Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 111
Filter
1.
Cereb Cortex ; 34(9)2024 Sep 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39233376

ABSTRACT

Repeated exposure to word forms and meanings improves lexical knowledge acquisition. However, the roles of domain-general and language-specific brain regions during this process remain unclear. To investigate this, we applied intermittent theta burst stimulation over the domain-general (group left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex) and domain-specific (Group L IFG) brain regions, with a control group receiving sham intermittent theta burst stimulation. Intermittent theta burst stimulation effects were subsequently assessed in functional magnetic resonance imaging using an artificial word learning task which consisted of 3 learning phases. A generalized psychophysiological interaction analysis explored the whole brain functional connectivity, while dynamic causal modeling estimated causal interactions in specific brain regions modulated by intermittent theta burst stimulation during repeated exposure. Compared to sham stimulation, active intermittent theta burst stimulation improved word learning performance and reduced activation of the left insula in learning phase 2. Active intermittent theta burst stimulation over the domain-general region increased whole-brain functional connectivity and modulated effective connectivity between brain regions during repeated exposure. This effect was not observed when active intermittent theta burst stimulation was applied to the language-specific region. These findings suggest that the domain-general region plays a crucial role in word formation rule learning, with intermittent theta burst stimulation enhancing whole-brain connectivity and facilitating efficient information exchange between key brain regions during new word learning.


Subject(s)
Brain , Language , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation , Humans , Male , Female , Young Adult , Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation/methods , Brain/physiology , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Adult , Cognition/physiology , Brain Mapping , Learning/physiology , Theta Rhythm/physiology , Verbal Learning/physiology , Neural Pathways/physiology
2.
J Physiol ; 602(10): 2343-2358, 2024 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38654583

ABSTRACT

Training rodents in a particularly difficult olfactory-discrimination (OD) task results in the acquisition of the ability to perform the task well, termed 'rule learning'. In addition to enhanced intrinsic excitability and synaptic excitation in piriform cortex pyramidal neurons, rule learning results in increased synaptic inhibition across the whole cortical network to the point where it precisely maintains the balance between inhibition and excitation. The mechanism underlying such precise inhibitory enhancement remains to be explored. Here, we use brain slices from transgenic mice (VGAT-ChR2-EYFP), enabling optogenetic stimulation of single GABAergic neurons and recordings of unitary synaptic events in pyramidal neurons. Quantal analysis revealed that learning-induced enhanced inhibition is mediated by increased quantal size of the evoked inhibitory events. Next, we examined the plasticity of synaptic inhibition induced by long-lasting, intrinsically evoked spike firing in post-synaptic neurons. Repetitive depolarizing current pulses from depolarized (-70 mV) or hyperpolarized (-90 mV) membrane potentials induced long-term depression (LTD) and long-term potentiation (LTP) of synaptic inhibition, respectively. We found a profound bidirectional increase in the ability to induce both LTD, mediated by L-type calcium channels, and LTP, mediated by R-type calcium channels after rule learning. Blocking the GABAB receptor reversed the effect of intrinsic stimulation at -90 mV from LTP to LTD. We suggest that learning greatly enhances the ability to modify the strength of synaptic inhibition of principal neurons in both directions. Such plasticity of synaptic plasticity allows fine-tuning of inhibition on each particular neuron, thereby stabilizing the network while maintaining the memory of the rule. KEY POINTS: Olfactory discrimination rule learning results in long-lasting enhancement of synaptic inhibition on piriform cortex pyramidal neurons. Quantal analysis of unitary inhibitory synaptic events, evoked by optogenetic minimal stimulation, revealed that enhanced synaptic inhibition is mediated by increased quantal size. Surprisingly, metaplasticity of synaptic inhibition, induced by intrinsically evoked repetitive spike firing, is increased bidirectionally. The susceptibility to both long-term depression (LTD) and long-term potentiation (LTP) of inhibition is enhanced after learning. LTD of synaptic inhibition is mediated by L-type calcium channels and LTP by R-type calcium channels. LTP is also dependent on activation of GABAB receptors. We suggest that learning-induced changes in the metaplasticity of synaptic inhibition enable the fine-tuning of inhibition on each particular neuron, thereby stabilizing the network while maintaining the memory of the rule.


Subject(s)
Mice, Transgenic , Neuronal Plasticity , Pyramidal Cells , Animals , Neuronal Plasticity/physiology , Mice , Pyramidal Cells/physiology , GABAergic Neurons/physiology , Learning/physiology , Long-Term Potentiation/physiology , Male , Synapses/physiology , Optogenetics , Neural Inhibition/physiology , Piriform Cortex/physiology , Mice, Inbred C57BL , Long-Term Synaptic Depression/physiology
3.
Dev Sci ; 27(4): e13498, 2024 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38517035

ABSTRACT

Children achieve better long-term language outcomes than adults. However, it remains unclear whether children actually learn language more quickly than adults during real-time exposure to input-indicative of true superior language learning abilities-or whether this advantage stems from other factors. To examine this issue, we compared the rate at which children (8-10 years) and adults extracted a novel, hidden linguistic rule, in which novel articles probabilistically predicted the animacy of associated nouns (e.g., "gi lion"). Participants categorized these two-word phrases according to a second, explicitly instructed rule over two sessions, separated by an overnight delay. Both children and adults successfully learned the hidden animacy rule through mere exposure to the phrases, showing slower response times and decreased accuracy to occasional phrases that violated the rule. Critically, sensitivity to the hidden rule emerged much more quickly in children than adults; children showed a processing cost for violation trials from very early on in learning, whereas adults did not show reliable sensitivity to the rule until the second session. Children also showed superior generalization of the hidden animacy rule when asked to classify nonword trials (e.g., "gi badupi") according to the hidden animacy rule. Children and adults showed similar retention of the hidden rule over the delay period. These results provide insight into the nature of the critical period for language, suggesting that children have a true advantage over adults in the rate of implicit language learning. Relative to adults, children more rapidly extract hidden linguistic structures during real-time language exposure. RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: Children and adults both succeeded in implicitly learning a novel, uninstructed linguistic rule, based solely on exposure to input. Children learned the novel linguistic rules much more quickly than adults. Children showed better generalization performance than adults when asked to apply the novel rule to nonsense words without semantic content. Results provide insight into the nature of critical period effects in language, indicating that children have an advantage over adults in real-time language learning.


Subject(s)
Language Development , Linguistics , Humans , Child , Adult , Male , Female , Reaction Time/physiology , Learning , Young Adult
4.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 248: 106046, 2024 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39241321

ABSTRACT

Learning in the everyday environment often requires the flexible integration of relevant multisensory information. Previous research has demonstrated preverbal infants' capacity to extract an abstract rule from audiovisual temporal sequences matched in temporal synchrony. Interestingly, this capacity was recently reported to be modulated by crossmodal correspondence beyond spatiotemporal matching (e.g., consistent facial emotional expressions or articulatory mouth movements matched with sound). To investigate whether such modulatory influence applies to non-social and non-communicative stimuli, we conducted a critical test using audiovisual stimuli free of social information: visually upward (and downward) moving objects paired with a congruent tone of ascending or incongruent (descending) pitch. East Asian infants (8-10 months old) from a metropolitan area in Asia demonstrated successful abstract rule learning in the congruent audiovisual condition and demonstrated weaker learning in the incongruent condition. This implies that preverbal infants use crossmodal dynamic pitch-height correspondence to integrate multisensory information before rule extraction. This result confirms that preverbal infants are ready to use non-social non-communicative information in serving cognitive functions such as rule extraction in a multisensory context.


Subject(s)
Pitch Perception , Humans , Infant , Male , Female , Pitch Perception/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Learning/physiology , Child Development/physiology , Communication , Photic Stimulation , Acoustic Stimulation
5.
Neuroimage ; 282: 120393, 2023 11 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37820861

ABSTRACT

In real-life communication, individuals use language that carries evident rewarding and punishing elements, such as praise and criticism. A common trend is to seek more praise while avoiding criticism. Furthermore, semantics is crucial for conveying information, but such semantic access to native and foreign languages is subtly distinct. To investigate how rule learning occurs in different languages and to highlight the importance of semantics in this process, we investigated both verbal and non-verbal rule learning in first (L1) and second (L2) languages using a reinforcement learning framework, including a semantic rule and a color rule. Our computational modeling on behavioral and brain imaging data revealed that individuals may be more motivated to learn and adhere to rules in an L1 compared to L2, with greater striatum activation during the outcome phase in the L1. Additionally, results on the learning rates and inverse temperature in the two rule learning tasks showed that individuals tend to be conservative and are reluctant to change their judgments regarding rule learning of semantic information. Moreover, the greater the prediction errors, the greater activation of the right superior temporal gyrus in the semantic-rule learning condition, demonstrating that such learning has differential neural correlates than symbolic rule learning. Overall, the findings provide insight into the neural mechanisms underlying rule learning in different languages, and indicate that rule learning involving verbal semantics is not a general symbolic learning that resembles a conditioned stimulus-response, but rather has its own specific characteristics.


Subject(s)
Learning , Semantics , Humans , Language , Brain/physiology , Temporal Lobe/diagnostic imaging , Temporal Lobe/physiology , Brain Mapping , Magnetic Resonance Imaging
6.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 44(9): 3897-3912, 2023 06 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37126607

ABSTRACT

Learning and recognition can be improved by sorting novel items into categories and subcategories. Such hierarchical categorization is easy when it can be performed according to learned rules (e.g., "if car, then automatic or stick shift" or "if boat, then motor or sail"). Here, we present results showing that human participants acquire categorization rules for new visual hierarchies rapidly, and that, as they do, corresponding hierarchical representations of the categorized stimuli emerge in patterns of neural activation in the dorsal striatum and in posterior frontal and parietal cortex. Participants learned to categorize novel visual objects into a hierarchy with superordinate and subordinate levels based on the objects' shape features, without having been told the categorization rules for doing so. On each trial, participants were asked to report the category and subcategory of the object, after which they received feedback about the correctness of their categorization responses. Participants trained over the course of a one-hour-long session while their brain activation was measured using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Over the course of training, significant hierarchy learning took place as participants discovered the nested categorization rules, as evidenced by the occurrence of a learning trial, after which performance suddenly increased. This learning was associated with increased representational strength of the newly acquired hierarchical rules in a corticostriatal network including the posterior frontal and parietal cortex and the dorsal striatum. We also found evidence suggesting that reinforcement learning in the dorsal striatum contributed to hierarchical rule learning.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping , Parietal Lobe , Humans , Brain Mapping/methods , Parietal Lobe/diagnostic imaging , Parietal Lobe/physiology , Learning/physiology , Brain/physiology , Reinforcement, Psychology , Magnetic Resonance Imaging
7.
Dev Sci ; 26(1): e13244, 2023 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35172393

ABSTRACT

We conducted a close replication of the seminal work by Marcus and colleagues from 1999, which showed that after a brief auditory exposure phase, 7-month-old infants were able to learn and generalize a rule to novel syllables not previously present in the exposure phase. This work became the foundation for the theoretical framework by which we assume that infants are able to learn abstract representations and generalize linguistic rules. While some extensions on the original work have shown evidence of rule learning, the outcomes are mixed, and an exact replication of Marcus et al.'s study has thus far not been reported. A recent meta-analysis by Rabagliati and colleagues brings to light that the rule-learning effect depends on stimulus type (e.g., meaningfulness, speech vs. nonspeech) and is not as robust as often assumed. In light of the theoretical importance of the issue at stake, it is appropriate and necessary to assess the replicability and robustness of Marcus et al.'s findings. Here we have undertaken a replication across four labs with a large sample of 7-month-old infants (N = 96), using the same exposure patterns (ABA and ABB), methodology (Headturn Preference Paradigm), and original stimuli. As in the original study, we tested the hypothesis that infants are able to learn abstract "algebraic" rules and apply them to novel input. Our results did not replicate the original findings: infants showed no difference in looking time between test patterns consistent or inconsistent with the familiarization pattern they were exposed to.


Subject(s)
Learning , Speech , Infant , Humans
8.
Multivariate Behav Res ; : 1-14, 2023 Feb 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36762914

ABSTRACT

This study is the first to investigate how 3-year-olds learn simple rules from feedback using the Toddler Card Sorting Task (TCST). To account for intra- and inter- individual differences in the learning process, latent Markov models were fitted to the time series of accuracy responses using maximum likelihood techniques (Visser et al., 2002). In a first, exploratory study (N = 110, 3- to 5-years olds) a considerable group of 3-year olds applied a hypothesis testing learning strategy. A second study confirmed these results with a preregistered study (3-years olds, N = 60). Under supportive learning conditions, a majority of 3-year- olds was capable of hypothesis testing. Furthermore, older children and those with bigger working memory capacities were more likely to use hypothesis testing, even though the latter group perseverated more than younger children or those with smaller working memory capacities. 3-year-olds are more advanced feedback-learners than assumed.

9.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 213: 105270, 2022 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34487976

ABSTRACT

Developmental studies have shown that infants exploit ordinal information to extract and generalize repetition-based rules from a sequence of items. Within the visual modality, this ability is constrained by the spatial layout within which items are delivered given that a left-to-right orientation boosts infants' rule learning, whereas a right-to-left orientation hinders this ability. Infants' rule learning operates across different domains and can also be transferred across modalities when learning is triggered by speech. However, no studies have investigated whether the transfer of rule learning occurs across different domains when language is not involved. Using a visual habituation procedure, we tested 7-month-old infants' ability to extract rule-like patterns from numerical sequences and generalize them to non-numerical sequences of visual shapes and whether this ability is affected by the spatial orientation. Infants were first habituated to left-to-right or right-to-left oriented numerical sequences instantiating an ABB rule and were then tested with the familiar rule instantiated across sequences of single geometrical shapes and a novel (ABA) rule. Results showed a transfer of learning from number to visual shapes for left-to-right oriented sequences but not for right-to-left oriented ones (Experiment 1) even when the direction of the numerical change (increasing vs. decreasing) within the habituation sequences violated a small-left/large-right number-space association (Experiment 2). These results provide the first demonstration that visual rule learning mechanisms in infancy operate at a high level of abstraction and confirm earlier findings that left-to-right oriented directional cues facilitate infants' representation of order.


Subject(s)
Child Development , Speech , Humans , Infant , Language , Space Perception
10.
Appl Intell (Dordr) ; 52(14): 16900-16915, 2022.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35370359

ABSTRACT

Drivers' improper driving behavior plays a vital role in road accidents. Different approaches have been proposed to classify and evaluate driving performance to ensure road safety. However, most of the techniques are based on neural networks which work like a black box and make the logical reasoning behind the classification decision unclear. In this paper, we propose a rule-based machine learning technique using a sequential covering algorithm to classify the driving maneuvers from time-series data. In the sequential covering algorithm, the impact of each rule is measured as the metrics of coverage and accuracy, where the coverage and accuracy indicate the amount of covered and correctly identified instances in a maneuver class, respectively. The final ruleset for each maneuver class is formed with only the significant rules. In this way, the rules are learned in an unsupervised manner and only the best performance of the rules are included in the ruleset. The set of rules is also optimized by pruning based on the performance of the test data. Application of the proposed system is beneficial compared to the traditional machine learning and deep learning approaches which typically require a larger dataset and higher computational time and complexity.

11.
Dev Sci ; 24(5): e13088, 2021 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33484594

ABSTRACT

Previous work has shown that infants as young as 8 months of age can use certain features of the environment, such as the shape or color of visual stimuli, as cues to organize simple inputs into hierarchical rule structures, a robust form of reinforcement learning that supports generalization of prior learning to new contexts. However, especially in cluttered naturalistic environments, there are an abundance of potential cues that can be used to structure learning into hierarchical rule structures. It is unclear how infants determine what features constitute a higher-order context to organize inputs into hierarchical rule structures. Here, we examine whether 9-month-old infants are biased to use social stimuli, relative to non-social stimuli, as a higher-order context to organize learning of simple visuospatial inputs into hierarchical rule sets. Infants were presented with four face/color-target location pairings, which could be learned most simply as individual associations. Alternatively, infants could use the faces or colorful backgrounds as a higher-order context to organize the inputs into simpler color-location or face-location rules, respectively. Infants were then given a generalization test designed to probe how they learned the initial pairings. The results indicated that infants appeared to use the faces as a higher-order context to organize simpler color-location rules, which then supported generalization of learning to new face contexts. These findings provide new evidence that infants are biased to organize reinforcement learning around social stimuli.


Subject(s)
Generalization, Psychological , Learning , Cues , Humans , Infant
12.
J Biomed Inform ; 117: 103691, 2021 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33610882

ABSTRACT

Survival data analysis has been leveraged in medical research to study disease morbidity and mortality, and to discover significant bio-markers affecting them. A crucial objective in studying high dimensional medical data is the development of inherently interpretable models that can efficiently capture sparse underlying signals while retaining a high predictive accuracy. Recently developed rule ensemble models have been shown to effectively accomplish this objective; however, they are computationally expensive when applied to survival data and do not account for sparsity in the number of variables included in the generated rules. To address these gaps, we present SURVFIT, a "doubly sparse" rule extraction formulation for survival data. This doubly sparse method can induce sparsity both in the number of rules and in the number of variables involved in the rules. Our method has the computational efficiency needed to realistically solve the problem of rule-extraction from survival data if we consider both rule sparsity and variable sparsity, by adopting a quadratic loss function with an overlapping group regularization. Further, a systematic rule evaluation framework that includes statistical testing, decomposition analysis and sensitivity analysis is provided. We demonstrate the utility of SURVFIT via experiments carried out on a synthetic dataset and a sepsis survival dataset from MIMIC-III.


Subject(s)
Algorithms , Learning
13.
Genomics ; 112(3): 2524-2534, 2020 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32045671

ABSTRACT

The development of embryonic cells involves several continuous stages, and some genes are related to embryogenesis. To date, few studies have systematically investigated changes in gene expression profiles during mammalian embryogenesis. In this study, a computational analysis using machine learning algorithms was performed on the gene expression profiles of mouse embryonic cells at seven stages. First, the profiles were analyzed through a powerful Monte Carlo feature selection method for the generation of a feature list. Second, increment feature selection was applied on the list by incorporating two classification algorithms: support vector machine (SVM) and repeated incremental pruning to produce error reduction (RIPPER). Through SVM, we extracted several latent gene biomarkers, indicating the stages of embryonic cells, and constructed an optimal SVM classifier that produced a nearly perfect classification of embryonic cells. Furthermore, some interesting rules were accessed by the RIPPER algorithm, suggesting different expression patterns for different stages.


Subject(s)
Embryo, Mammalian/metabolism , Embryonic Development/genetics , Machine Learning , Transcriptome , Animals , Gene Expression Profiling , Mice , Single-Cell Analysis , Support Vector Machine
14.
Infancy ; 26(3): 442-454, 2021 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33709450

ABSTRACT

Rule learning (RL) refers to infants' ability to extract high-order, repetition-based rules from a sequence of elements and to generalize them to new items. RL has been demonstrated in both the auditory and the visual modality, but no studies have investigated infants' transfer of learning across these two modalities, a process that is fundamental for the development of many complex cognitive skills. Using a visual habituation procedure within a cross-modal RL task, we tested 7-month-old infants' transfer of learning both from speech to vision (auditory-visual-AV-condition) and from vision to speech (visual-auditory-VA-condition). Results showed a transfer of learning in the AV condition, but only for those infants who were able to efficiently extract the rule during the learning (habituation) phase. In contrast, in the VA condition infants provided no evidence of RL. Overall, this study indicates that 7-month-old infants can transfers high-order rules across modalities with an advantage for transferring from speech to vision, and that this ability is constrained by infants' individual differences in the way they process the to-be-learned rules.


Subject(s)
Speech , Transfer, Psychology , Humans , Infant , Learning , Linguistics
15.
Proc Biol Sci ; 287(1932): 20201262, 2020 08 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32781947

ABSTRACT

We humans sort the world around us into conceptual groups, such as 'the same' or 'different', which facilitates many cognitive tasks. Applying such abstract concepts can improve problem-solving success and is therefore worth the cognitive investment. In this study, we investigated whether ants (Lasius niger) can learn the relational rule of 'the same' or 'different' by training them in an odour match-to-sample test over 48 visits. While ants in the 'different' treatment improved significantly over time, reaching around 65% correct decisions, ants in the 'same' treatment did not. Ants did not seem able to learn such abstract relational concepts, but instead created their own individual strategy to try to solve the problem: some ants decided to 'always go left', others preferred a 'go to the more salient cue' heuristic which systematically biased their decisions. These heuristics even occasionally lowered the success rate in the experiment below chance, indicating that following any rule may be more desirable then making truly random decisions. As the finding that ants resort to heuristics when facing hard-to-solve decisions was discovered post-hoc, we strongly encourage other researchers to ask whether employing heuristics in the face of challenging tasks is a widespread phenomenon in insects.


Subject(s)
Ants/physiology , Heuristics , Learning , Animals , Behavior, Animal , Odorants , Problem Solving
16.
J Biomed Inform ; 107: 103455, 2020 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32497685

ABSTRACT

Modeling factors influencing disease phenotypes, from biomarker profiling study datasets, is a critical task in biomedicine. Such datasets are typically generated from high-throughput 'omic' technologies, which help examine disease mechanisms at an unprecedented resolution. These datasets are challenging because they are high-dimensional. The disease mechanisms they study are also complex because many diseases are multifactorial, resulting from the collective activity of several factors, each with a small effect. Bayesian rule learning (BRL) is a rule model inferred from learning Bayesian networks from data, and has been shown to be effective in modeling high-dimensional datasets. However, BRL is not efficient at modeling multifactorial diseases since it suffers from data fragmentation during learning. In this paper, we overcome this limitation by implementing and evaluating three types of ensemble model combination strategies with BRL- uniform combination (UC; same as Bagging), Bayesian model averaging (BMA), and Bayesian model combination (BMC)- collectively called Ensemble Bayesian Rule Learning (EBRL). We also introduce a novel method to visualize EBRL models, called the Bayesian Rule Ensemble Visualizing tool (BREVity), which helps extract interpret the most important rule patterns guiding the predictions made by the ensemble model. Our results using twenty-five public, high-dimensional, gene expression datasets of multifactorial diseases, suggest that, both EBRL models using UC and BMC achieve better predictive performance than BMA and other classic machine learning methods. Furthermore, BMC is found to be more reliable than UC, when the ensemble includes sub-optimal models resulting from the stochasticity of the model search process. Together, EBRL and BREVity provides researchers a promising and novel tool for modeling multifactorial diseases from high-dimensional datasets that leverages strengths of ensemble methods for predictive performance, while also providing interpretable explanations for its predictions.


Subject(s)
Machine Learning , Bayes Theorem
17.
Anim Cogn ; 22(5): 825-838, 2019 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31264123

ABSTRACT

Strategies used in artificial grammar learning can shed light into the abilities of different species to extract regularities from the environment. In the A(X)nB rule, A and B items are linked, but assigned to different positional categories and separated by distractor items. Open questions are how widespread is the ability to extract positional regularities from A(X)nB patterns, which strategies are used to encode positional regularities and whether individuals exhibit preferences for absolute or relative position encoding. We used visual arrays to investigate whether cotton-top tamarins (Saguinusoedipus) can learn this rule and which strategies they use. After training on a subset of exemplars, two of the tested monkeys successfully generalized to novel combinations. These tamarins discriminated between categories of tokens with different properties (A, B, X) and detected a positional relationship between non-adjacent items even in the presence of novel distractors. The pattern of errors revealed that successful subjects used visual similarity with training stimuli to solve the task and that successful tamarins extracted the relative position of As and Bs rather than their absolute position, similarly to what has been observed in other species. Relative position encoding appears to be favoured in different tasks and taxa. Generalization, though, was incomplete, since we observed a failure with items that during training had always been presented in reinforced arrays, showing the limitations in grasping the underlying positional rule. These results suggest the use of local strategies in the extraction of positional rules in cotton-top tamarins.


Subject(s)
Learning , Reinforcement, Psychology , Saguinus , Animals , Female , Male , Movement
18.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 113(27): E3977-84, 2016 07 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27325756

ABSTRACT

The ability to abstract a regularity that underlies strings of sounds is a core mechanism of the language faculty but might not be specific to language learning or even to humans. It is unclear whether and to what extent nonhuman animals possess the ability to abstract regularities defining the relation among arbitrary auditory items in a string and to generalize this abstraction to strings of acoustically novel items. In this study we tested these abilities in a songbird (zebra finch) and a parrot species (budgerigar). Subjects were trained in a go/no-go design to discriminate between two sets of sound strings arranged in an XYX or an XXY structure. After this discrimination was acquired, each subject was tested with test strings that were structurally identical to the training strings but consisted of either new combinations of known elements or of novel elements belonging to other element categories. Both species learned to discriminate between the two stimulus sets. However, their responses to the test strings were strikingly different. Zebra finches categorized test stimuli with previously heard elements by the ordinal position that these elements occupied in the training strings, independent of string structure. In contrast, the budgerigars categorized both novel combinations of familiar elements as well as strings consisting of novel element types by their underlying structure. They thus abstracted the relation among items in the XYX and XXY structures, an ability similar to that shown by human infants and indicating a level of abstraction comparable to analogical reasoning.


Subject(s)
Finches , Language , Learning , Melopsittacus , Speech Perception , Animals , Cognition , Female , Language Development , Male
19.
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci ; 18(5): 1034-1048, 2018 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29943175

ABSTRACT

When learning rule-based categories, sufficient cognitive resources are needed to test hypotheses, maintain the currently active rule in working memory, update rules after feedback, and to select a new rule if necessary. Prior research has demonstrated that conjunctive rules are more complex than unidimensional rules and place greater demands on executive functions like working memory. In our study, event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded while participants performed a conjunctive rule-based category learning task with trial-by-trial feedback. In line with prior research, correct categorization responses resulted in a larger stimulus-locked late positive complex compared to incorrect responses, possibly indexing the updating of rule information in memory. Incorrect trials elicited a pronounced feedback-locked P300 elicited which suggested a disconnect between perception, and the rule-based strategy. We also examined the differential processing of stimuli that were able to be correctly classified by the suboptimal single-dimensional rule ("easy" stimuli) versus those that could only be correctly classified by the optimal, conjunctive rule ("difficult" stimuli). Among strong learners, a larger, late positive slow wave emerged for difficult compared with easy stimuli, suggesting differential processing of category items even though strong learners performed well on the conjunctive category set. Overall, the findings suggest that ERP combined with computational modelling can be used to better understand the cognitive processes involved in rule-based category learning.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Cognition/physiology , Learning/physiology , Computer Simulation , Electroencephalography , Evoked Potentials , Female , Humans , Male , Young Adult
20.
J Neurosci ; 36(40): 10314-10322, 2016 10 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27707968

ABSTRACT

Recent research indicates that adults and infants spontaneously create and generalize hierarchical rule sets during incidental learning. Computational models and empirical data suggest that, in adults, this process is supported by circuits linking prefrontal cortex (PFC) with striatum and their modulation by dopamine, but the neural circuits supporting this form of learning in infants are largely unknown. We used near-infrared spectroscopy to record PFC activity in 8-month-old human infants during a simple audiovisual hierarchical-rule-learning task. Behavioral results confirmed that infants adopted hierarchical rule sets to learn and generalize spoken object-label mappings across different speaker contexts. Infants had increased activity over right dorsal lateral PFC when rule sets switched from one trial to the next, a neural marker related to updating rule sets into working memory in the adult literature. Infants' eye blink rate, a possible physiological correlate of striatal dopamine activity, also increased when rule sets switched from one trial to the next. Moreover, the increase in right dorsolateral PFC activity in conjunction with eye blink rate also predicted infants' generalization ability, providing exploratory evidence for frontostriatal involvement during learning. These findings provide evidence that PFC is involved in rudimentary hierarchical rule learning in 8-month-old infants, an ability that was previously thought to emerge later in life in concert with PFC maturation. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT: Hierarchical rule learning is a powerful learning mechanism that allows rules to be selected in a context-appropriate fashion and transferred or reused in novel contexts. Data from computational models and adults suggests that this learning mechanism is supported by dopamine-innervated interactions between prefrontal cortex (PFC) and striatum. Here, we provide evidence that PFC also supports hierarchical rule learning during infancy, challenging the current dogma that PFC is an underdeveloped brain system until adolescence. These results add new insights into the neurobiological mechanisms available to support learning and generalization in very early postnatal life, providing evidence that PFC and the frontostriatal circuitry are involved in organizing learning and behavior earlier in life than previously known.


Subject(s)
Generalization, Psychological/physiology , Learning/physiology , Prefrontal Cortex/physiology , Blinking/physiology , Brain Chemistry , Brain Mapping , Female , Frontal Lobe/physiology , Functional Laterality/physiology , Humans , Infant , Male , Neostriatum/physiology , Neural Pathways/physiology , Prefrontal Cortex/chemistry , Psychomotor Performance , Spectroscopy, Near-Infrared
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL