Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 12 de 12
Filtrar
1.
Behav Res Methods ; 55(1): 364-416, 2023 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35384605

RESUMO

In this paper, we present a review of how the various aspects of any study using an eye tracker (such as the instrument, methodology, environment, participant, etc.) affect the quality of the recorded eye-tracking data and the obtained eye-movement and gaze measures. We take this review to represent the empirical foundation for reporting guidelines of any study involving an eye tracker. We compare this empirical foundation to five existing reporting guidelines and to a database of 207 published eye-tracking studies. We find that reporting guidelines vary substantially and do not match with actual reporting practices. We end by deriving a minimal, flexible reporting guideline based on empirical research (Section "An empirically based minimal reporting guideline").


Assuntos
Movimentos Oculares , Tecnologia de Rastreamento Ocular , Humanos , Pesquisa Empírica
2.
Psychol Res ; 84(7): 1777-1788, 2020 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31004194

RESUMO

Successful self-control during food choice might require inhibition of impulses to avoid indulging in tempting but calorie-dense foods, and this might particularly apply to individuals restraining their food intake. Adopting a novel within-participant modeling approach, we tested 62 females during a mouse-tracking based binary food choice task. Subsequent ratings of foods on palatability, healthiness, and calorie density were modeled as predictors for both decision outcome (choice) and decision process (measures of self-control conflict) while considering the moderating role of restrained eating. Results revealed that individuals higher on restrained eating were less likely to choose more high-calorie foods and showed less self-control conflict when choosing healthier foods. The latter finding is in contrast with the common assumption of self-control as requiring effortful and conscious inhibition of temptation impulses. Interestingly, restrained eaters rated healthy and low-calorie foods as more palatable than individuals with lower restrained eating scores, both in the main experiment and an independent replication study, hinting at an automatic and rather effortless mechanism of self-control (palatability shift) that obviates effortful inhibition of temptation impulses.


Assuntos
Dieta Redutora/psicologia , Ingestão de Alimentos/psicologia , Preferências Alimentares/psicologia , Inibição Psicológica , Motivação , Autocontrole/psicologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Áustria , Tomada de Decisões , Feminino , Humanos , Adulto Jovem
4.
Behav Brain Sci ; 41: e139, 2018 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31064529

RESUMO

Randomness in the selection process of to-be-replicated target papers is critical for replication success or failure. If target papers are chosen because of the ease of doing a replication, or because replicators doubt the reported findings, replications are likely to fail. To date, the selection of replication targets is biased.


Assuntos
Ciência , Editoração , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes
5.
Behav Res Methods ; 50(4): 1446-1460, 2018 08.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29218587

RESUMO

The goal of this study was to validate AFFDEX and FACET, two algorithms classifying emotions from facial expressions, in iMotions's software suite. In Study 1, pictures of standardized emotional facial expressions from three databases, the Warsaw Set of Emotional Facial Expression Pictures (WSEFEP), the Amsterdam Dynamic Facial Expression Set (ADFES), and the Radboud Faces Database (RaFD), were classified with both modules. Accuracy (Matching Scores) was computed to assess and compare the classification quality. Results show a large variance in accuracy across emotions and databases, with a performance advantage for FACET over AFFDEX. In Study 2, 110 participants' facial expressions were measured while being exposed to emotionally evocative pictures from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS), the Geneva Affective Picture Database (GAPED) and the Radboud Faces Database (RaFD). Accuracy again differed for distinct emotions, and FACET performed better. Overall, iMotions can achieve acceptable accuracy for standardized pictures of prototypical (vs. natural) facial expressions, but performs worse for more natural facial expressions. We discuss potential sources for limited validity and suggest research directions in the broader context of emotion research.


Assuntos
Bases de Dados Factuais/normas , Emoções/classificação , Expressão Facial , Adulto , Algoritmos , Pesquisa Comportamental/métodos , Confiabilidade dos Dados , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Software
6.
Behav Res Methods ; 49(5): 1769-1779, 2017 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27797092

RESUMO

The recent introduction of inexpensive eyetrackers has opened up a wealth of opportunities for researchers to study attention in interactive tasks. No software package has previously been available to help researchers exploit those opportunities. We created "the pyeTribe," a software package that offers, among others, the following features: first, a communication platform between many eyetrackers to allow for simultaneous recording of multiple participants; second, the simultaneous calibration of multiple eyetrackers without the experimenter's supervision; third, data collection restricted to periods of interest, thus reducing the volume of data and easing analysis. We used a standard economic game (the public goods game) to examine the data quality and demonstrate the potential of our software package. Moreover, we conducted a modeling analysis, which illustrates how combining process and behavioral data can improve models of human decision-making behavior in social situations. Our software is open source.


Assuntos
Medições dos Movimentos Oculares , Software , Jogos de Vídeo/psicologia , Humanos
7.
Appetite ; 71: 242-51, 2013 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23994507

RESUMO

The predominant, but largely untested, assumption in research on food choice is that people obey the classic commandments of rational behavior: they carefully look up every piece of relevant information, weight each piece according to subjective importance, and then combine them into a judgment or choice. In real world situations, however, the available time, motivation, and computational resources may simply not suffice to keep these commandments. Indeed, there is a large body of research suggesting that human choice is often better accommodated by heuristics-simple rules that enable decision making on the basis of a few, but important, pieces of information. We investigated the prevalence of such heuristics in a computerized experiment that engaged participants in a series of choices between two lunch dishes. Employing MouselabWeb, a process-tracing technique, we found that simple heuristics described an overwhelmingly large proportion of choices, whereas strategies traditionally deemed rational were barely apparent in our data. Replicating previous findings, we also observed that visual stimulus segments received a much larger proportion of attention than any nutritional values did. Our results suggest that, consistent with human behavior in other domains, people make their food choices on the basis of simple and informationally frugal heuristics.


Assuntos
Apetite , Comportamento de Escolha , Preferências Alimentares/psicologia , Adulto , Peso Corporal , Tomada de Decisões , Feminino , Comportamentos Relacionados com a Saúde , Conhecimentos, Atitudes e Prática em Saúde , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Modelos Teóricos , Motivação
8.
Psychol Rev ; 115(1): 263-73, 2008 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18211202

RESUMO

Comments on the article by E. Brandstätter, G. Gigerenzer, and R. Hertwig. Resolution of debates in cognition usually comes from the introduction of constraints in the form of new data about either the process or representation. Decision research, in contrast, has relied predominantly on testing models by examining their fit to choices. The authors examine a recently proposed choice strategy, the priority heuristic, which provides a novel account of how people make risky choices. The authors identify a number of properties that the priority heuristic should have as a process model and illustrate how they may be tested. The results, along with prior research, suggest that although the priority heuristic captures some variability in the attention paid to outcomes, it fails to account for major characteristics of the data, particularly the frequent transitions between outcomes and their probabilities. The article concludes with a discussion of the properties that should be captured by process models of risky choice and the role of process data in theory development.


Assuntos
Tomada de Decisões , Modelos Psicológicos , Assunção de Riscos , Humanos
9.
J Exp Psychol Gen ; 147(2): 147-169, 2018 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29369680

RESUMO

There is a disconnect in the literature between analyses of risky choice based on cumulative prospect theory (CPT) and work on predecisional information processing. One likely reason is that for expectation models (e.g., CPT), it is often assumed that people behaved only as if they conducted the computations leading to the predicted choice and that the models are thus mute regarding information processing. We suggest that key psychological constructs in CPT, such as loss aversion and outcome and probability sensitivity, can be interpreted in terms of attention allocation. In two experiments, we tested hypotheses about specific links between CPT parameters and attentional regularities. Experiment 1 used process tracing to monitor participants' predecisional attention allocation to outcome and probability information. As hypothesized, individual differences in CPT's loss-aversion, outcome-sensitivity, and probability-sensitivity parameters (estimated from participants' choices) were systematically associated with individual differences in attention allocation to outcome and probability information. For instance, loss aversion was associated with the relative attention allocated to loss and gain outcomes, and a more strongly curved weighting function was associated with less attention allocated to probabilities. Experiment 2 manipulated participants' attention to losses or gains, causing systematic differences in CPT's loss-aversion parameter. This result indicates that attention allocation can to some extent cause choice regularities that are captured by CPT. Our findings demonstrate an as-if model's capacity to reflect characteristics of information processing. We suggest that the observed CPT-attention links can be harnessed to inform the development of process models of risky choice. (PsycINFO Database Record


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Cognição/fisiologia , Teoria Psicológica , Adolescente , Adulto , Afeto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Adulto Jovem
10.
PLoS One ; 13(1): e0188951, 2018.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29298294

RESUMO

Worldwide, more than one million people die on the roads each year. A third of these fatal accidents are attributed to speeding, with properties of the individual driver and the environment regarded as key contributing factors. We examine real-world speeding behavior and its interaction with illuminance, an environmental property defined as the luminous flux incident on a surface. Drawing on an analysis of 1.2 million vehicle movements, we show that reduced illuminance levels are associated with increased speeding. This relationship persists when we control for factors known to influence speeding (e.g., fluctuations in traffic volume) and consider proxies of illuminance (e.g., sight distance). Our findings add to a long-standing debate about how the quality of visual conditions affects drivers' speed perception and driving speed. Policy makers can intervene by educating drivers about the inverse illuminance‒speeding relationship and by testing how improved vehicle headlights and smart road lighting can attenuate speeding.


Assuntos
Acidentes de Trânsito , Condução de Veículo , Luz , Medição de Risco , Suíça , Tempo (Meteorologia)
11.
Behav Res Methods ; 37(2): 293-300, 2005 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16171202

RESUMO

We describe WebDiP (Web Decision Processes)--an open-source, online tool-which enables a researcher to track participants while they search for information in a database, available through the Internet. After various instructions on setup and configuration are given, a detailed view of WebDiP explains the system's technical features. Furthermore, other open-source tools are mentioned that helped in programming WebDiP, running it, or analyzing data gathered with it. We present new approaches of how open-source thinking can be incorporated into a research process and discuss future perspectives of WebDiP.


Assuntos
Internet , Software , Humanos
12.
Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput ; 35(2): 227-35, 2003 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12834077

RESUMO

The focus of this study is the effect of the location (laboratory vs. Web) of experiments on active information search in decision-making tasks. In two experiments, participants were confronted with two different search method versions (list vs. keyword) for acquiring information about a task from a database. The amount and type of information gathered and the time required for task completion were measured. In Experiment 1, significantly more information was searched for in the laboratory than on the Web when the list version was employed, whereas there was no difference between locations in the keyword version. In Experiment 2, the participants were assigned randomly to the Web or the laboratory condition. The results of Experiment 1 were replicated. Whereas location (and the presence or absence of an experimenter) had an effect on the absolute amount of information gathered in both experiments, the relative distribution and type of information items did not differ.


Assuntos
Disseminação de Informação/métodos , Serviços de Informação , Armazenamento e Recuperação da Informação/métodos , Internet , Meio Social , Tomada de Decisões , Humanos , Aprendizagem , Modelos Psicológicos , Psicologia Experimental/métodos , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Assunção de Riscos
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA