Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 9 de 9
Filtrar
1.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 118(15)2021 04 13.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33876754

RESUMO

Military personnel in industrialized societies often develop posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) during combat. It is unclear whether combat-related PTSD is a universal evolutionary response to danger or a culture-specific syndrome of industrialized societies. We interviewed 218 Turkana pastoralist warriors in Kenya, who engage in lethal cattle raids, about their combat experiences and PTSD symptoms. Turkana in our sample had a high prevalence of PTSD symptoms, but Turkana with high symptom severity had lower prevalence of depression-like symptoms than American service members with high symptom severity. Symptoms that facilitate responding to danger were better predicted by combat exposure, whereas depressive symptoms were better predicted by exposure to combat-related moral violations. The findings suggest that some PTSD symptoms stem from an evolved response to danger, while depressive PTSD symptoms may be caused by culturally specific moral norm violations.


Assuntos
Povos Indígenas/psicologia , Transtornos de Estresse Pós-Traumáticos/etnologia , Adulto , Evolução Biológica , Comparação Transcultural , Humanos , Quênia , Desenvolvimento Moral , Transtornos de Estresse Pós-Traumáticos/psicologia , Inquéritos e Questionários/normas
2.
Behav Brain Sci ; 47: e18, 2024 Jan 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38224042

RESUMO

Glowacki recognizes the importance of norms in enabling war and peace, but does not focus on the cultural evolutionary mechanisms by which these norms are maintained. We highlight how group-structured cultural selection shapes the scale and nature of peaceful intergroup interactions. The mechanistic perspective reveals that there are many more cases of peaceful intergroup relations than the current account implies.


Assuntos
Condições Sociais , Guerra , Humanos
3.
Behav Brain Sci ; 43: e119, 2020 05 28.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32460954

RESUMO

We support the goal to integrate models of culture and cognition. However, we are not convinced that the free energy principle and Thinking Through Other Minds will be useful in achieving it. There are long traditions of modeling both cultural evolution and cognition. Demonstrating that FEP or TTOM can integrate these models will require a bit more math.


Assuntos
Cognição , Evolução Cultural , Humanos , Matemática
4.
Behav Brain Sci ; 39: e58, 2016 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27561598

RESUMO

The main objective of our target article was to sketch the empirical case for the importance of selection at the level of groups on cultural variation. Such variation is massive in humans, but modest or absent in other species. Group selection processes acting on this variation is a framework for developing explanations of the unusual level of cooperation between non-relatives found in our species. Our case for cultural group selection (CGS) followed Darwin's classic syllogism regarding natural selection: If variation exists at the level of groups, if this variation is heritable, and if it plays a role in the success or failure of competing groups, then selection will operate at the level of groups. We outlined the relevant domains where such evidence can be sought and characterized the main conclusions of work in those domains. Most commentators agree that CGS plays some role in human evolution, although some were considerably more skeptical. Some contributed additional empirical cases. Some raised issues of the scope of CGS explanations versus competing ones.


Assuntos
Evolução Biológica , Seleção Genética , Processos Grupais , Humanos , Relações Interpessoais , Comportamento Social
5.
Behav Brain Sci ; 39: e30, 2016 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25347943

RESUMO

Human cooperation is highly unusual. We live in large groups composed mostly of non-relatives. Evolutionists have proposed a number of explanations for this pattern, including cultural group selection and extensions of more general processes such as reciprocity, kin selection, and multi-level selection acting on genes. Evolutionary processes are consilient; they affect several different empirical domains, such as patterns of behavior and the proximal drivers of that behavior. In this target article, we sketch the evidence from five domains that bear on the explanatory adequacy of cultural group selection and competing hypotheses to explain human cooperation. Does cultural transmission constitute an inheritance system that can evolve in a Darwinian fashion? Are the norms that underpin institutions among the cultural traits so transmitted? Do we observe sufficient variation at the level of groups of considerable size for group selection to be a plausible process? Do human groups compete, and do success and failure in competition depend upon cultural variation? Do we observe adaptations for cooperation in humans that most plausibly arose by cultural group selection? If the answer to one of these questions is "no," then we must look to other hypotheses. We present evidence, including quantitative evidence, that the answer to all of the questions is "yes" and argue that we must take the cultural group selection hypothesis seriously. If culturally transmitted systems of rules (institutions) that limit individual deviance organize cooperation in human societies, then it is not clear that any extant alternative to cultural group selection can be a complete explanation.


Assuntos
Comportamento Cooperativo , Evolução Cultural , Adaptação Fisiológica , Altruísmo , Evolução Biológica , Comportamento Competitivo , Processos Grupais , Humanos , Relações Interpessoais , Seleção Genética , Comportamento Social
6.
Evol Anthropol ; 24(2): 50-61, 2015.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25914359

RESUMO

When humans wage war, it is not unusual for battlefields to be strewn with dead warriors. These warriors typically were men in their reproductive prime who, had they not died in battle, might have gone on to father more children. Typically, they are also genetically unrelated to one another. We know of no other animal species in which reproductively capable, genetically unrelated individuals risk their lives in this manner. Because the immense private costs borne by individual warriors create benefits that are shared widely by others in their group, warfare is a stark evolutionary puzzle that is difficult to explain. Although several scholars have posited models of the evolution of human warfare, these models do not adequately explain how humans solve the problem of collective action in warfare at the evolutionarily novel scale of hundreds of genetically unrelated individuals. We propose that group-structured cultural selection explains this phenomenon.


Assuntos
Evolução Biológica , Evolução Cultural , Seleção Genética , Guerra , Animais , Antropologia Física , Formigas/fisiologia , Humanos , Pan troglodytes/fisiologia
7.
Behav Brain Sci ; 37(3): 280-1, 2014 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24970427

RESUMO

Smaldino makes a solid contribution to the literature on the evolution of human social organization by pointing out that group-level-traits (GLTs) often emerge from the interactions of group members in such a way that their effects are not easily partitioned into individual selection. However, we argue that he too readily dismisses institutional analysis as a tool for understanding these traits.


Assuntos
Comportamento Cooperativo , Evolução Cultural , Processos Grupais , Seleção Genética , Humanos
9.
Sci Rep ; 13(1): 1160, 2023 01 20.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36670128

RESUMO

Some human groups are organized hierarchically and some are distributed. Both types of groups occur in economic, political, and military domains, but it is unclear why hierarchical organizations are favored in certain contexts and distributed organizations are favored in others. I propose that these different organizational structures can be explained by human groups having different constraints on their ability to foster cooperation within the group. Human within-group cooperation is often maintained by monitoring and punishment. In hierarchical groups, monitoring and punishment are organized into tree-like command-and-control structures with supervisors responsible for monitoring the cooperation of their subordinates and punishing non-cooperators. By contrast, in distributed groups, monitoring is diffuse and punishment is collective. I propose that the organization of cooperative human groups is constrained by the costs of monitoring and punishment. I formalize this hypothesis with a model where individuals in a group cooperate to produce public goods while embedded in a network of monitoring and punishment responsibilities. I show that, when punishment costs are high and monitoring costs are low, socially-optimal monitoring and punishment networks are distributed. The size of these distributed networks is constrained by monitoring costs. However, when punishment costs are low, socially-optimal networks are hierarchical. Monitoring costs do not constrain the size of hierarchical networks but determine how many levels of supervision are required to foster cooperation in the hierarchical group. These results may explain the increasingly large and hierarchical groups throughout much of human history. They also suggest that the recent emergence of large-scale distributed organizations has been possible because new technologies, like the internet, have made monitoring costs extremely low.


Assuntos
Comportamento Cooperativo , Comportamento Social , Humanos , Processos Grupais , Punição , Custos e Análise de Custo , Teoria dos Jogos
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA