontâneas , Estimulação Acústica , Adolescente , Audiometria , Limiar Auditivo , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Criança , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
14.
Brain Struct Funct ; 226(7): 2019-2039, 2021 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34100151

RESUMO

Many aging adults experience some form of hearing problems that may arise from auditory peripheral damage. However, it has been increasingly acknowledged that hearing loss is not only a dysfunction of the auditory periphery but also results from changes within the entire auditory system, from periphery to cortex. Damage to the auditory periphery is associated with an increase in neural activity at various stages throughout the auditory pathway. Here, we review neurophysiological evidence of hyperactivity, auditory perceptual difficulties that may result from hyperactivity, and outline open conceptual and methodological questions related to the study of hyperactivity. We suggest that hyperactivity alters all aspects of hearing-including spectral, temporal, spatial hearing-and, in turn, impairs speech comprehension when background sound is present. By focusing on the perceptual consequences of hyperactivity and the potential challenges of investigating hyperactivity in humans, we hope to bring animal and human electrophysiologists closer together to better understand hearing problems in older adulthood.


Assuntos
Perda Auditiva , Plasticidade Neuronal , Estimulação Acústica , Animais , Vias Auditivas , Surdez , Testes Auditivos , Humanos , Percepção da Fala
15.
J Neurosci Methods ; 334: 108603, 2020 Jan 23.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31982459

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Neuroimaging methods including fMRI provide powerful tools to observe whole-brain functional networks. This is particularly powerful in animal models, allowing these networks to be probed using complementary methods. However, most animals must be anesthetized for neuroimaging, giving rise to complications resulting from anesthetic effects on the animal's physiological and neurological functions. For example, an established protocol for feline neuroimaging involves co-administration of ketamine and isoflurane - the latter of which is known to suppress cortical function. NEW METHOD: Here, we compare this established protocol to alfaxalone, a single-agent anesthetic for functional neuroimaging. We first compare the two in a controlled environment to assess relative safety and to measure physiological stability over an extended time window. We then compare patterns of auditory and visually-evoked activity measured at 7  T to assess mean signal strength and between-subjects signal variability. RESULTS IN COMPARISON WITH EXISTING METHODS: We show that alfaxalone results in more stable respiratory rates over the 120 min testing period, with evidence of smaller between-measurements variability within this time window, when compared to ketamine plus isoflurane. Moreover, we demonstrate that both agents evoke similar mean BOLD signals across animals, but that alfaxalone elicits more consistent BOLD activity in response to sound stimuli across all ROIs observed. CONCLUSIONS: Alfaxalone is observed to be more physiologically stable, evoking a more consistent BOLD signal across animals than the co-administration of ketamine and isoflurane. Thus, an alfaxalone-based protocol may represent a better approach for neuroimaging in animal models requiring anesthesia.

16.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 124(4): 2133-48, 2008 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19062854

RESUMO

The purpose of this research was to investigate the changes that occur in human distortion product otoacoustic emission (DPOAE) level functions over continuous frequency bands in response to activation of the medial olivocochlear (MOC) efferent system by contralateral broadband noise. DPOAEs were obtained using continuous upward ramps of the lower frequency tone (f(1)) while the higher frequency tone (f(2)) was fixed. These ramps were designed to change the stimulus frequency ratio f(2)/f(1) over a fixed range for each fixed f(2) value of 2, 3, and 4 kHz. Contralateral noise was presented on alternating ramps and the DPOAEs with and without contralateral noise were averaged separately. Stimulus frequency ratios of 1.10 and 1.22, and noise levels of 60 and 50 dB sound pressure level (SPL) were employed. Changes in DPOAE level were generally suppression (a reduction in DPOAE magnitude), but enhancement was also observed. For most participants, changes were evident for much of the frequency ranges tested. Average absolute changes for 60 dB SPL noise were 0.95, 0.81, and 0.42 dB for the wider stimulus frequency ratios and f(2) of 2, 3, and 4 kHz, respectively. For the narrower ratio and 60 dB SPL noise, the changes were larger with average absolute changes of 1.33, 1.09, and 0.87 dB. For the narrower ratio and 50 dB SPL noise, the changes were 1.08, 0.78, and 0.55 dB with f(2) of 2, 3, and 4 kHz, respectively. DPOAE nulls were observed and a common response pattern was a shift of emission morphology to higher frequencies with contralateral acoustic stimulation. The method appears promising for relatively rapid evaluation of the MOC efferent system in humans and offers information complementary to measurement strategies that explore the effects of stimulus level.


Assuntos
Estimulação Acústica , Cóclea/inervação , Núcleo Olivar/fisiologia , Emissões Otoacústicas Espontâneas , Percepção da Altura Sonora , Adulto , Limiar Auditivo , Vias Eferentes/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Ruído , Mascaramento Perceptivo , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Adulto Jovem
17.
Brain Struct Funct ; 223(2): 819-835, 2018 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28940055

RESUMO

It has been well established that following sensory loss, cortical areas that would normally be involved in perceiving stimuli in the absent modality are recruited to subserve the remaining senses. Despite this compensatory functional reorganization, there is little evidence to date for any substantial change in the patterns of anatomical connectivity between sensory cortices. However, while many auditory areas are contracted in the deaf, the second auditory cortex (A2) of the cat undergoes a volumetric expansion following hearing loss, suggesting this cortical area may demonstrate a region-specific pattern of structural reorganization. To address this hypothesis, and to complement existing literature on connectivity within auditory cortex, we injected a retrograde neuronal tracer across the breadth and cortical thickness of A2 to provide the first comprehensive quantification of projections from cortical and thalamic auditory and non-auditory regions to the second auditory cortex, and to determine how these patterns are affected by the onset of deafness. Neural projections arising from auditory, visual, somatomotor, and limbic cortices, as well as thalamic nuclei, were compared across normal hearing, early-deaf, and late-deaf animals. The results demonstrate that, despite previously identified changes in A2 volume, the pattern of projections into this cortical region are unaffected by the onset of hearing loss. These results fail to support the idea that crossmodal plasticity reflects changes in the pattern of projections between cortical regions and provides evidence that the pattern of connectivity that supports normal hearing is retained in the deaf brain.


Assuntos
Córtex Auditivo/patologia , Vias Auditivas/patologia , Surdez/patologia , Tálamo/patologia , Estimulação Acústica , Fatores Etários , Animais , Animais Recém-Nascidos , Biotina/análogos & derivados , Biotina/metabolismo , Mapeamento Encefálico , Gatos , Dextranos/metabolismo , Modelos Animais de Doenças , Potenciais Evocados Auditivos do Tronco Encefálico/fisiologia , Técnicas de Rastreamento Neuroanatômico , Fatores de Tempo
18.
Hear Res ; 343: 118-127, 2017 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27306930

RESUMO

Crossmodal plasticity takes place following sensory loss, such that areas that normally process the missing modality are reorganized to provide compensatory function in the remaining sensory systems. For example, congenitally deaf cats outperform normal hearing animals on localization of visual stimuli presented in the periphery, and this advantage has been shown to be mediated by the posterior auditory field (PAF). In order to determine the nature of the anatomical differences that underlie this phenomenon, we injected a retrograde tracer into PAF of congenitally deaf animals and quantified the thalamic and cortical projections to this field. The pattern of projections from areas throughout the brain was determined to be qualitatively similar to that previously demonstrated in normal hearing animals, but with twice as many projections arising from non-auditory cortical areas. In addition, small ectopic projections were observed from a number of fields in visual cortex, including areas 19, 20a, 20b, and 21b, and area 7 of parietal cortex. These areas did not show projections to PAF in cats deafened ototoxically near the onset of hearing, and provide a possible mechanism for crossmodal reorganization of PAF. These, along with the possible contributions of other mechanisms, are considered.


Assuntos
Córtex Auditivo/fisiopatologia , Surdez/fisiopatologia , Audição , Plasticidade Neuronal , Tálamo/fisiopatologia , Adaptação Fisiológica , Adaptação Psicológica , Animais , Vias Auditivas/fisiopatologia , Percepção Auditiva , Comportamento Animal , Gatos , Surdez/congênito , Surdez/psicologia , Modelos Animais de Doenças , Feminino , Técnicas de Rastreamento Neuroanatômico , Córtex Visual/fisiopatologia , Percepção Visual
19.
J Comp Neurol ; 525(15): 3190-3206, 2017 Oct 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28653335

RESUMO

Brain atlases play an important role in effectively communicating results from neuroimaging studies in a standardized coordinate system. Furthermore, brain atlases extend analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) data by delineating regions of interest over which to evaluate the extent of functional activation as well as measures of inter-regional connectivity. Here, we introduce a three-dimensional atlas of the cat cerebral cortex based on established cytoarchitectonic and electrophysiological findings. In total, 71 cerebral areas were mapped onto the gray matter (GM) of an averaged T1-weighted structural MRI acquired at 7 T from eight adult domestic cats. In addition, a nonlinear registration procedure was used to generate a common template brain as well as GM, white matter, and cerebral spinal fluid tissue probability maps to facilitate tissue segmentation as part of the standard preprocessing pipeline for MRI data analysis. The atlas and associated files can also be used for planning stereotaxic surgery and for didactic purposes.


Assuntos
Atlas como Assunto , Encéfalo/anatomia & histologia , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagem , Gatos/anatomia & histologia , Imageamento Tridimensional , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Algoritmos , Animais , Líquido Cefalorraquidiano/diagnóstico por imagem , Feminino , Substância Cinzenta/anatomia & histologia , Substância Cinzenta/diagnóstico por imagem , Imageamento Tridimensional/métodos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética/métodos , Vias Neurais/anatomia & histologia , Vias Neurais/diagnóstico por imagem , Procedimentos Neurocirúrgicos , Dinâmica não Linear , Reconhecimento Automatizado de Padrão/métodos , Substância Branca/anatomia & histologia , Substância Branca/diagnóstico por imagem
20.
Curr Biol ; 26(22): R1185-R1187, 2016 11 21.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27875697

RESUMO

Two recent studies have independently demonstrated that short periods of visual deprivation early in human development can have long-term functional consequences on sensory perception and on the balance between auditory and visual attention.


Assuntos
Percepção Auditiva , Percepção Visual , Atenção , Humanos , Privação Sensorial , Visão Ocular
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA