Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 108
Filter
1.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 114(14): E2806-E2815, 2017 04 04.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28320968

ABSTRACT

Certain numerical abilities appear to be relatively ubiquitous in the animal kingdom, including the ability to recognize and differentiate relative quantities. This skill is present in human adults and children, as well as in nonhuman primates and, perhaps surprisingly, is also demonstrated by lower species such as mosquitofish and spiders, despite the absence of cortical computation available to primates. This ubiquity of numerical competence suggests that representations that connect to numerical tasks are likely subserved by evolutionarily conserved regions of the nervous system. Here, we test the hypothesis that the evaluation of relative numerical quantities is subserved by lower-order brain structures in humans. Using a monocular/dichoptic paradigm, across four experiments, we show that the discrimination of displays, consisting of both large (5-80) and small (1-4) numbers of dots, is facilitated in the monocular, subcortical portions of the visual system. This is only the case, however, when observers evaluate larger ratios of 3:1 or 4:1, but not smaller ratios, closer to 1:1. This profile of competence matches closely the skill with which newborn infants and other species can discriminate numerical quantity. These findings suggest conservation of ontogenetically and phylogenetically lower-order systems in adults' numerical abilities. The involvement of subcortical structures in representing numerical quantities provokes a reconsideration of current theories of the neural basis of numerical cognition, inasmuch as it bolsters the cross-species continuity of the biological system for numerical abilities.


Subject(s)
Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Psychophysics/methods , Visual Perception/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Mathematics , Middle Aged , Nontherapeutic Human Experimentation , Photic Stimulation , Psychophysics/instrumentation , Visual Cortex/physiology
2.
Muscle Nerve ; 59(2): 229-235, 2019 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30390398

ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION: In this study we assessed the test-retest reliability of a Rydel-Seiffer tuning fork and an electronic vibrameter with hand-held and fixed probes. METHODS: Fifty healthy volunteers were assessed twice in the upper and lower limb 15 minutes apart. Reliability was assessed by intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) and standard error of measurement (SEM). The effect of stimulus parameters on vibration disappearance threshold (VDT) was assessed by analysis of variance. RESULTS: All 3 tools showed good reliability (ICCs = 0.65-0.95). Vibrameter recordings with the fixed probe showed high variability. The vibrameter was more sensitive in detecting body-site and age differences in vibration thresholds than the tuning fork. Significantly higher VDT was observed when higher starting amplitudes and slower rates of change were used. DISCUSSION: The hand-held vibrameter is a superior tool to monitor vibration sense. The stimulus amplitude and rate of change are important to control as they alter VDT. Muscle Nerve 59:229-235, 2019.


Subject(s)
Lower Extremity/physiology , Perception/physiology , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Vibration , Adult , Female , Healthy Volunteers , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Neural Conduction/physiology , Neurologic Examination , Psychophysics/instrumentation , Psychophysics/methods , Reference Values , Reproducibility of Results , Young Adult
3.
Perception ; 45(9): 1070-83, 2016 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27271338

ABSTRACT

Precise timing and presentation of stimuli is critical in vision research, still, the limiting factor in successful recognition is often the monitor itself that is used to present the stimuli. The most widespread method is the use of monitors controlled by personal computers. Traditionally, most experiments used cathode-ray tubes but they are more and more difficult to access, and instead, liquid-crystal displays are getting more and more popular. The two types have fundamentally different working principles and limitations in displaying the stimulus.In our experiments, the temporal precision of the stimulus presentation was in focus. We investigated whether liquid-crystal displays, which are not considered to be fit to display fast successive stimuli, can represent an alternative choice for cathode-ray tubes. We used the double flash and the flicker illusion to compare the technical capabilities of the two monitor types. These illusions not only do require a precise timing but also a very short exposure to the stimuli. At the same time, the interstimulus interval is also of extreme importance. In addition, these illusions require peripheral stimulation of the retina, which is more sensitive to the temporal aspects of the visual stimulus. On the basis of previous studies and our own psychophysical results, we suggest that liquid-crystal displays might be a good alternative for precise, frame-to-frame stimulus presentation even if parts of the stimuli are projected on the peripheral retina.


Subject(s)
Data Display , Illusions/physiology , Psychophysics/instrumentation , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Liquid Crystals , Male , Young Adult
4.
Laterality ; 20(1): 22-48, 2015.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24821308

ABSTRACT

Neuropsychological and neuroimaging research has established that knowledge related to tool use and tool recognition is lateralized to the left cerebral hemisphere. Recently, behavioural studies with the visual half-field technique have confirmed the lateralization. A limitation of this research was that different sets of stimuli had to be used for the comparison of tools to other objects and objects to non-objects. Therefore, we developed a new set of stimuli containing matched triplets of tools, other objects and non-objects. With the new stimulus set, we successfully replicated the findings of no visual field advantage for objects in an object recognition task combined with a significant right visual field advantage for tools in a tool recognition task. The set of stimuli is available as supplemental data to this article.


Subject(s)
Functional Laterality , Photic Stimulation/instrumentation , Psychophysics/instrumentation , Analysis of Variance , Humans , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Reaction Time , Recognition, Psychology
5.
Int J Audiol ; 53(12): 887-94, 2014 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25156231

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: Psychophysical tuning curves (PTCs) have been used predominantly in laboratory settings to assess frequency selectivity in the auditory system. Recently, a fast-PTC procedure has been developed for diagnosis of cochlear dead regions. In this study the clinical feasibility of using fast PTCs in adults was investigated. Success rate (the number of successes per number of attempts) and repeatability were assessed. The range of the tip frequency (ftip) shift was established and different methods of ftip estimation compared. DESIGN: Fast PTCs were measured for signal frequencies (fs) of 500, 1000, 2000, 3000, and 4000 Hz on two occasions using an upward-sweeping masker. Five methods were used to estimate ftip for each PTC. STUDY SAMPLE: Thirty-two adults with normal hearing were tested. RESULTS: All participants were able to successfully complete the task, the majority of them with minimal training. The moving average, quadratic function, and double lowpass filtering methods had the highest success rate in ftip estimation. The quadratic function method had the smallest 95% range of - 3.4% fs to 10.2% fs and the best test-retest reliability of 5.1% fs. CONCLUSIONS: Fast PTCs show potential for clinical use due to a high success rate with minimal training required. We suggest the quadratic function method for routine clinical use as it had the smallest 95% range, a high success rate in ftip estimation and the best test-retest reliability. For fast PTCs measured for signal frequencies from 500 to 4000 Hz using an upward-sweeping masker, we suggest the normative range of ftip to be - 3% fs to 10% fs with a test-retest reliability of 5% fs.


Subject(s)
Auditory Threshold/physiology , Psychophysics/instrumentation , Psychophysics/methods , Acoustic Stimulation , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Cochlea/physiopathology , Female , Hearing/physiology , Hearing Loss, High-Frequency/diagnosis , Hearing Loss, High-Frequency/physiopathology , Humans , Male , Reproducibility of Results
6.
Sensors (Basel) ; 14(10): 19785-805, 2014 Oct 22.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25340451

ABSTRACT

A photo-realistic representation of material appearance can be achieved by means of bidirectional texture function (BTF) capturing a material's appearance for varying illumination, viewing directions, and spatial pixel coordinates. BTF captures many non-local effects in material structure such as inter-reflections, occlusions, shadowing, or scattering. The acquisition of BTF data is usually time and resource-intensive due to the high dimensionality of BTF data. This results in expensive, complex measurement setups and/or excessively long measurement times. We propose an approximate BTF acquisition setup based on a simple, affordable mechanical gantry containing a consumer camera and two LED lights. It captures a very limited subset of material surface images by shooting several video sequences. A psychophysical study comparing captured and reconstructed data with the reference BTFs of seven tested materials revealed that results of our method show a promising visual quality. Speed of the setup has been demonstrated on measurement of human skin and measurement and modeling of a glue dessication time-varying process. As it allows for fast, inexpensive, acquisition of approximate BTFs, this method can be beneficial to visualization applications demanding less accuracy, where BTF utilization has previously been limited.


Subject(s)
Image Enhancement , Psychophysics/instrumentation , Humans , Light , Psychophysics/methods
7.
Behav Res Methods ; 46(2): 499-516, 2014 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24197711

ABSTRACT

A method for creating and presenting video-recorded synchronized audiovisual stimuli at a high frame rate-which would be highly useful for psychophysical studies on, for example, just-noticeable differences and gating-is presented. Methods for accomplishing this include recording audio and video separately using an exact synchronization signal, editing the recordings and finding exact synchronization points, and presenting the synchronized audiovisual stimuli with a desired frame rate on a cathode ray tube display using MATLAB and Psychophysics Toolbox 3. The methods from an empirical gating study (Moradi, Lidestam, & Rönnberg, Frontiers in Psychology 4:359, 2013) are presented as an example of the implementation of playback at 120 fps.


Subject(s)
Audiovisual Aids , Data Display , Psychophysics/methods , Video Recording , Motion , Motion Pictures , Movement , Psychophysics/instrumentation , Software , Software Design , Time Factors , Video Recording/instrumentation
8.
J Appl Res Intellect Disabil ; 27(3): 247-63, 2014 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23784802

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: In individuals with Down syndrome, virtually all structures of the eye have some abnormality, which likely diminishes vision. We examined basic vision functions in adults with Down syndrome. MATERIALS AND METHODS: Participants completed a battery of psychophysical tests that probed a comprehensive array of visual functions. The performance of adults with Down syndrome was compared with younger and older adults without intellectual disability. RESULTS: Adults with Down syndrome had significant vision deficits, reduced sensitivity across spatial frequencies and temporal modulation rates, reduced stereopsis, impaired vernier acuity and anomalies in colour discrimination. The pattern of deficits observed was similar to those seen by researchers examining adults with Alzheimer's disease. CONCLUSIONS: Our findings suggest that a common mechanism may be responsible for the pattern of deficits observed, possibly the presence of Alzheimer's disease neuropathology in the visual association cortex. We also showed that individuals with mild to moderate intellectual disability are capable of participating in studies employing state-of-the-art psychophysical procedures. This has wider implications in terms of their ability to participate in research that use similar techniques.


Subject(s)
Down Syndrome/physiopathology , Vision Disorders/diagnosis , Vision Tests/methods , Adult , Color Vision/physiology , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Depth Perception/physiology , Down Syndrome/complications , Humans , Middle Aged , Psychophysics/instrumentation , Psychophysics/methods , Random Allocation , Vision Disorders/etiology , Vision Tests/instrumentation , Visual Acuity/physiology , Young Adult
9.
Hist Psychol ; 17(2): 149-58, 2014 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24818744

ABSTRACT

In the fall and winter of 1910, Max Wertheimer (1880-1943) performed his famous experiments on perceived motion, published in 1912. Besides slider experiments he mainly used a wheel tachistoscope developed by Friedrich Schumann (1863-1940) at the end of the 19th century. The Adolf-Wuerth-Center for the History of Psychology has several wheel tachistoscopes in its collection of instruments. Their provenance can be traced back to the Institute of Psychology of the University of Frankfurt and the University of Zurich. It is very plausible that Wertheimer, who performed his experiments at the Frankfurt Institute, used one of them. But the wheel tachistoscope alone is not sufficient to reconstruct Wertheimer's original experiments. As always, the devil is in the details. Wertheimer's descriptions of the necessary accessories, a prism, a viewing device, and an electric motor to move the wheel, are rather sparse. This article describes the results of a search for traces in the literature, in archives, and in literary depositories to shed some light on Wertheimer's experimental equipment. As a result, it was possible to reconstruct the entire apparatus and to obtain the same optical impressions with the reconstructed devices as Wertheimer's observers reported. In addition, one of his results was replicated with new participants exactly 100 years after its first publication.


Subject(s)
Equipment Design/history , Gestalt Theory/history , Psychophysics/instrumentation , History, 20th Century , Humans , Psychophysics/history
10.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 25(1): 62-73, 2013 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23198891

ABSTRACT

In monocular pattern rivalry, a composite image is shown to both eyes. The patient experiences perceptual alternations in which the two stimulus components alternate in clarity or salience. We used fMRI at 3T to image brain activity while participants perceived monocular rivalry passively or indicated their percepts with a task. The stimulus patterns were left/right oblique gratings, face/house composites, or a nonrivalrous control stimulus that did not support the perception of transparency or image segmentation. All stimuli were matched for luminance, contrast, and color. Compared with the control stimulus, the cortical activation for passive viewing of grating rivalry included dorsal and ventral extrastriate cortex, superior and inferior parietal regions, and multiple sites in frontal cortex. When the BOLD signal for the object rivalry task was compared with the grating rivalry task, a similar whole-brain network was engaged, but with significantly greater activity in extrastriate regions, including V3, V3A, fusiform face area (FFA), and parahippocampal place area (PPA). In addition, for the object rivalry task, FFA activity was significantly greater during face-dominant periods whereas parahippocampal place area activity was greater during house-dominant periods. Our results demonstrate that slight stimulus changes that trigger monocular rivalry recruit a large whole-brain network, as previously identified for other forms of bistability. Moreover, the results indicate that rivalry for complex object stimuli preferentially engages extrastriate cortex. We also establish that even with natural viewing conditions, endogenous attentional fluctuations in monocular pattern rivalry will differentially drive object-category-specific cortex, similar to binocular rivalry, but without complete suppression of the nondominant image.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Vision, Monocular/physiology , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/instrumentation , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Psychophysics/instrumentation , Psychophysics/methods , Retina/physiology , Single-Blind Method
11.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 25(1): 49-61, 2013 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23198890

ABSTRACT

This study examined the cortical representation of vibrotactile detection in humans using event-related fMRI paired with psychophysics. Suprathreshold vibrotactile stimulation activated several areas, including primary (SI) and second somatosensory cortices (SII/PV). For threshold-level stimuli, poststimulus activity in contralateral and ipsilateral SII/PV was the best correlate of detection success. In these areas, evoked signals on hit trials were significantly greater than on missed trials in all participants, and the relative activity level across stimulation amplitudes matched perceptual performance. Activity in the anterior insula and superior temporal gyrus also correlated with hits and misses, suggesting that a "ventral stream" of somatosensory representations may play a crucial role in detection. In contrast, poststimulus activity in Area SI was not well correlated with perception and showed an overall negative response profile for threshold-level stimulation. A different correlate of detection success was, however, observed in SI. Activity in this representation immediately before stimulus onset predicted performance, a finding that was unique to SI. These findings emphasize the potential role of SII/PV in detection, the importance of state dynamics in SI for perception, and the possibility that changes in the temporal and spatial pattern of SI activity may be essential to the optimal representation of threshold-level stimuli for detection.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Psychophysics/methods , Signal Detection, Psychological/physiology , Somatosensory Cortex/physiology , Touch Perception/physiology , Adult , Brain Mapping/instrumentation , Brain Mapping/methods , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/instrumentation , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Psychophysics/instrumentation , Young Adult
12.
Dev Med Child Neurol ; 55(3): 257-63, 2013 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23121196

ABSTRACT

AIM: Sensory function is assumed to recover almost completely in obstetric brachial plexus lesion (OBPL), and is reported to recover much better than motor function. However, there is no obvious physiological reason why this should be so. Any persistent problems with sensory innervation might contribute to disability, therefore we aimed to assess sensory dysfunction in adults resulting from OBPL. METHOD: Adults with conservatively treated OBPL (n=17; 12 females, five males; median age 38y; lesion levels: C5-C6, n=7; C5-C7, n=7; C5-C8, n=2; C5-Th1, n=1) and 19 healthy comparison persons (10 females, nine males; median age 23y) were investigated. Sensory function was measured using Semmes-Weinstein monofilaments, two-point discrimination, object recognition, and a locognosia test. RESULTS: Scores of the Semmes-Weinstein monofilaments and two-point discrimination, but not object recognition or locognosia, were significantly worse in those with OBPL than in those without OBPL. INTERPRETATION: There may be systematic abnormalities in sensory function in adults with conservatively treated OBPL. The existence of these impairments and their contribution to functional impairment needs to be acknowledged.


Subject(s)
Birth Injuries/physiopathology , Brachial Plexus Neuropathies/physiopathology , Hand/physiopathology , Psychophysics/methods , Sensation Disorders/physiopathology , Adult , Birth Injuries/diagnosis , Brachial Plexus Neuropathies/diagnosis , Brachial Plexus Neuropathies/etiology , Cross-Sectional Studies , Female , Fingers/physiopathology , Humans , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Psychophysics/instrumentation , Sensation Disorders/diagnosis , Sensation Disorders/etiology , Young Adult
13.
Optom Vis Sci ; 88(2): E334-43, 2011 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21169875

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: We describe a compact and convenient clinical apparatus for the measurement of suppression based on a previously reported laboratory-based approach. In addition, we report and validate a novel, rapid psychophysical method for measuring suppression using this apparatus, which makes the technique more applicable to clinical practice. METHODS: By using a Z800 dual pro head-mounted display driven by a MAC laptop, we provide dichoptic stimulation. Global motion stimuli composed of arrays of moving dots are presented to each eye. One set of dots move in a coherent direction (termed signal) whereas another set of dots move in a random direction (termed noise). To quantify performance, we measure the signal/noise ratio corresponding to a direction-discrimination threshold. Suppression is quantified by assessing the extent to which it matters which eye sees the signal and which eye sees the noise. RESULTS: A space-saving, head-mounted display using current video technology offers an ideal solution for clinical practice. In addition, our optimized psychophysical method provided results that were in agreement with those produced using the original technique. We made measures of suppression on a group of nine adult amblyopic participants using this apparatus with both the original and new psychophysical paradigms. All participants had measurable suppression ranging from mild to severe. The two different psychophysical methods gave a strong correlation for the strength of suppression (rho = -0.83, p = 0.006). CONCLUSION: Combining the new apparatus and new psychophysical method creates a convenient and rapid technique for parametric measurement of interocular suppression. In addition, this apparatus constitutes the ideal platform for suppressors to combine information between their eyes in a similar way to binocularly normal people. This provides a convenient way for clinicians to implement the newly proposed binocular treatment of amblyopia that is based on antisuppression training.


Subject(s)
Amblyopia/diagnosis , Diagnosis, Computer-Assisted , Photic Stimulation/instrumentation , Psychophysics/instrumentation , Psychophysics/methods , Adult , Aged , Amblyopia/physiopathology , Contrast Sensitivity , Equipment Design , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Motion Perception , Sensory Thresholds , Visual Acuity , Visual Perception
14.
Int J Audiol ; 50(4): 237-42, 2011 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21299376

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: To implement a fast method for measuring psychophysical tuning curves (PTCs) for use in clinical applications, such as assessment of frequency selectivity and detection of dead regions in the cochlea. DESIGN: The method is based on that described by Sek et al (2005) and has been implemented in software that can be run on a PC with a good-quality sound card. In addition to the main narrowband noise masker, a lowpass noise masker can be generated to prevent detection of a distortion band corresponding to the simple difference tone. RESULTS: The software includes a routine for measuring the absolute threshold at the signal frequency and includes methods for estimating the frequency at the tip of the PTC. A PTC can typically be determined in about three minutes. A small amount of practice (two to three runs) may be required to achieve stable results. CONCLUSIONS: The software implementation allows PTCs to be measured quickly without a requirement for specialised equipment.


Subject(s)
Audiometry/methods , Auditory Perception , Cochlea/physiopathology , Hearing Loss/diagnosis , Psychophysics/methods , Acoustic Stimulation , Audiometry/instrumentation , Auditory Threshold , Equipment Design , Hearing Loss/physiopathology , Hearing Loss/psychology , Humans , Noise/adverse effects , Perceptual Masking , Predictive Value of Tests , Psychophysics/instrumentation , Reproducibility of Results , Severity of Illness Index , Signal Detection, Psychological , Software
15.
Nat Hum Behav ; 5(10): 1402-1417, 2021 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33958744

ABSTRACT

Reflectance, lighting and geometry combine in complex ways to create images. How do we disentangle these to perceive individual properties, such as surface glossiness? We suggest that brains disentangle properties by learning to model statistical structure in proximal images. To test this hypothesis, we trained unsupervised generative neural networks on renderings of glossy surfaces and compared their representations with human gloss judgements. The networks spontaneously cluster images according to distal properties such as reflectance and illumination, despite receiving no explicit information about these properties. Intriguingly, the resulting representations also predict the specific patterns of 'successes' and 'errors' in human perception. Linearly decoding specular reflectance from the model's internal code predicts human gloss perception better than ground truth, supervised networks or control models, and it predicts, on an image-by-image basis, illusions of gloss perception caused by interactions between material, shape and lighting. Unsupervised learning may underlie many perceptual dimensions in vision and beyond.


Subject(s)
Light , Surface Properties , Visual Perception/physiology , Computer Graphics , Contrast Sensitivity , Form Perception , Humans , Lighting/methods , Materials Science , Photic Stimulation , Psychophysics/instrumentation , Psychophysics/methods , Task Performance and Analysis
16.
Sci Rep ; 11(1): 23553, 2021 12 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34876618

ABSTRACT

Joint torque feedback is a new and promising means of kinesthetic feedback imposed by a wearable device. The torque feedback provides the wearer temporal and spatial information during a motion task. Nevertheless, little research has been conducted on quantifying the psychophysical parameters of how well humans can perceive external torques under various joint conditions. This study aims to investigate the just noticeable difference (JND) perceptual ability of the elbow joint to joint torques. The paper focuses on the ability of two primary joint proprioceptors, the Golgi-tendon organ (GTO) and muscle spindle (MS), to detect elbow torques, since touch and pressure sensors were masked. We studied 14 subjects while the arm was isometrically contracted (static condition) and was moving at a constant speed (dynamic condition). In total there were 10 joint conditions investigated, which varied the direction of the arm's movement and the preload direction as well as torque direction. The JND torques under static conditions ranged from 0.097 Nm with no preload to 0.197 Nm with a preload of 1.28 Nm. The maximum dynamic JND torques were 0.799 Nm and 0.428 Nm, when the arm was flexing and extending at 213 degrees per second, respectively.


Subject(s)
Elbow Joint/physiology , Adult , Biomechanical Phenomena/physiology , Differential Threshold/physiology , Elbow Joint/anatomy & histology , Elbow Joint/innervation , Exoskeleton Device/statistics & numerical data , Feedback, Sensory/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Mechanoreceptors/physiology , Models, Biological , Models, Neurological , Muscle Spindles/innervation , Muscle Spindles/physiology , Psychophysics/instrumentation , Psychophysics/statistics & numerical data , Range of Motion, Articular/physiology , Software , Torque
17.
Optom Vis Sci ; 87(9): E682-96, 2010 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20601914

ABSTRACT

Ocular scattering is an optical phenomenon that degrades the retinal image in the human eye, similar to the effects of ocular aberrations or diffraction. However, there is still a lack of standardization in defining and measuring ocular scattering. Several devices and procedures have been described during the last decade, but most have not been validated or been shown to be reliable. The development of a standard procedure for measuring and describing ocular scattering is an ongoing subject in visual science. This review aims to provide a general definition of the concept of ocular scattering, based on the work of previous authors, and to highlight the advantages and disadvantages of measurement techniques that have been described.


Subject(s)
Diagnostic Techniques, Ophthalmological/standards , Eye Diseases/physiopathology , Eye/physiopathology , Light , Optical Phenomena , Scattering, Radiation , Eye Diseases/diagnosis , Glare , Humans , Models, Theoretical , Psychophysics/instrumentation , Psychophysics/methods , Vision, Ocular
18.
J Vis ; 10(14): 15, 2010 Dec 16.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21163958

ABSTRACT

Modern neurophysiological and psychophysical studies of vision are typically based on computer-generated stimuli presented on flat screens. While this approach allows precise delivery of stimuli, it suffers from a fundamental limitation in terms of the maximum achievable spatial coverage. This constraint becomes important in studies that require stimulation of large expanses of the visual field, such as those involving the mapping of receptive fields throughout the extent of a cortical area or subcortical nucleus, or those comparing neural response properties across a wide range of eccentricities. Here we describe a simple and highly cost-effective method for the projection of computer-generated stimuli on a hemispheric screen, which combines the advantages of computerized control and wide-field (100° × 75°) delivery, without the requirement of highly specialized hardware. The description of the method includes programming techniques for the generation of stimuli in spherical coordinates and for the quantitative determination of receptive field sizes and shapes. The value of this approach is demonstrated by quantitative electrophysiological data obtained in the far peripheral representations of various cortical areas, including automated mapping of receptive field extents in cortex that underwent plasticity following lesions.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping/methods , Models, Neurological , Photic Stimulation/methods , Space Perception/physiology , Visual Cortex/physiology , Animals , Calibration , Callithrix , Equipment Design , Evoked Potentials, Visual/physiology , Female , Male , Neuronal Plasticity/physiology , Photic Stimulation/instrumentation , Psychophysics/instrumentation , Psychophysics/methods , Visual Fields/physiology
19.
J Neurosci Methods ; 323: 48-55, 2019 07 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31103713

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: To study visual processing, it is necessary to precisely control visual stimuli while recording neural and behavioral responses. It can be important to present stimuli over a broad area of the visual field, which can be technically difficult. NEW METHOD: We present a simple geometry that can be used to display panoramic stimuli. A single digital light projector generates images that are reflected by mirrors onto flat screens that surround an animal. It can be used for behavioral and neurophysiological measurements, so virtually identical stimuli can be presented. Moreover, this geometry permits light from the projector to be used to activate optogenetic tools. RESULTS: Using this geometry, we presented panoramic visual stimulation to Drosophila in three paradigms. We presented drifting contrast gratings while recording walking and turning speed. We used the same projector to activate optogenetic channels during visual stimulation. Finally, we used two-photon microscopy to record responses in direction-selective cells to drifting gratings. COMPARISON WITH EXISTING METHOD(S): Existing methods have typically required custom hardware or curved screens, while this method requires only flat back projection screens and a digital light projector. The projector generates images in real time and does not require pre-generated images. Finally, while many setups are large, this geometry occupies a 30 × 20 cm footprint with a 25 cm height. CONCLUSIONS: This flexible geometry enables measurements of behavioral and neural responses to panoramic stimuli. This allows moderate throughput behavioral experiments with simultaneous optogenetic manipulation, with easy comparisons between behavior and neural activity using virtually identical stimuli.


Subject(s)
Behavior, Animal/physiology , Electrophysiology/instrumentation , Optogenetics/instrumentation , Photic Stimulation/instrumentation , Psychophysics/instrumentation , Visual Perception/physiology , Animals , Drosophila , Visual Fields/physiology
20.
Work ; 62(1): 27-35, 2019.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30741711

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Flexed head and neck postures are associated with the development of neck pain in the office environment. There is little evidence regarding whether a wearable posture sensor would improve the head and neck postures of office workers. OBJECTIVE: The aim of this study was to evaluate the effect of the wearable posture sensor on the posture and physical demands on the head and neck during office work. METHODS: Nineteen participants performed a typing task with and without the wearable sensor in the sitting and standing positions. They were allowed to adjust their workstation during the experiment based on a psychophysical method. The flexion angles of the head and neck, the gravitational moment on the neck, and the positions of the workstation components were measured. RESULTS: On average, participants with the wearable sensor had 8% lower neck flexion angles and 14% lower gravitational moments on the neck than those of participants without the wearable sensor. The effect of the wearable sensor on reducing postural stress of the neck was more significant when using the standing workstation compared to the sitting workstation. CONCLUSIONS: The wearable posture sensor could be an effective tool to alleviate the postural stress of the neck in the office setting.


Subject(s)
Ergonomics/instrumentation , Head/physiology , Neck/physiology , Posture/physiology , Workplace/standards , Adult , Ergonomics/standards , Female , Humans , Male , Neck Pain/prevention & control , Office Management/standards , Psychophysics/instrumentation , Psychophysics/standards , Sitting Position , Standing Position , Wearable Electronic Devices/psychology , Workplace/psychology
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL