Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 55
Filtrar
1.
Arch Sex Behav ; 53(1): 205-211, 2024 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37415027

RESUMO

The fraternal birth order effect (FBOE) is the phenomenon whereby the probability that a man has a same-sex sexual orientation in adulthood increases with each biological older brother. Several studies have found evidence that the FBOE is limited to right-handed men, and left-handed men do not show an FBOE. Recent debates about the appropriate methods for quantifying the FBOE center on distinguishing the FBOE from other effects, such as the female fecundity effect (FFE), whereby mothers more prone to bearing gay sons are also more fecund. The FBOE and FFE are confounded in that a real FFE will result in data consistent with the FBOE under some analyses. Here, we applied some recent proposed analytic methods for the FBOE to the property of handedness. A straightforward application of Khovanova's technique to the binary trait of handedness yielded support for a fraternal birth order effect consistent with the maternal immune hypothesis, in that the ratios of handedness differed between men with one older brother only, and men with one younger brother only, while no such effect was seen in women. This effect was not seen, however, when the confounding effects of parental age were controlled for. Models including factors to simultaneously test multiple posited effects find significant female fecundity effects, as well as paternal age and birth order effects on handedness in men, but no FBOE. The effects seen in women were different, with no fecundity or parental age effects, but birth order and sex of older siblings had effects. We conclude, based on this evidence, that many of the factors thought to contribute to sexual orientation in men may also have an influence on handedness, and further note that parental age is a potential confound which may be overlooked by some analyses of the FBOE.


Assuntos
Ordem de Nascimento , Homossexualidade Masculina , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Lateralidade Funcional , Irmãos , Comportamento Sexual
2.
Gen Comp Endocrinol ; 296: 113538, 2020 09 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32585214

RESUMO

A fit animal must develop testes or ovaries, with brain and physiology to match. In species with alternative male morphs this coordination of development across tissues operates within sexes as well as between. For Pelvicachromis pulcher, an African cichlid in which early pH exposure influences both sex and alternative male morph, we sequence both copies of aromatase (cyp19a1), a key gene for sex determination. We analyze gene expression and epigenetic state, comparing gonad and brain tissue from females, alternative male morphs, and fry. Relative to brain, we find elevated expression of the A-copy in the ovaries but not testes. Methylation analysis suggests strong epigenetic regulation, with one region specifying sex and another specifying tissue. We find elevated brain expression of the B-copy with no sex or male morph differences. B-copy methylation follows that of the A-copy rather than corresponding to B-copy expression. In 30-day old fry, we see elevated B-copy expression in the head, but we do not see the expected elevated A-copy expression in the trunk that would reflect ovarian development. Interestingly, the A-copy epialleles that distinguish ovaries from testes are among the most explanatory patterns for variation among fry, suggesting epigenetic marking of sex prior to differentiation and thus laying the groundwork for mechanistic studies of epigenetic regulation of sex and morph differentiation.


Assuntos
Aromatase/genética , Encéfalo/enzimologia , Ciclídeos/genética , Epigênese Genética , Gônadas/enzimologia , Processos de Determinação Sexual/genética , Animais , Aromatase/metabolismo , Metilação de DNA/genética , Feminino , Regulação da Expressão Gênica no Desenvolvimento , Regulação Enzimológica da Expressão Gênica , Masculino , Análise de Componente Principal , Regiões Promotoras Genéticas/genética , RNA Mensageiro/genética , Diferenciação Sexual/genética
3.
J Neurophysiol ; 120(1): 250-262, 2018 07 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29589816

RESUMO

The cerebellum is organized into parasagittal zones defined by its climbing and mossy fiber inputs, efferent projections, and Purkinje cell (PC) response properties. Additionally, parasagittal stripes can be visualized with molecular markers, such as heterogeneous expression of the isoenzyme zebrin II (ZII), where sagittal stripes of high ZII expression (ZII+) are interdigitated with stripes of low ZII expression (ZII-). In the pigeon vestibulocerebellum, a ZII+/- stripe pair represents a functional unit, insofar as both ZII+ and ZII- PCs within a stripe pair respond best to the same pattern of optic flow. In the present study, we attempted to determine whether there were any differences in the responses between ZII+ and ZII- PCs within a functional unit in response to optic flow stimuli. In pigeons of either sex, we recorded complex spike activity (CSA) from PCs in response to optic flow, marked recording sites with a fluorescent tracer, and determined the ZII identity of recorded PCs by immunohistochemistry. We found that CSA of ZII+ PCs showed a greater depth of modulation in response to the preferred optic flow pattern compared with ZII- PCs. We suggest that these differences in the depth of modulation to optic flow stimuli are due to differences in the connectivity of ZII+ and ZII- PCs within a functional unit. Specifically, ZII+ PCs project to areas of the vestibular nuclei that provide inhibitory feedback to the inferior olive, whereas ZII- PCs do not. NEW & NOTEWORTHY Although the cerebellum appears to be a uniform structure, Purkinje cells (PCs) are heterogeneous and can be categorized on the basis of the expression of molecular markers. These phenotypes are conserved across species, but the significance is undetermined. PCs in the vestibulocerebellum encode optic flow resulting from self-motion, and those that express the molecular marker zebrin II (ZII+) exhibit more sensitivity to optic flow than those that do not express zebrin II (ZII-).


Assuntos
Potenciais de Ação , Proteínas do Tecido Nervoso/metabolismo , Células de Purkinje/fisiologia , Animais , Columbidae , Feminino , Masculino , Proteínas do Tecido Nervoso/genética , Fluxo Óptico , Células de Purkinje/metabolismo
4.
Anim Cogn ; 20(3): 537-551, 2017 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28324234

RESUMO

Cerebral lateralization, the partitioning of functions into a certain hemisphere of the brain, is ubiquitous among vertebrates. Evidence suggests that the cognitive processing of a stimulus is performed with a specific hemisphere depending in part upon the emotional valence of the stimulus (i.e. whether it is appetitive or aversive). Recent work has implicated a predominance of right-hemisphere processing for aversive stimuli. In fish with laterally placed eyes, the preference to view an object with a specific eye has been used as a proxy for assessing cerebral lateralization. The habenula, one of the most well-known examples of an asymmetrical neural structure, has been linked to behavioural asymmetry in some fish species. Here, we exposed convict cichlid fish (Amatitlania nigrofasciata) to both a social and non-social lateralization task and assessed behavioural lateralization in either the presence or absence of an aversive stimulus, damage-induced alarm cues. We also assessed whether behavioural asymmetry in these tests was related to asymmetry of the habenular nuclei. We found that when alarm cues were present, fish showed increased left-eye (and by proxy, right hemisphere) preference for stimulus viewing. In addition, females, but not males, showed stronger eye preferences when alarm cues were present. We did not find a relationship between behavioural lateralization and habenular lateralization. Our results conflict with previous reports of concordance between behavioural and habenular lateralization in this fish species. However, our results do provide support for the hypothesis of increased right-hemisphere use when an organism is exposed to aversive stimuli.


Assuntos
Ciclídeos/anatomia & histologia , Ciclídeos/fisiologia , Animais , Comportamento Animal/fisiologia , Sinais (Psicologia) , Feminino , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Habenula/anatomia & histologia , Habenula/fisiologia , Masculino , Fatores Sexuais , Olfato/fisiologia , Comportamento Social , Percepção Visual/fisiologia
5.
Anim Cogn ; 18(1): 53-64, 2015 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24966123

RESUMO

Visiting multiple locations and returning to the start via the shortest route, referred to as the traveling salesman (or salesperson) problem (TSP), is a valuable skill for both humans and non-humans. In the current study, pigeons were trained with increasing set sizes of up to six goals, with each set size presented in three distinct configurations, until consistency in route selection emerged. After training at each set size, the pigeons were tested with two novel configurations. All pigeons acquired routes that were significantly more efficient (i.e., shorter in length) than expected by chance selection of the goals. On average, the pigeons also selected routes that were more efficient than expected based on a local nearest-neighbor strategy and were as efficient as the average route generated by a crossing-avoidance strategy. Analysis of the routes taken indicated that they conformed to both a nearest-neighbor and a crossing-avoidance strategy significantly more often than expected by chance. Both the time taken to visit all goals and the actual distance traveled decreased from the first to the last trials of training in each set size. On the first trial with novel configurations, average efficiency was higher than chance, but was not higher than expected from a nearest-neighbor or crossing-avoidance strategy. These results indicate that pigeons can learn to select efficient routes on a TSP problem.


Assuntos
Columbidae , Resolução de Problemas , Navegação Espacial , Animais , Feminino , Habituação Psicofisiológica , Masculino , Aprendizagem Espacial
6.
BMC Neurosci ; 15: 127, 2014 Nov 28.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25429715

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Individuals with Williams syndrome, a neurogenetic condition caused by deletion of a set of genes at chromosomal location 7q11.23, exhibit a remarkable suite of traits including hypersociality with high, nonselective friendliness and low social anxiety, expressive language relatively well-developed but under-developed social-communication skills overall, and reduced visual-spatial abilities. Deletions and duplications of the Williams-syndrome region have also been associated with autism, and with schizophrenia, two disorders centrally involving social cognition. Several lines of evidence have linked the gene GTF2I (General Transcription Factor IIi) with the social phenotypes of Williams syndrome, but a role for this gene in sociality within healthy populations has yet to be investigated. RESULTS: We genotyped a large set of healthy individuals for two single-nucleotide polymorphisms in the GTF2I gene that have recently been significantly associated with autism, and thus apparently exhibit functional effects on autism-related social phenotypes. GTF2I genotypes for these SNPs showed highly significant association with low social anxiety combined with reduced social-communication abilities, which represents a metric of the Williams-syndrome cognitive profile as described from previous studies. CONCLUSIONS: These findings implicate the GTF2I gene in the neurogenetic basis of social communication and social anxiety, both in Williams syndrome and among individuals in healthy populations.


Assuntos
Cognição , Polimorfismo de Nucleotídeo Único , Comportamento Social , Fatores de Transcrição TFII/genética , Síndrome de Williams/genética , Síndrome de Williams/psicologia , Ansiedade/genética , Feminino , Técnicas de Genotipagem , Humanos , Masculino , Fenótipo , Testes Psicológicos , Inquéritos e Questionários
7.
J Hum Genet ; 59(6): 332-6, 2014 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24785688

RESUMO

Imprinted genes have been posited to have important roles in human brain development and cognition, but their effects in nonclinical populations have yet to be investigated. Single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) of the imprinted gene LRRTM1 have previously been associated with schizophrenia risk and with handedness in individuals with dyslexia. We tested the hypothesis that genetic variation (SNPs) and epigenetic variation (methylation) in this gene are associated with schizotypy and handedness in a nonclinical population. Risk alleles of the three schizophrenia-linked SNPs were associated with significantly and substantially higher levels of total schizotypy. Variation in SNP genotypes was not associated with handedness, but levels of methylation in a block of CpG sites in the putative LRRTM1 promoter region were associated with more-mixed handedness. These findings provide evidence of continuity between schizophrenia and schizotypy with regard to the psychological effects of allelic variation in this imprinted gene, and show that epigenetic variation in an imprinted gene mediates the development and expression of human handedness.


Assuntos
Lateralidade Funcional/genética , Impressão Genômica , Proteínas de Membrana/genética , Proteínas do Tecido Nervoso/genética , Esquizofrenia/genética , Alelos , Ilhas de CpG , Feminino , Estudos de Associação Genética , Genética Populacional , Humanos , Masculino , Metilação , Polimorfismo de Nucleotídeo Único , Psicometria
8.
Am J Hum Biol ; 26(2): 183-8, 2014.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24288299

RESUMO

OBJECTIVES: While consanguineous marriage has been shown to result in a small increase in risk of recessive Mendelian disorders among offspring, far less research has been conducted on the effects of inbreeding on complex traits. These effects, thought to result from increased developmental instability due to loss of heterozygosity, are expected to be found more pervasively than rare recessive Mendelian traits and are expected to result in increased developmental noise. Here, we test for a direct effect of inbreeding on 2D : 4D, a putative indicator of prenatal hormonal environment. METHODS: We compared the 2D : 4D ratios of 122 male and 108 female consanguineous (children of first cousin marriages) high school and university students to those of 142 male and 122 females controls. RESULTS: Across hands and sex, consanguineous parentage was consistently associated with lower, more masculine-typical, digit ratios. Digit ratios were 1.3-1.9 times more variable among the consanguineous group than the control group. While socio-economic status cannot explain the effects seen in our data, we found that lower, more masculinized, digit ratios were associated with lower family income. CONCLUSIONS: Our results suggest that consanguineous marriages are associated with a fetal environment that influences morphological development and may have associated behavioral sequelae.


Assuntos
Consanguinidade , Dedos/anatomia & histologia , Adolescente , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Classe Social , Turquia , Adulto Jovem
9.
Biol Lett ; 9(6): 20130395, 2013.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24132092

RESUMO

William D. Hamilton postulated the existence of 'genes underlying altruism', under the rubric of inclusive fitness theory, a half-century ago. Such genes are now poised for discovery. In this article, we develop a set of intuitive criteria for the recognition and analysis of genes for altruism and describe the first candidate genes affecting altruism from social insects and humans. We also provide evidence from a human population for genetically based trade-offs, underlain by oxytocin-system polymorphisms, between alleles for altruism and alleles for non-social cognition. Such trade-offs between self-oriented and altruistic behaviour may influence the evolution of phenotypic diversity across all social animals.


Assuntos
Altruísmo , Aptidão Genética , Insetos/genética , Comportamento Social , Alelos , Animais , Comportamento Animal , Evolução Biológica , Hierarquia Social , Humanos , Modelos Genéticos , Ocitocina/metabolismo , Fenótipo , Polimorfismo Genético , Seleção Genética
11.
Behav Brain Res ; 439: 114228, 2023 02 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36436731

RESUMO

We know little about how - or even if in some species - fish shoal in darkness. We hypothesized that 'dark shoaling' occurs in zebrafish and therefore must depend upon lateral line sensory input. Shoaling in groups of five adult zebrafish was analyzed with motion tracking software. We measured average inter-individual distance, time near the arena wall (thigmotaxis zone) and total distance traveled under normal room light, and in near-complete darkness (infrared light at 850 nm). These observations were repeated in fish treated with cobalt chloride (CoCl2), which ablates lateral line function. In untreated controls, dark shoaling was reduced compared to in light, but nonetheless still present. Elimination of lateral line sensory input by CoCl2 treatment similarly reduced, but did not eliminate, shoaling under both light and dark. Our findings indicate that normal zebrafish shoaling in light or dark requires both visual and lateral line inputs, with neither alone sufficient for normal shoaling.


Assuntos
Sistema da Linha Lateral , Comportamento Social , Animais , Comportamento Animal , Peixe-Zebra
12.
Am J Hum Biol ; 24(5): 682-9, 2012.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22806965

RESUMO

OBJECTIVES: The 2D:4D digit ratio, the relative lengths of the index and ring fingers in humans, is a widely used proxy measure for prenatal testosterone exposure. Varying distributions of androgen and estrogen receptors on the second and fourth digits, both of which regulate digit development, appears to be the basis for this effect. Polymorphism in a tandem repeat in the gene coding for the estrogen receptor α (ESR1) in zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata) not only explains a significant amount of variation in digit ratio but also seems to explain the significant correlation between digit ratio and sexual behavior in these birds. Here, we investigate the effect of TA polymorphism in ESR1 on 2D:4D and aggressive behavior in men. METHODS: We genotyped ESR1 polymorphism in samples collected for a previous study in which we had demonstrated an association between androgen receptor polymorphism and aggression, but not 2D:4D. RESULTS: We found a significant effect of ESR1 TA repeat number on left hand 2D:4D ratio. More TA repeats were associated with higher, more feminized, digit ratios. We found no effect on right hand 2D:4D. We also found an effect of ESR1 polymorphism on aggressive behavior. Greater heterozygosity in TA(n) was associated with lower physical aggression. CONCLUSIONS: Our results suggest that a significant amount of left hand 2D:4D variation and aggressive behavior is due to this variation in ESR1, and that some of the correlation between digit ratio and social behavior is due to pleiotropic effects of ESR1 variation on the two traits.


Assuntos
Agressão , Receptor alfa de Estrogênio/genética , Dedos/anatomia & histologia , Polimorfismo Genético , Regiões Promotoras Genéticas , Adolescente , Alberta , Humanos , Masculino , Repetições de Microssatélites , Caracteres Sexuais , Fatores Sexuais , Inquéritos e Questionários , Adulto Jovem
13.
Behav Genet ; 41(4): 543-56, 2011 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20967566

RESUMO

Variation in prenatal exposure to androgens is thought to be responsible for some of the individual differences in aggressive behavior among adults. A putative indicator of prenatal testosterone exposure, 2D:4D (the index to ring finger length) ratios have shown a weak correlation with aggression. Variation in sensitivity of the androgen receptor, resulting from polymorphism in the AR gene, is also thought to influence the relative expression of sexually dimorphic traits within each sex, including aggressive behavior and 2D:4D. Here we examine variation in aggression, 2D:4D, and polymorphism in the AR and MAO-A genes in a sample of 188 men. We find no evidence of AR gene influence on right hand 2D:4D, and a weak trend towards more feminine-typical left hand 2D:4D in men with more sensitive androgen receptors. Men with more sensitive androgen receptors tended to score lower on many of the subscales of the Aggression Questionnaire and Indirect Aggression Questionnaire. We found no influence of MAO-A allele on either digit ratio or aggressive behavior. We conclude that more masculine-typical 2D:4D does not reflect greater sensitivity to testosterone through variation in this locus on the AR gene, and that AR alleles conferring greater sensitivity to testosterone are associated with lower, not higher propensity to aggression.


Assuntos
Monoaminoxidase/genética , Receptores Androgênicos/genética , Adulto , Alelos , Androgênios/metabolismo , Sequência de Bases , Feminino , Variação Genética , Genótipo , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Genéticos , Dados de Sequência Molecular , Polimorfismo Genético , Inquéritos e Questionários
14.
Laterality ; 16(4): 385-400, 2011 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20700854

RESUMO

Cerebral lateralisation, the partitioning of cognitive functioning into one hemisphere of the brain, was once considered unique to humans; however, recent research in a variety of taxa suggests that lateralisation is an evolutionarily ancient adaptation. Handedness is the most obvious manifestation of cerebral lateralisation in humans. Much of the literature on handedness has focused on the direction, rather than the strength, of this lateralisation. From both genetic and evolutionary perspectives it may be more informative to study degrees of cerebral lateralisation rather than direction. Strong evidence suggests that the strength may be more closely associated with individual differences in behaviour in humans than the direction, and individual variation in the degree of lateralisation has been found to correlate with personality-like characteristics such as aggressiveness in fish. The association between different patterns of lateralisation and personality characteristics may help explain how variation in the strength of lateralisation is evolutionarily stable in natural populations. The present study investigated the relationship between aggression and strength of handedness in humans. We found a significant interaction between sex and lateralisation with respect to aggression. In males, trait aggression was significantly higher in strong-handers than in mixed-handers, while no difference was seen in females. This finding highlights the importance of considering sex as a factor when investigating relationships between cerebral lateralisation and personality characteristics. Potential causes and consequences of the sex interaction as well as future directions for research are discussed.


Assuntos
Agressão/fisiologia , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Personalidade/fisiologia , Caracteres Sexuais , Adolescente , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Fatores Sexuais , Inquéritos e Questionários , Adulto Jovem
15.
Sex Dev ; 15(1-3): 93-107, 2021.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34433170

RESUMO

Studying environmental sex determination (ESD) in cichlids provides a phylogenetic and comparative approach to understand the evolution of the underlying mechanisms, their impact on the evolution of the overlying systems, and the neuroethology of life history strategies. Natural selection normally favors parents who invest equally in the development of male and female offspring, but evolution may favor deviations from this 50:50 ratio when environmental conditions produce an advantage for doing so. Many species of cichlids demonstrate ESD in response to water chemistry (temperature, pH, and oxygen concentration). The relative strengths of and the exact interactions between these factors vary between congeners, demonstrating genetic variation in sensitivity. The presence of sizable proportions of the less common sex towards the environmental extremes in most species strongly suggests the presence of some genetic sex-determining loci acting in parallel with the ESD factors. Sex determination and differentiation in these species does not seem to result in the organization of a final and irreversible sexual fate, so much as a life-long ongoing battle between competing male- and female-determining genetic and hormonal networks governed by epigenetic factors. We discuss what is and is not known about the epigenetic mechanism behind the differentiation of both gonads and sex differences in the brain. Beyond the well-studied tilapia species, the 2 best-studied dwarf cichlid systems showing ESD are the South American genus Apistogramma and the West African genus Pelvicachromis. Both species demonstrate male morphs with alternative reproductive tactics. We discuss the further neuroethology opportunities such systems provide to the study of epigenetics of alternative life history strategies and other behavioral variation.


Assuntos
Ciclídeos , Animais , Ciclídeos/genética , Epigênese Genética/genética , Feminino , Gônadas , Masculino , Filogenia , Análise para Determinação do Sexo
16.
Autism Res Treat ; 2021: 6634584, 2021.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34188957

RESUMO

Mutations affecting the synaptic-scaffold gene SHANK3 represent the most common genetic causes of autism with intellectual disability, accounting for about 1-2% of cases. Rare variants of this gene have also been associated with schizophrenia, and its deletion results in the autistic condition known as Phelan-McDermid syndrome. Despite the importance of SHANK3 as a paradigmatic gene mediating neurodevelopmental disorders, its psychological effects in nonclinical populations have yet to be studied. We genotyped the nonsynonymous, functional SHANK3 SNP rs9616915 in a large population of typical individuals scored for autism spectrum traits (the Autism Quotient, AQ) and schizotypy spectrum traits (the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire, SPQ-BR). Males, but not females, showed significant genotypic effects for the SPQ-BR subscale associated with speech and language: Odd Speech. These findings, in conjunction with animal model studies showing vocalization and auditory effects of SHANK3 mutations, and studies indicating severe language alterations and speech-associated white matter tract abnormalities in Phelan-McDermid syndrome, suggest that SHANK3 differentially affects the development and expression of human language and speech. Imaging genetic and speech-language studies of typical individuals carrying different genotypes of rs9616915 should provide novel insights into the neurological and psychological bases of speech and language alterations among individuals with SHANK3 mutations and Phelan-McDermid syndrome.

17.
Front Psychiatry ; 12: 665721, 2021.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33868063

RESUMO

How is cognitive empathy related to sociality, imagination, and other psychological constructs? How is it altered in disorders of human social cognition? We leveraged a large data set (1,168 students, 62% female) on the Reading the Mind in the Eyes test (RMET), the Autism Quotient (AQ), and the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (SPQ-BR) to test the hypotheses that the RMET, as a metric of cognitive empathy, reflects mainly social abilities, imagination, or both. RMET showed the expected female bias in performance, though only for eyes that expressed emotions and not for neutral expressions. RMET performance was significantly, and more strongly, associated with the AQ and SPQ subscales that reflect aspects of imagination (AQ-Imagination and SPQ-Magical Ideation) than aspects of social abilities (AQ-Social, AQ-Communication, and SPQ-Interpersonal subscales). These results were confirmed with multiple regression analysis, which also implicated increased attention (AQ-Attention Switching and, marginally non-significantly, AQ-Attention to Detail) in RMET performance. The two imagination-related correlates of RMET performance also show the strongest sex biases for the AQ and SPQ: male biased in AQ-Imagination, and female biased in SPQ-Magical Ideation, with small to medium effect sizes. Taken together, these findings suggest that cognitive empathy, as quantified by the RMET, centrally involves imagination, which is underdeveloped (with a male bias) on the autism spectrum and overdeveloped (with a female bias) on the schizotypy spectrum, with optimal emotion-recognition performance intermediate between the two. The results, in conjunction with previous studies, implicate a combination of optimal imagination and focused attention in enhanced RMET performance.

18.
Theor Popul Biol ; 75(2-3): 176-82, 2009.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19268678

RESUMO

Sir Philip Sidney games are a widely used model of simple signaling. Johnstone and Grafen [Johnstone, R.A., Grafen, A., 1993. Dishonesty and the handicap principle. Animal Behaviour 46, 759-764] present a version in which the Evolutionarily Stable Strategy (ESS) is for most signalers to "honestly" signal, with a small minority of signalers who "cheat". This model is among the most frequently cited papers on the topic of "dishonest" signaling and supports the view that signals may be "dishonest" as long as they are "honest on average". Using genetic algorithms, we demonstrate that another solution exists to the game, an evolutionarily stable set of Nash equilibria in which members of the set never signal and all donors give their resource. Payoffs to players using this set of strategies is greater those when playing the "dishonest" signaling ESS. We demonstrate that a random population is far more likely to evolve to this non-communicating strategy set than the "dishonest" signaling ESS. We also discuss the dynamics of biological game theory models and the advances of genetic algorithms as a heuristic solution method for these models.


Assuntos
Evolução Biológica , Comunicação , Enganação , Modelos Teóricos , Algoritmos
19.
MethodsX ; 6: 1832-1837, 2019.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31497513

RESUMO

The elevated plus maze is a commonly used and well-validated test of anxiety-related behaviour in rodents. The use of fish in behavioural neuroscience paradigms is increasing, necessitating an equivalent test for studying anxiety-like behaviour in fish. Because behaviour in the elevated plus maze is driven by aversion to open space, the submerged plus maze described here uses transparent walls to elicit similar behaviour in fish. The tendency of fish to explore or avoid the sections of the maze containing transparent walls is used as proxy for anxiety level. This submerged plus maze was designed and validated for convict cichlid (Amatitlania nigrofasciata) fish.

20.
Behav Brain Res ; 362: 332-337, 2019 04 19.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30599155

RESUMO

The elevated plus maze is a prominent and well-documented test for studying anxiety in rodents. Fish are becoming more prevalent in studies of anxiety, yet the elevated plus maze has not been adapted and validated for fish. In the present study, we created an aquatic version of the elevated plus maze called the 'submerged plus maze,' which is shaped like a plus symbol with four arms alternating between black and transparent walls. We used convict cichlid fish (Amatitlania nigrofasciata) and administered diazepam to validate the apparatus for studying anxiety-like behaviour. After diazepam exposure, fish spent more time in and entered more open arms than after vehicle exposure, consistent with the effect of benzodiazepines on rodents in the elevated plus maze. The submerged plus maze maintains construct validity for testing anxiety in convict cichlid fish.


Assuntos
Ansiedade/fisiopatologia , Comportamento Animal/efeitos dos fármacos , Diazepam/farmacologia , Atividade Motora/efeitos dos fármacos , Animais , Ansiolíticos/farmacologia , Comportamento Exploratório/efeitos dos fármacos , Peixes , Aprendizagem em Labirinto/efeitos dos fármacos
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA